Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2021

Open Access 09-01-2020 | Original Article

Self-prioritization during stimulus processing is not obligatory

Auteurs: Siobhan Caughey, Johanna K. Falbén, Dimitra Tsamadi, Linn M. Persson, Marius Golubickis, C. Neil Macrae

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2021

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

An emerging literature has suggested that self-relevance automatically enhances stimulus processing (i.e., the self-prioritization effect). Specifically, during shape–label matching tasks, geometric shapes associated with the self are identified more rapidly than comparable stimuli paired with other targets (e.g., friend, stranger). Replicating and extending work that challenges the putative automaticity of this effect, here we hypothesized that self-relevance facilitates stimulus processing only when task sets draw attention to previously formed shape–label associations in memory. The results of a shape-classification task confirmed this prediction. Compared to shapes associated with a friend, those paired with the self were classified more rapidly when participants were required to report who the stimulus denoted (i.e., self or friend). In contrast, self-relevance failed to facilitate performance when participants judged either what the shape was (i.e., triangle or square, diamond or circle) or where it was located on the screen (i.e., above or below fixation). These findings further elucidate the conditions under which self-relevance does—and does not—influence stimulus processing.
Opmerkingen

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
A commonly reported finding in the psychological literature is that self-relevance facilitates stimulus processing. Compared to material associated with other people (e.g., friend, mother, celebrity), items linked with the self are easier to notice, evaluate, and remember (e.g., Bargh & Pratto, 1986; Keyes & Brady, 2010; Kuiper & Rogers, 1979; Shapiro, Caldwell, & Sorensen, 1997; Symons & Johnson, 1997). Remedying a troublesome stimulus confound that undermined previous work on this topic (i.e., higher familiarity of own face/name vs. other face/name), recent research has demonstrated that the benefits of self-relevance extend even to inconsequential stimuli; notably, geometric shapes (Humphreys & Sui, 2016; Sui & Humphreys, 2017). After pairing shapes with various person-related labels (circle = you, triangle = friend, square = stranger), participants’ perceptual-matching judgments (i.e., do shape–label stimulus pairs match the previously learned associations?) were fastest for stimuli associated with the self (vs. best friend or stranger), a stimulus-prioritization effect that is claimed to reflect an obligatory facet of social-cognitive functioning (Janczyk, Humphreys, & Sui, 2019; Sui, He, & Humphreys, 2012; Sui, Liu, Mevorach, & Humphreys, 2013; Sui, Liu, Wang, & Han, 2009; Sui, Sun, Peng, & Humphreys, 2014). But is this necessarily the case?
There are at least a couple of reasons for questioning the alleged automaticity of self-prioritization during stimulus processing. First, a problematic aspect of the standard shape–label matching paradigm is that, throughout the task, participants must monitor the self-relevance of the stimuli (i.e., self-relevance is made salient through the experimental instructions), otherwise perceptual matching cannot be performed successfully (Sui et al., 2012, 2013, 2014). Indeed, when this explicit task requirement is removed, self-prioritization is eliminated (Dalmaso, Castelli, & Galfano, in press; Siebold, Weaver, Donk, & van Zoest, 2015; Stein, Siebold, & Zoest, 2016; Wade & Vickery, 2018). For example, in a rapid oculomotor search paradigm, Siebold et al. (2015) reported no facilitation in eye movements to lines previously associated with the self (vs. stranger). Similarly, using breaking continuous flash suppression (b-CFS) to examine the ease with which items (i.e., Gabors) access consciousness during a stimulus-localization task, Stein et al. (2016) observed no effect of self-relevance on the time taken for Gabors to overcome interocular suppression. Interestingly, however, contrasting Stein et al. (2016), Macrae, Visokomogilski, Golubickis, Cunningham, and Sahraie (2017) demonstrated enhanced access to visual awareness for self-relevant (vs. other-relevant) shapes when participants performed a stimulus-identification task. What this latter finding suggests is that self-prioritization is contingent upon task instructions (e.g., stimulus localization vs. stimulus identification) that draw attention to the self-relevance of target-object associations in memory.
