Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2009

01-03-2009 | Original Article

Dimensional weighting of primary and secondary target-defining dimensions in visual search for singleton conjunction targets

Auteurs: Ralph Weidner, Hermann J. Müller

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2009

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Two experiments investigated dimension-based attentional processing in a complex singleton conjunction search task. In Experiment 1, observers had to discern the presence of a singleton target defined by a conjunction of size (fixed primary dimension) with either color or motion direction (secondary dimension). Similar to findings in singleton feature search, changes (vs. repetitions) of the secondary dimension across trials resulted in reaction time (RT) costs—which were, however, increased by a factor of 3–5 compared to singleton feature search. In Experiment 2, the coding of search-critical, dimensional saliency signals was investigated by additionally presenting targets redundantly defined in both secondary dimensions, with redundant-target signals being either spatially coincident or separate (i.e., one vs. two target items). Redundant-target RTs significantly violated Miller’s (Cognit Psychol 14:247–279, 1982) race model inequality only when redundant signals were spatially coincident (i.e., bound to a single object), indicating coactive processing of target information in the two secondary dimensions. These findings suggest that the coding and combining of signals from different visual dimensions operates in parallel. Increased change costs in singleton conjunction search are likely to reflect a reduced amount of weight available for processing the secondary target-defining dimensions, due to a large amount of weight being bound by the primary dimension.
Voetnoten
1
It is less immediately clear how FIT could be appropriately extended to accommodate dimension-specific intertrial effects. On possibility is to assume (feature) units that pool activity across target feature maps and thus represent the mere presence of a given target feature in the display, without pointing to its location (cf. Treisman & Gormican, 1988). Such units could then be checked serially in cross-dimensional feature search to ascertain whether a possible target feature is present in a given dimension. If the result is positive, a detection response can be given. If not, then checking switches to other possible features in other dimensions. Indeed, this is a proposal made by Treisman (1988). To explain the dimension-specificity (rather than feature-specificity) of the intertrial effects, this proposal would have to assume further that (1) the pooling units integrate feature activity across all possible target feature maps within a dimension, thereby providing some sort of dimension-specific target presence signal, and (2) that that dimensional pooling unit is checked first on a given trial that indicated the presence of a target on the last trial. However, even with this extension, FIT would still fail to explain why cross-dimensional integration of dimension-specific target signals is location-specific in nature (because the pooling units signal no information about target location).
 
2
This assumption is theoretically new: neither GS nor FIT state anything about filtering being necessarily resource demanding or binding weight.
 
