Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2010

01-09-2010 | Original Article

Switch costs and the operand-recognition paradigm

Auteurs: Arron W. S. Metcalfe, Jamie I. D. Campbell

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Experimental research in cognitive arithmetic frequently relies on participants’ self-reports to discriminate solutions based on direct memory retrieval from use of procedural strategies. Given concerns about the validity and reliability of strategy reports, Thevenot et al. in Mem Cogn 35:1344–1352, (2007) developed the operand-recognition paradigm as an objective measure of arithmetic strategies. Participants performed addition or number comparison on two sequentially presented operands followed by a speeded operand-recognition task. Recognition times increased with problem size following addition but not comparison. Thevenot et al. argued that the complexity of addition strategies increases with problem size. A corresponding increase in operand-recognition time occurs because, as problem size increases, working memory contains more numerical distracters. However, because addition is substantially more difficult than comparison, and difficulty increases with problem size for addition but not comparison, their findings could be due to difficulty-related task-switching costs. We repeated Thevenot et al. (Experiment 1) but added a control condition wherein participants performed a parity (odd or even) task instead of operand recognition. We replicated their findings for operand recognition but found robust, albeit smaller, effects of addition problem size on parity judgements. The results indicate that effects of strategy complexity in the operand-recognition paradigm are confounded with task-switching effects, which complicates its application as a precise measure of strategy complexity in arithmetic.
Voetnoten
1
An earlier experiment in our lab found no switch costs using a letter-comparison control task in which participants decided if a presented letter preceded or followed the letter E. This was a poor task-switch control, however, because the numerical task set (i.e., addition or number comparison) would not compete with letter comparison. In contrast, like operand recognition, parity judgments following addition or comparison required participants to switch number-processing task sets.
 
2
For the problem pair 7 5, Task 2 trials were always paired with a false comparison due to a coding error; this affected 0.01% of Task 2 trials and had no practical consequences for the recognition or parity tasks. Also, because false Task 2 addition answers were ±1 from the correct sum participants could use addition parity rules to solve Task 2 addition problems (Lemaire and Reder 1999). Future research might wish to avoid this possibility, but the current experiment was designed to be a close replication of Thevenot et al. (2007, Experiment 1) and adopted their rules for false answer stimuli.
 
3
We refer to the component stages of addition and comparison as Task 1 and Task 2, but because the Task 2 decision stage necessarily refers to the processes and representations generated during Task 1, it is reasonable to assume that they constitute an integrated task set and that task difficulty is properly indexed by total solution time (i.e., Task 1 + Task 2 RT).
 
4
As the parity task did not afford analysis in terms of first versus second operand we did not include this as a factor in our main analysis.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance (pp. 421–452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance (pp. 421–452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Arbuthnott, K. D. (2008). Asymmetric switch cost and backward inhibition: Carryover activation and inhibition in switching between tasks of unequal difficulty. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 91–100.PubMed Arbuthnott, K. D. (2008). Asymmetric switch cost and backward inhibition: Carryover activation and inhibition in switching between tasks of unequal difficulty. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 91–100.PubMed
go back to reference Ashcraft, M. H. (1995). Cognitive psychology and simple arithmetic: A review and summary of new directions. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 3–34. Ashcraft, M. H. (1995). Cognitive psychology and simple arithmetic: A review and summary of new directions. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 3–34.
go back to reference Campbell, J. I. D., & Metcalfe, A. W. S. (2008). Arabic digit naming speed: Task context and redundancy gain. Cognition, 107, 218–237.CrossRefPubMed Campbell, J. I. D., & Metcalfe, A. W. S. (2008). Arabic digit naming speed: Task context and redundancy gain. Cognition, 107, 218–237.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Campbell, J. I. D., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 299–315.CrossRef Campbell, J. I. D., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 299–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Clapp, F. L. (1924). The number combinations: Their relative difficulty and the frequency of their appearance in textbooks. University of Wisconsin Bureau of Educational Research, Bulletin No. 2. Clapp, F. L. (1924). The number combinations: Their relative difficulty and the frequency of their appearance in textbooks. University of Wisconsin Bureau of Educational Research, Bulletin No. 2.
go back to reference Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (1995). Number processing in pure alexia: The effect of hemispheric asymmetries and task demands. Neurocase, 1, 121–137.CrossRef Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (1995). Number processing in pure alexia: The effect of hemispheric asymmetries and task demands. Neurocase, 1, 121–137.CrossRef
go back to reference Imbo, I., Vandierendonck, A., & Rosseel, Y. (2007). The influence of problem features and individual differences on strategic performance in simple arithmetic. Memory and Cognition., 35, 454–463. Imbo, I., Vandierendonck, A., & Rosseel, Y. (2007). The influence of problem features and individual differences on strategic performance in simple arithmetic. Memory and Cognition., 35, 454–463.
go back to reference Kirk, E. P., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2001). Telling stories: The perils and promise of using verbal reports to study math strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 157–175.CrossRefPubMed Kirk, E. P., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2001). Telling stories: The perils and promise of using verbal reports to study math strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 157–175.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference LeFevre, J.-A., Sadesky, G. S., & Bisanz, J. (1996). Selection of procedures in mental addition: Reassessing the problem size effect in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 216–230.CrossRef LeFevre, J.-A., Sadesky, G. S., & Bisanz, J. (1996). Selection of procedures in mental addition: Reassessing the problem size effect in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 216–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Lemaire, P., & Reder, L. (1999). What affects strategy selection in arithmetic? The example of parity and five effects on product verification. Memory and Cognition, 22, 364–382. Lemaire, P., & Reder, L. (1999). What affects strategy selection in arithmetic? The example of parity and five effects on product verification. Memory and Cognition, 22, 364–382.
go back to reference Monsell, S., Sumner, P., & Waters, H. (2003). Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches. Memory and Cognition, 31, 327–342. Monsell, S., Sumner, P., & Waters, H. (2003). Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches. Memory and Cognition, 31, 327–342.
go back to reference Monsell, S., Yeung, N., & Azuma, R. (2000). Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychological Research, 63, 250–264.CrossRefPubMed Monsell, S., Yeung, N., & Azuma, R. (2000). Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychological Research, 63, 250–264.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thevenot, C., Barrouillet, P., & Fayol, M. (2001). Algorithmic solution of arithmetic problems and operands–answer associations in LTM. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 599–611.CrossRef Thevenot, C., Barrouillet, P., & Fayol, M. (2001). Algorithmic solution of arithmetic problems and operands–answer associations in LTM. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 599–611.CrossRef
go back to reference Thevenot, C., Fanget, M., & Fayol, M. (2007). Retrieval or nonretrieval strategies in mental arithmetic? An operand recognition paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1344–1352. Thevenot, C., Fanget, M., & Fayol, M. (2007). Retrieval or nonretrieval strategies in mental arithmetic? An operand recognition paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1344–1352.
go back to reference Yeung, N., & Monsell, S. (2003). Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: The role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 455–489.CrossRefPubMed Yeung, N., & Monsell, S. (2003). Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: The role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 455–489.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (2005). What everyone finds: The problem size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 331–346). New York: Psychology Press. Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (2005). What everyone finds: The problem size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 331–346). New York: Psychology Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Switch costs and the operand-recognition paradigm
Auteurs
Arron W. S. Metcalfe
Jamie I. D. Campbell
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0272-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2010

Psychological Research 5/2010 Naar de uitgave