Second, albeit in a different (though related) experimental context (i.e., object ownership), Falbén et al. (2019) reported that self-relevance only facilitated performance when semantic task sets (e.g., reporting the ownership or identity of pencils/pens) directed attention to previously formed target–object associations. When emphasis switched instead to a perceptual appraisal of the stimuli (i.e., reporting the orientation of pencils/pens), self-prioritization failed to emerge. Likewise, when required to report which of two objects initially appeared on the computer screen (i.e., temporal order judgment task)—a mug owned-by-self or a mug owned-by-the-experimenter—Constable, Welsh, Huffman, and Pratt (2019) revealed that participants were biased toward reporting that self-owned (vs. experimenter-owned) items were presented first. This effect was abolished, however, when the requested judgment was orthogonal to the dimension of interest (i.e., ownership), such that participants were asked to report whether a mug appeared to the left or right of fixation. Collectively these studies suggest that, during stimulus processing, self-prioritization is sensitive to the salience of self-object associations in memory.
Of course, ownership paradigms differ in several important respects from the shape–label matching tasks that have dominated work on this topic (Humphreys & Sui, 2016; Sui & Gu, 2017; Sui & Humphreys, 2015, 2017), but perhaps most significantly in terms of the status of the stimuli on which judgments are rendered (Constable et al., 2019; Falbén et al., 2019; Golubickis, Falbén, Cunningham, & Macrae, 2018; Sui et al., 2012, 2013, 2014; Truong, Roberts, & Todd, 2017). Whereas in perceptual-matching tasks geometric shapes serve as proxies for various social targets (e.g., self is a triangle, friend is a square); in ownership tasks, in contrast, everyday objects are linked with the self and others through association (e.g., self owns pens, friend owns pencils). This difference in the manner in which stimuli acquire self-relevance (i.e., self-surrogate vs. self-associate) may have important implications for the course and products of self-referential processing, hence the automaticity of self-prioritization (Humphreys & Sui, 2016; Sui & Humphreys, 2017). Accordingly, extending previous research (Constable et al., 2019; Falbén et al., 2019), here we considered whether task sets moderate self-prioritization when participants evaluate stimuli (i.e., geometric shapes) that denote the self and a friend (Sui et al., 2012).
Even when judging geometric shapes that designate the self (and others), we suspect that stimulus prioritization only occurs when the cognitive system is configured in particular ways. That is, like other seemingly automatic processes, self-prioritization is dependent on higher-order factors—including intentions, goals, and task sets—that shape stimulus processing and response generation (Kiefer, 2007; Memelink & Hommel, 2013; Moors & De Houwer, 2006; Naccache, Blandin, & Dehaene, 2002). In particular, whereas self-relevance should facilitate performance when task sets yield access to shape–target associations in memory (Janczyk et al., 2019; Sui et al., 2012, 2013, 2014); absent these specific processing conditions, self-prioritization is unlikely to emerge (Constable et al., 2019; Dalmaso et al., in press; Falbén et al., 2019; Siebold et al., 2015; Stein et al., 2016). Functioning in this way, self-prioritization is consistent with Hommel’s (1998, 2004, 2018) Theory of Event Coding (TEC). According to this account, processing episodes generate event files whereby elements of the experience are associated and stored in memory (e.g., self-is-a-square). Crucially, re-experiencing any of these stimuli (e.g., a square) at a later date can trigger partial or complete retrieval of the event file (e.g., the self-square association), thereby impacting task performance to varying degrees. In this way, the TEC provides a useful framework for understanding the generation of self-prioritization effects during stimulus processing (Hommel, 2004, 2018).
In a shape-classification task, here we considered how task sets impact self-prioritization (Constable et al., 2019; Falbén et al., 2019). Participants first associated geometric shapes (e.g., square, triangle) with either the self or a best friend (Sui et al., 2012). Next, to explore the automaticity of self-prioritization, they completed three blocks of trials in a computer-based shape-classification task, with each block requiring a different judgment to be undertaken on the stimuli. Specifically, participants reported: (i) to whom the shapes referred (‘who’ task); (ii) the identity of the shapes (‘what’ task); and (iii) whether the shapes appeared above or below the fixation cross on the screen (‘where’ task). In so doing, by probing different aspects of the stimuli (i.e., social status vs. physical status vs. perceptual status), the current experiment comprised a conceptual replication and extension of Falbén et al. (2019), in that the targets of judgment on this occasion were geometric shapes that served as proxies for the self and best friend (cf. self-owned vs. friend-owned objects). We expected self-prioritization to be moderated by the extent to which task sets access target–shape associations in memory during stimulus processing (Hommel, 2004; Memelink & Hommel, 2013).