3
According to the SOS model (Grossberg et al., 1994), a display is initially grouped on the basis of one dimension (e.g., size) and then, within the selected subset, on the basis of the second dimension (e.g., motion direction or color). Given that the settings associated with these operations persist across trials and that the selection processes require attentional weight shifts, hierarchical processing is able to account for increased costs on change trials. Attentional weight would have to be consecutively shifted to the primary target defining dimension to perform the initial selection operation and subsequently to one of the secondary target defining dimensions in order to check the presence of a higher saliency signal within this subset. The time consuming initial selection operation could account for the overall increase of detection RTs in conjunction relative to feature search. Higher dimensional switch costs would arise when a specific sequence of initial selection operations and saliency detection does not result in target detection. In this case, the sequence of operations (including the time consuming initial selection operation) would have to be carried out anew by selecting different subsets. However, this—essentially serial—model is inconsistent with the results of the present Experiment 2, which revealed both secondary target dimensions to influence search in parallel.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, D. A. (1971). Parallel encoding within and between elementary stimulus dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 104–108. Allport, D. A. (1971). Parallel encoding within and between elementary stimulus dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 104–108.
go back to reference Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: Evidence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 451–470.PubMedCrossRef Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: Evidence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 451–470.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cave, K. R., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 22(2), 225–271.PubMedCrossRef Cave, K. R., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 22(2), 225–271.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chun, M. M., & Wolfe, J. M. (1996). Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present? Cognitive Psychology, 30(1), 39–78.PubMedCrossRef Chun, M. M., & Wolfe, J. M. (1996). Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present? Cognitive Psychology, 30(1), 39–78.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 501–517.CrossRef Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 501–517.CrossRef
go back to reference Egeth, H. E., Virzi, R. A., & Garbart, H. (1984). Searching for conjunctively defined targets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 10, 32–39.CrossRef Egeth, H. E., Virzi, R. A., & Garbart, H. (1984). Searching for conjunctively defined targets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 10, 32–39.CrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, C. W. (1988). A source of error in attempts to distinguish coactivation from separate activation in the perception of redundant targets. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 191–193. Eriksen, C. W. (1988). A source of error in attempts to distinguish coactivation from separate activation in the perception of redundant targets. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 191–193.
go back to reference Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1973). The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding fromvisual displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 155–160. Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1973). The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding fromvisual displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 155–160.
go back to reference Found, A., & Müller, H. J. (1996). Searching for unknown feature targets on more than one dimension: Investigating a “dimension-weighting” account. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 88–101. Found, A., & Müller, H. J. (1996). Searching for unknown feature targets on more than one dimension: Investigating a “dimension-weighting” account. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 88–101.
go back to reference Grossberg, S., Mingolla, E., & Ross, W. D. (1994). A neural theory of attentive visual search: Interactions of boundary, surface, spatial, and object representations. Psycholological Review, 101, 470–489.CrossRef Grossberg, S., Mingolla, E., & Ross, W. D. (1994). A neural theory of attentive visual search: Interactions of boundary, surface, spatial, and object representations. Psycholological Review, 101, 470–489.CrossRef
go back to reference Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40(10–12), 1489–1506.PubMedCrossRef Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40(10–12), 1489–1506.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of Attention (pp. 29–61). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of Attention (pp. 29–61). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
go back to reference Kaptein, N. A., Theeuwes, J., & van der Heijden, A. (1995). Search for a conjunctively defined target can be selectively limited to a color-defined subset of elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(5), 1053–1069. Kaptein, N. A., Theeuwes, J., & van der Heijden, A. (1995). Search for a conjunctively defined target can be selectively limited to a color-defined subset of elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(5), 1053–1069.
go back to reference Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry. Human Neurobiology, 4(4), 219–227.PubMed Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry. Human Neurobiology, 4(4), 219–227.PubMed
go back to reference Krummenacher, J., Müller, H. J., & Heller, D. (2001). Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: Evidence for parallel-coactive processing of dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 901–917. Krummenacher, J., Müller, H. J., & Heller, D. (2001). Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: Evidence for parallel-coactive processing of dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 901–917.
go back to reference Krummenacher, J., Müller, H. J., & Heller, D. (2002). Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: Parallel-coactive processing of dimensions is location specific. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 1303–1322.CrossRef Krummenacher, J., Müller, H. J., & Heller, D. (2002). Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: Parallel-coactive processing of dimensions is location specific. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 1303–1322.CrossRef
go back to reference Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 75–82.CrossRef Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 75–82.CrossRef
go back to reference McLeod, P., Driver, J., & Crisp, J. (1988). Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel. Nature, 332, 154–155.PubMedCrossRef McLeod, P., Driver, J., & Crisp, J. (1988). Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel. Nature, 332, 154–155.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J. (1982). Divided attention: Evidence for co-activation with redundant signals. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 247–279.PubMedCrossRef Miller, J. (1982). Divided attention: Evidence for co-activation with redundant signals. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 247–279.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(2), 315–330.PubMed Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(2), 315–330.PubMed
go back to reference Müller, H. J., Heller, D., & Ziegler, J. (1995). Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1–17. Müller, H. J., Heller, D., & Ziegler, J. (1995). Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1–17.
go back to reference Müller, H. J., Reimann, B., & Krummenacher, J. (2003). Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus-and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 1021–1035.PubMedCrossRef Müller, H. J., Reimann, B., & Krummenacher, J. (2003). Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus-and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 1021–1035.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nakayama, K., & Silverman, G. H. (1986). Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions. Nature, 320, 264–265.PubMedCrossRef Nakayama, K., & Silverman, G. H. (1986). Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions. Nature, 320, 264–265.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pollmann, S., Weidner, R., Müller, H. J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). A fronto-posterior network involved in visual dimension changes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 480–494.PubMedCrossRef Pollmann, S., Weidner, R., Müller, H. J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). A fronto-posterior network involved in visual dimension changes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 480–494.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMedCrossRef Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ramnani, N., & Owen, A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(3), 184–194.PubMedCrossRef Ramnani, N., & Owen, A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(3), 184–194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects: The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 40(2), 201–237. Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects: The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 40(2), 201–237.
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95(1), 15–48.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95(1), 15–48.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A., & Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 459–478.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A., & Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 459–478.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wei, P., Lü, J., Müller, H. J., & Zhou, X. (2008). Searching for two feature singletons in the visual scene: The localized attentional interference effect. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 175–188.CrossRef Wei, P., Lü, J., Müller, H. J., & Zhou, X. (2008). Searching for two feature singletons in the visual scene: The localized attentional interference effect. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 175–188.CrossRef
go back to reference Weidner, R., Pollmann, S., Müller, H. J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). Top-down controlled visual dimension weighting: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 318–328.PubMedCrossRef Weidner, R., Pollmann, S., Müller, H. J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). Top-down controlled visual dimension weighting: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 318–328.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolfe, J. (1998). Visual search. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 13–74). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Wolfe, J. (1998). Visual search. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 13–74). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238. Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.
Metagegevens
Titel
Dimensional weighting of primary and secondary target-defining dimensions in visual search for singleton conjunction targets
Auteurs
Ralph Weidner
Hermann J. Müller
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2009
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2009
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0208-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2009

Psychological Research 2/2009 Naar de uitgave