Method

Participants and design

Eighty undergraduates (14 males, Mage = 20.67, SD = 3.06) took part in the research.1 All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Five participants (1 male) were excluded from the statistical analysis for failing to follow the experimental instructions. Informed consent was obtained from participants prior to the commencement of the experiment and the protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee at the School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen. The experiment had a 3 (Judgment: who vs. what vs. where) × 2 (Target: self vs. friend) × 2 (Shapes: triangle & square vs. diamond & circle) mixed design with repeated measures on the first two factors.

Stimulus materials and procedure

Participants arrived at the laboratory individually, were greeted by an experimenter, seated in front of a desktop computer, and told they would be performing a shape-classification task. Following Sui et al. (2012), the experiment had two phases. The first phase comprised a learning task in which participants were required to associate geometric shapes with the self and a best friend. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the shape conditions. For half of the participants, the shapes were a triangle and a square; for the others they were a diamond and a circle. Two categories of shapes were used to provide a between-participants replication of the experiment. The shapes were not presented at this stage and shape–target associations and shape–target order were counterbalanced across the sample during the learning task.
Next, participants were informed they would be performing a shape-classification task. The task comprised three blocks of trials, with a different stimulus-related judgment required in each block. In one block (i.e., ‘who’ judgment), participants reported whether the shape presented on the screen represented them or their friend. In another block (i.e., ‘what ’judgment), whether the shape was a triangle or a square (diamond or circle). In a final block (i.e., ‘where’ judgment), whether the shape was displayed above or below the fixation cross on the screen. In all blocks, half of the items appeared above and half below the fixation cross and the order of the judgment tasks was counterbalanced across the sample. Before the start of each block, the experimenter explained that participants would be presented with a series of shapes and their task was simply to categorize each shape (via a button press), as quickly and accurately as possible, according to the specific judgment task. Responses were given using two buttons on the keyboard (i.e., N & M). Key-response mappings were counterbalanced across participants and across judgment tasks.
Each trial began with the presentation of a central fixation cross for 500 ms, followed by the image of a shape for 100 ms. After each shape was presented, the screen turned blank and participants had 1100 ms in which to respond (Sui et al., 2012). Following each response, the fixation cross reappeared and the next trial commenced. The images of the shapes were 214 × 214 pixels in size. Participants initially performed 8 practice trials for each type of judgment, followed by a block of 196 experimental trials in which stimuli occurred equally often in a random order. In total, there were 588 trials, with 196 trials in each block (i.e., judgment task), and 98 trials in each condition (i.e., 98 self-trials & 98-friend-trials). On completion of the task, participants were debriefed, thanked, and dismissed.

Results and discussion

Responses faster than 200 ms were excluded from the analysis, eliminating less than 1% of the overall number of trials (see Table 1 for treatment means). A multilevel model was used to examine the response time (RT) and accuracy data. Analyses were conducted with the R package ‘lmer4’ (Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, Sarkar, & R Core Team, 2015), with participants as a crossed random effect (Judd, Westfall, & Kenny, 2012). Analysis of the RTs yielded main effects of Judgment (b = 0.376, SE = 0.621, t  = 60.49, p < 0.001) and Target (b = − 1.519, SE = 0.504, t = − 3.01, p = 0.003), and a significant Judgment × Target interaction (b = − 1.635, SE = 0.619, t = − 2.64, p = 0.008). Further analysis of the interaction revealed that, for ‘who’ judgments, RTs were faster for shapes that denoted the self than shapes that denoted a friend (b = − 3.465, SE = 0.906, t = − 3.83, p < 0.001). No such self-prioritization effect emerged on either ‘what’ (b = − 0.515, SE = 0.866, t = − 0.59, p = 0.552) or ‘where’ (b = − 0.311, SE = 0.717, t = − 0.43, p = 0.665) judgments.2
Table 1
Mean reaction time (RT) and accuracy (%) as a function of Judgment and Target
Judgment
Target
Mean RT (ms)
Accuracy (%)
Who
Self
408 (121)
89 (31)
 
Friend
413 (121)
90 (30)
What
Self
407 (120)
92 (28)
 
Friend
409 (120)
93 (26)
Where
Self
341(97)
97 (18)
 
Friend
341 (95)
96 (19)
Standard deviations appear within parentheses
A multilevel logistic regression analysis on the accuracy of responses yielded main effects of Judgment (b = − 0.573, SE = 0.025, z = − 22.90, p < 0.001) and Target (b = − 0.41, SE = 0.020, z = − 2.02, p = 0.044). The Judgment X Target interaction, however, was not significant (b = − 0.013, SE = 0.025, z = 0.53, p = 0.594).
These results confirm that, across different pairs of shapes, self-prioritization is dependent on the task set that is operating during stimulus processing (Constable et al., 2019; Dalmaso et al., in press; Falbén et al., 2019; Siebold et al., 2015; Stein et al., 2016). Whereas self-prioritization was observed when judgments pertained to the target (i.e., ‘who’ judgment) with whom geometric shapes had previously been associated, no such effect emerged when they probed either the identity (i.e., ‘what’ judgment) or location (i.e., ‘where’ judgment) of the shapes. Moreover, the comparable RTs in the ‘who’ and ‘what’ (vs. ‘where’) tasks reveal that self-prioritization was not a function of task difficulty—stimulus prioritization only occurred when self-relevance was an explicit component of the task set (Siebold et al., 2015; Stein et al., 2016).

Discussion

An extensive literature has demonstrated the benefits of self-relevance on stimulus processing (Humphreys & Sui, 2016; Sui & Humphreys, 2015). Using a perceptual-matching task, researchers have shown that participants make faster and less errant responses to stimuli related to the self than other persons. Critically, this effect has been replicated on numerous occasions, with self-prioritization extending to a wide range of stimuli, including: geometric shapes (Desebrock, Sui, & Spence, 2018; Enock, Sui, Hewstone, & Humphreys, 2018; Golubickis et al., 2017; Schäfer, Frings, & Wentura, 2016; Schäfer, Wentura, & Frings, 2017; Schäfer, Wesslein, Spence, Wenura, & Frings, 2016; Sui et al., 2012, 2013, 2014), badges of sports teams (Moradi, Sui, Hewstone, & Humphreys, 2015), objects (Schäfer, Wentura, & Frings, 2015), computer-generated avatars (Mattan, Quinn, Apperly, Sui, & Rothshtein, 2015), Gabor patches (Macrae, Visokomogilski, Golubickis, & Sahraie, 2018; Stein et al., 2016), lines (Siebold et al., 2015), and faces (Payne, Tsakiris, & Maister, 2017). In addition, self-prioritization has been reported across various sensory modalities (Frings & Wentura, 2014; Schäfer et al., 2015, 2016b).
Notwithstanding multiple demonstrations of self-prioritization during stimulus processing, lingering questions remain regarding the status of this effect. Based largely on studies using perceptual-matching tasks, conventional wisdom asserts that the mind is intricately tuned to personally meaningful material, such that self-relevance automatically enhances stimulus processing via bottom-up attentional capture (Humphreys & Sui, 2016; Sui & Humphreys, 2015; Sui & Rotshtein, 2019). Beyond shape–label matching tasks, however, the advantages of self-relevance appear to be far from obligatory. Across a range of paradigms and measures (e.g., oculomotor search, b-CFS, temporal order judgments, object categorization), self-relevance has been shown to exert little influence on task performance (e.g., Constable et al., 2019; Dalmaso et al., in press; Falbén et al., 2019; Siebold et al., 2015; Stein et al., 2016; Wade & Vickery, 2018), prompting Siebold et al. (2015, p. 1014) to conclude that “…the occurrence of the self-prioritization effect is specific to the requirements of the matching paradigm.” Echoing these sentiments, here we demonstrated the conditional automaticity of self-prioritization during a shape-classification task.
Replicating prior research, self-prioritization was contingent upon the task set (i.e., ‘who’ vs. ‘what’ vs. ‘where’) that was operating during stimulus evaluation (Constable et al., 2019; Dalmaso et al., in press; Falbén et al., 2019).3 Importantly, however, the current results diverged from Falbén et al. (2019) in an interesting way. Using an object-ownership task, Falbén et al. (2019) revealed that self-relevance facilitated performance when task sets probed either the ownership or physical identity of the stimuli (i.e., ‘who’ and ‘what’ tasks). When emphasis switched instead to a perceptual analysis of the objects (i.e., ‘where’ task), self-relevance had no influence on performance, thereby implying that self-prioritization requires the operation of semantic task sets. Here, in contrast, self-prioritization failed to materialize under a comparable semantic processing objective (i.e., ‘what’ is the shape?), emerging instead only when self-relevance was an overt aspect of the requested judgment (i.e., ‘who’ is the shape?). This difference highlights the task-related dependency of self-prioritization. Given the pivotal (and natural) role that proprietorship plays in everyday life (Beggan, 1992; Belk, 1988; 2014; James, 1890), ownership likely forges potent self-object connections in memory that can be accessed via multiple semantic pathways (Hommel, 2004). In contrast, the methodologically expedient—though somewhat artificial—activity of associating shapes with social targets is unlikely to create associations that can be reactivated in this way (Siebold et al., 2015; Stein et al., 2016), hence task sets must emphasize self-relevance to elicit stimulus prioritization. Thus, depending on the task context and the judgmental requirements, whether stimuli are self-surrogates or self-associates is an important determinant of prioritized processing.
Together with related research, the current findings underscore the malleability of self-prioritization (Constable et al., 2019; Dalmaso et al., in press; Falbén et al., 2019; Siebold et al., 2015; Stein et al., 2016; Wade & Vickery, 2018). Far from depicting a mandatory effect, the benefits of self-relevance appear sensitive to how stimuli are encountered, encoded, and evaluated (Bargh, 1989; Hommel, 2004, 2018; Moors & De Houwer, 2006). Of particular importance is the manner in which self-relevance is established. This, of course, would be expected if one assumes that self-referential processing guides behavior in a flexible way (e.g., Baars, 1988; Baumeister, 1998; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Heatherton, 2011; Neisser, 1988). If every potentially self-relevant stimulus was prioritized during processing, social-cognitive functioning would rapidly grind to halt. For example, absent the objective of driving into town, one’s car has no immediate applicability (i.e., goal-relevance). It would therefore be somewhat cumbrous if the mind was to prioritize this currently insignificant stimulus based only on its possible self-relevance. Under attentional control, self-relevance can guide cognition in a strategic, adaptive manner (Hommel, 2004, 2018).
The contextual flexibility that daily functioning demands may be provided by an intentional weighting mechanism that, prior to task performance (e.g., ‘who’ vs. ‘where’ tasks), increases the output gain on task-relevant features of the event file (i.e., target-object associations) in memory. In other words, depending on the task at hand, the same event file may be cognitively coded (i.e., weighted) in quite different ways, thereby producing quite different effects (see Memelink & Hommel, 2013). What is needed, therefore, are studies that tap the prioritization (or otherwise) of stimuli in task contexts that capture life outside the laboratory. Specifically, research must go beyond the explicit appraisal of inconsequential (though self-relevant) shapes to consider exactly when, how, and for whom different types of material are prioritized during stimulus processing (Golubickis et al., in press). In so doing, work of this kind will explicate the conditions under which self-relevance does—and indeed does not—facilitate thinking and doing.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Voetnoten
1
Sample size was based on Falbén et al. (2019).
 
2
To explore the possibility that task order influenced self-prioritization, we conducted an additional analysis of the RTs in which this repeated-measures factor was added to the multilevel model. The only additional effects to emerge in this analysis were a main effect of Order (b = − 6.420, SE = 0.636, t = − 10.10, p < 0.001) and a significant Order × Judgment interaction (b = − 6.889, SE = 1.083, t = − 6.36, p < 0.001). Further inspection of the interaction revealed an effect of Order on ‘who’ judgments (b = − 20.779, SE = 8.335, t = − 2.49, p = 0.015), such that responses (to both self and friend) were fastest (M = 393, SD = 115) when this task was performed third (vs. second  [M = 419, SD = 122] or first [M = 430, SD = 120]). Thus, task order did not impact self-prioritization.
 
3
The absence of a self-prioritization effect when judging the location of the stimuli could due to a floor effect in RTs (but see Falbén et al., 2019). Of greater theoretical interest, however, was the failure of the two semantic task sets to elicit comparable effects, despite RTs that were equivalent.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press. Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford Press. Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Bargh, J. A., & Pratto, F. (1986). Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 293–311. Bargh, J. A., & Pratto, F. (1986). Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 293–311.
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680–740). New York: McGraw-Hill. Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680–740). New York: McGraw-Hill.
go back to reference Beggan, J. K. (1992). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 229–237. Beggan, J. K. (1992). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 229–237.
go back to reference Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 139–168. Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 139–168.
go back to reference Belk, R. W. (2014). The extended self unbound. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 22, 133–134. Belk, R. W. (2014). The extended self unbound. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 22, 133–134.
go back to reference Constable, M. D., Welsh, T. N., Huffman, G., & Pratt, J. (2019). I before U: Temporal order judgements reveal bias for self-owned objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 589–598. Constable, M. D., Welsh, T. N., Huffman, G., & Pratt, J. (2019). I before U: Temporal order judgements reveal bias for self-owned objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 589–598.
go back to reference Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.PubMed Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.PubMed
go back to reference Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L., & Galfano, G. (in press). Self-related shapes can hold the eyes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L., & Galfano, G. (in press). Self-related shapes can hold the eyes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.
go back to reference Desebrock, C., Sui, J., & Spence, C. (2018). Self-reference in action: Arm-movement responses are enhanced in perceptual matching. Acta Psychologica, 190, 258–266.PubMed Desebrock, C., Sui, J., & Spence, C. (2018). Self-reference in action: Arm-movement responses are enhanced in perceptual matching. Acta Psychologica, 190, 258–266.PubMed
go back to reference Enock, F., Sui, J., Hewstone, M., & Humphreys, G. W. (2018). Self and team prioritization effects in perceptual matching: Evidence for a shared representation. Acta Psychologica, 182, 107–118.PubMed Enock, F., Sui, J., Hewstone, M., & Humphreys, G. W. (2018). Self and team prioritization effects in perceptual matching: Evidence for a shared representation. Acta Psychologica, 182, 107–118.PubMed
go back to reference Falbén, J. K., Golubickis, M., Balseryte, R., Persson, L. M., Tsamadi, D., Caughey, S., & Macrae, C. N. (2019). How prioritized is self-prioritization during stimulus processing? Visual Cognition, 27, 46–51. Falbén, J. K., Golubickis, M., Balseryte, R., Persson, L. M., Tsamadi, D., Caughey, S., & Macrae, C. N. (2019). How prioritized is self-prioritization during stimulus processing? Visual Cognition, 27, 46–51.
go back to reference Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2014). Self-prioritization processes in action and perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1737–1740.PubMed Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2014). Self-prioritization processes in action and perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1737–1740.PubMed
go back to reference Golubickis, M., Falbén, J. K., Cunningham, W. A., & Macrae, C. N. (2018). Exploring the self-ownership effect: Separating stimulus and response biases. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 295–306.PubMed Golubickis, M., Falbén, J. K., Cunningham, W. A., & Macrae, C. N. (2018). Exploring the self-ownership effect: Separating stimulus and response biases. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 295–306.PubMed
go back to reference Golubickis, M., Falbén, J. K., Sahraie, A., Visokomogilski, A., Cunningham, W. A., Sui, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2017). Self-prioritization and perceptual matching: The effects of temporal construal. Memory & Cognition, 45, 1223–1239. Golubickis, M., Falbén, J. K., Sahraie, A., Visokomogilski, A., Cunningham, W. A., Sui, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2017). Self-prioritization and perceptual matching: The effects of temporal construal. Memory & Cognition, 45, 1223–1239.
go back to reference Golubickis, M., Ho, N. S. P., Falbén, J. K., Schwertel, C. L., Maiuri, A., Dublas, D., Cunningham, W. A., & Macrae, C. N. (in press). Valence and ownership: Object desirability influences self-prioritization. Psychological Research. Golubickis, M., Ho, N. S. P., Falbén, J. K., Schwertel, C. L., Maiuri, A., Dublas, D., Cunningham, W. A., & Macrae, C. N. (in press). Valence and ownership: Object desirability influences self-prioritization. Psychological Research.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216. Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.PubMed Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2018). Representing oneself and others: An event-coding approach. Experimental Psychology, 65, 323–331.PubMed Hommel, B. (2018). Representing oneself and others: An event-coding approach. Experimental Psychology, 65, 323–331.PubMed
go back to reference Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2016). Attentional control and the self: The self-attention network (SAN). Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 5–17.PubMed Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2016). Attentional control and the self: The self-attention network (SAN). Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 5–17.PubMed
go back to reference James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry-Holt & Co. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry-Holt & Co.
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2019). The central locus of self-prioritisation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1068–1083. Janczyk, M., Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2019). The central locus of self-prioritisation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1068–1083.
go back to reference Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 54–69.PubMed Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 54–69.PubMed
go back to reference Keyes, H., & Brady, N. (2010). Self-face recognition is characterized by “bilateral gain” and by faster, more accurate performance which persists when faces are inverted. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 840–847.PubMed Keyes, H., & Brady, N. (2010). Self-face recognition is characterized by “bilateral gain” and by faster, more accurate performance which persists when faces are inverted. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 840–847.PubMed
go back to reference Kiefer, M. (2007). Top-down modulation of “automatic” processes: A gating framework. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 289–306. Kiefer, M. (2007). Top-down modulation of “automatic” processes: A gating framework. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 289–306.
go back to reference Kuiper, N. A., & Rogers, T. B. (1979). Encoding of personal information: Self-other differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 499–514. Kuiper, N. A., & Rogers, T. B. (1979). Encoding of personal information: Self-other differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 499–514.
go back to reference Macrae, C. N., Visokomogilski, A., Golubickis, M., Cunningham, W. A., & Sahraie, A. (2017). Self-relevance prioritizes access to visual awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43, 438–443.PubMed Macrae, C. N., Visokomogilski, A., Golubickis, M., Cunningham, W. A., & Sahraie, A. (2017). Self-relevance prioritizes access to visual awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43, 438–443.PubMed
go back to reference Macrae, C. N., Visokomogilski, A., Golubickis, M., & Sahraie, A. (2018). Self-relevance enhances the benefits of attention on perception. Visual Cognition, 26, 475–481. Macrae, C. N., Visokomogilski, A., Golubickis, M., & Sahraie, A. (2018). Self-relevance enhances the benefits of attention on perception. Visual Cognition, 26, 475–481.
go back to reference Mattan, B., Quinn, K. A., Apperly, I. A., Sui, J., & Rothstein, P. (2015). Is it always me first? Effects of self-tagging on third-person perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1100–1117.PubMed Mattan, B., Quinn, K. A., Apperly, I. A., Sui, J., & Rothstein, P. (2015). Is it always me first? Effects of self-tagging on third-person perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1100–1117.PubMed
go back to reference Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77, 249–259.PubMed Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77, 249–259.PubMed
go back to reference Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A conceptual and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326. Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A conceptual and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326.
go back to reference Moradi, Z., Sui, J., Hewstone, M., & Humphreys, G. W. (2015). In-group modulation of perceptual matching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1255–1277. Moradi, Z., Sui, J., Hewstone, M., & Humphreys, G. W. (2015). In-group modulation of perceptual matching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1255–1277.
go back to reference Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424.PubMed Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424.PubMed
go back to reference Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1, 5–59. Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1, 5–59.
go back to reference Payne, S., Tsakiris, M., & Maister, L. (2017). Can the self become another? Investigating the effects of self-association with a new facial identity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 1085–1097. Payne, S., Tsakiris, M., & Maister, L. (2017). Can the self become another? Investigating the effects of self-association with a new facial identity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 1085–1097.
go back to reference Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Development Core Team. (2015). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. The Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN), Vienna, Austria. Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Development Core Team. (2015). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. The Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN), Vienna, Austria.
go back to reference Schäfer, S., Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2016a). About the composition of self-relevance: Conjunctions not features are bound to the self. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 887–982. Schäfer, S., Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2016a). About the composition of self-relevance: Conjunctions not features are bound to the self. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 887–982.
go back to reference Schäfer, S., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2015). Self-prioritization beyond perception. Experimental Psychology, 62, 415–425.PubMed Schäfer, S., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2015). Self-prioritization beyond perception. Experimental Psychology, 62, 415–425.PubMed
go back to reference Schäfer, S., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2017). Distinctiveness effects in self-prioritization. Visual Cognition, 25, 399–411. Schäfer, S., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2017). Distinctiveness effects in self-prioritization. Visual Cognition, 25, 399–411.
go back to reference Schäfer, S., Wesslein, A. K., Spence, C., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2016b). Self-prioritization in vision, audition, and touch. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 2141–2150.PubMed Schäfer, S., Wesslein, A. K., Spence, C., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2016b). Self-prioritization in vision, audition, and touch. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 2141–2150.PubMed
go back to reference Shapiro, K. L., Caldwell, J., & Sorensen, R. E. (1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: A visual “cocktail party” effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 504–514.PubMed Shapiro, K. L., Caldwell, J., & Sorensen, R. E. (1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: A visual “cocktail party” effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 504–514.PubMed
go back to reference Siebold, A., Weaver, M. D., Donk, M., & van Zoest, W. (2015). Social salience does not transfer to oculomotor visual search. Visual Cognition, 23, 989–1019. Siebold, A., Weaver, M. D., Donk, M., & van Zoest, W. (2015). Social salience does not transfer to oculomotor visual search. Visual Cognition, 23, 989–1019.
go back to reference Stein, T., Siebold, A., & Zoest, M. V. (2016). Testing the idea of privileged awareness of self-relevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1–16. Stein, T., Siebold, A., & Zoest, M. V. (2016). Testing the idea of privileged awareness of self-relevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1–16.
go back to reference Sui, J., & Gu, X. (2017). Self as object: Emerging trends in self research. Trends in Neurosciences, 40, 643–653.PubMed Sui, J., & Gu, X. (2017). Self as object: Emerging trends in self research. Trends in Neurosciences, 40, 643–653.PubMed
go back to reference Sui, J., He, X., & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). Perceptual effects of social salience: Evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1105–1117.PubMed Sui, J., He, X., & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). Perceptual effects of social salience: Evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1105–1117.PubMed
go back to reference Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2015). The integrative self: How self-reference integrates perception and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 719–728.PubMed Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2015). The integrative self: How self-reference integrates perception and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 719–728.PubMed
go back to reference Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2017). The ubiquitous self: What the properties of self-bias tell us about the self. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396, 222–235.PubMed Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2017). The ubiquitous self: What the properties of self-bias tell us about the self. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396, 222–235.PubMed
go back to reference Sui, J., Liu, M., Mevorach, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). The salient self: The left intra-parietal sulcus responds to social as well as perceptual salience after self-association. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 1060–1068. Sui, J., Liu, M., Mevorach, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). The salient self: The left intra-parietal sulcus responds to social as well as perceptual salience after self-association. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 1060–1068.
go back to reference Sui, J., Liu, C. H., Wang, L., & Han, S. (2009). Attentional orientation induced by temporarily established self-referential cues. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology., 62, 844–849. Sui, J., Liu, C. H., Wang, L., & Han, S. (2009). Attentional orientation induced by temporarily established self-referential cues. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology., 62, 844–849.
go back to reference Sui, J., & Rotshtein, P. (2019). Self-prioritization and the attentional systems. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 148–152.PubMed Sui, J., & Rotshtein, P. (2019). Self-prioritization and the attentional systems. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 148–152.PubMed
go back to reference Sui, J., Sun, Y., Peng, K., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). The automatic and the expected self: Separating self- and familiarity biases effects by manipulating stimulus probability. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1176–1184. Sui, J., Sun, Y., Peng, K., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). The automatic and the expected self: Separating self- and familiarity biases effects by manipulating stimulus probability. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1176–1184.
go back to reference Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–394.PubMed Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–394.PubMed
go back to reference Truong, G., Roberts, K. H., & Todd, R. M. (2017). I saw mine first: A prior-entry effect for newly acquired ownership. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43, 192–205.PubMed Truong, G., Roberts, K. H., & Todd, R. M. (2017). I saw mine first: A prior-entry effect for newly acquired ownership. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43, 192–205.PubMed
go back to reference Wade, G. L., & Vickery, T. J. (2018). Target self-relevance speeds visual search responses but does not improve search efficiency. Visual Cognition, 26, 563–582. Wade, G. L., & Vickery, T. J. (2018). Target self-relevance speeds visual search responses but does not improve search efficiency. Visual Cognition, 26, 563–582.
Metagegevens
Titel
Self-prioritization during stimulus processing is not obligatory
Auteurs
Siobhan Caughey
Johanna K. Falbén
Dimitra Tsamadi
Linn M. Persson
Marius Golubickis
C. Neil Macrae
Publicatiedatum
09-01-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01283-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2021

Psychological Research 2/2021 Naar de uitgave