Changes in School Adjustment Following School Closure
Contradictory to H1, this study could not establish general decreases in school adjustment during school closure. Arguably, there is significant variation in how adolescents cope with school closures. Previous research has found that some adolescents actually seem to fare well by school closures whereas others are much harder affected (Salmela-Aro et al.
2021). For example, some adolescents might engage more easily with their schoolwork at home than others depending on their social environments. Overall, these different individual pathways may balance each other out. This might explain why the results could not establish an average change in school adjustment during and following school closure across all adolescents. Alternatively, changes in school adjustment could have been small in such a short period of time; and the sample size of the present study might have been too small to detect them.
Instead of general changes in school adjustment, this study showed that the consequences of school closures depended on adolescents’ ethnic and SES backgrounds. In support of H3.1., school closure augmented already existing school adjustment gaps between ethnic minority adolescents and their ethnic majority peers (Heath and Brinbaum,
2014). Previous research already indicated reduced school adjustment particularly among adolescents from ethnic minority (e.g., Paizan et al.
2021) and lower SES backgrounds (e.g., Easterbrook et al.
2022). The current findings corroborate and extend these earlier findings. They demonstrate that gaps in school belonging, school engagement, and academic self-esteem between ethnic minority and ethnic majority youth were augmented or appeared during school closure. This was the case despite the brief time interval between the two time points at pre- and during school closure (i.e., only 3 months). The findings did not show significant SES-based differences during school closure. Still, there were also indications that reopening the schools benefitted the school adjustment of youth with a lower subjective SES more. Moreover, the results demonstrate that ethnicity-based gaps in school adjustment reduced or disappeared again after reopening the schools.
Embedding these findings within the COVID-19 containment measures at the time in Belgium, there were almost no containment measures in place 1 week before school closure (T1); schools closed one day to the next. At T2, a full lockdown was in place including the closure of schools. Schools were fully open again 1 year after school closure (T3). No school closures happened between the second and third wave of data collection (except for a prolonged Fall break). Still, other containment measures were in place at T3, including the closure of non-essential shops, bars, and restaurants. Although these measures were not lifted yet, the results showed that school adjustment gaps reduced or disappeared at T3. It is thus unclear whether increasing gaps at T2 were solely due to school closures or due to a combination of COVID-19-related factors including other containment measures. Still, for the reduction in gaps at T3, it is most likely that schools reopening was the driving factor. School belonging, school engagement, and academic self-esteem are precursors of later school achievement (Gillen-O’Neel and Fuligni,
2013). The findings of the present study might therefore (partially) explain increased achievement gaps during the COVID-19 pandemic. School closures might therefore have important long-lasting consequences via reduced achievement; and they may maintain or augment already existing inequalities.
The Role of School and Home Contexts
The present findings not only demonstrated the consequences of school closures but also highlighted protective mechanisms in the school context. In line with H2, adolescents who received higher quality of online instruction and experienced less teacher rejection reported better school adjustment during school closure; and this was the case while taking initial differences in school adjustment prior to school closure into account. In line with a dynamic systems theory of development (Thelen and Smith,
2006), different social interactions may lead to different developmental pathways. Research on the protective and risk factors in development posits that positive relationships with peers (Grew et al.
2022) or teachers (McGrath and Van Bergen,
2015) can protect adolescents against various stressors. Accordingly, positive teacher-pupil interactions during school closures seem to sustain adolescents’ school adjustment. Both components reflect different aspects of teacher-pupil interactions. Quality of online instruction refers to more instrumental support during school closure. Quality of teacher-pupil relationships refers to a more affective component of teacher-pupil interactions. Previous research already established the important role of teacher rejection (and support) for adolescents’ school adjustment (Roorda et al.
2011). The current findings extend these findings by looking at
changes in school adjustment from before to during school closures during the COVID-19 pandemic; and they demonstrate the important complementary and additive effect of instrumental and affective components of teacher-pupil interactions during school closures.
While teacher-student interactions are key to the school adjustment of all adolescents, they are particularly important for adolescents from ethnic minority backgrounds. In line with H3.2, high teacher support protected school adjustment during school closures especially among ethnic minority youth. However, none of the other interactions were significant. This implies that processes work rather similar across adolescents from different ethnic and SES backgrounds. The quality of the teacher-pupil relationship might be particularly prone to show different effects among ethnic minority and ethnic majority youth. Teachers generally have an ethnic majority background. Consequently, teacher-pupil relationships have a distinct intergroup component for ethnic minority youth (Baysu et al.
2021). Feeling support from the teacher might signal identity valuation and inclusion in school to ethnic minority youth; and it might therefore buffer against negative school adjustment consequences during school closures (Baysu et al.
2021). The quality of online instruction might on the contrary be less susceptible to perceived differential treatment by teachers, because it relates to perceptions of instrumental support (at a group-level) such as correcting exercises together or what you learn at school. Similarly, these processes might be less pronounced among youth from lower SES backgrounds, whose disadvantaged backgrounds are relatively less visible (but see SOM.3 for significant interactions between teacher rejection and SES on psychological adjustment).
Interestingly, this study did not find any effects of the home context (i.e., family support with homework and resources at home) when taking the school context into account (but see SOM.
3 for significant interactions between family support with homework and SES on psychological adjustment). Whereas previous studies found that the home context plays a critical role during school closures (e.g., Paizan et al.
2021), they did not simultaneously take the school context into account. It seems that schools, and teachers in particular, play a more important role in sustaining adolescents’ school adjustment during school closures. This is in line with the idea that contextual factors often have domain-specific effects (Benner and Graham,
2013). Accordingly, school-related factors seem to predict school adjustment more strongly than home-related factors. Teachers might serve as a critical bridge between school and pupils during school closures; and they might therefore secure adolescents’ school adjustment regardless of the home context. Future research should incorporate factors in both school and home contexts to shed further light on which factors can buffer against the adverse consequences of school closures.
Although school and home contexts related relatively similar to adolescents’ school adjustment, adolescents from ethnic minority and lower SES backgrounds were less likely to reside in supportive environments during school closures. In line with H3.3., adolescents from ethnic minority and lower SES backgrounds were less likely to receive support from family and teachers and had fewer resources. These contexts had in turn negative consequences for their school adjustment during school closures. Importantly, adolescents were all in the same school with the same teachers. These differences can therefore not be attributed to school-level variation, but rather point toward perceptions of differential treatment based on ethnic and SES origin. Although positive contexts benefit adolescents from different ethnic and SES backgrounds similarly, less optimal contexts pose them at risk of more adverse developmental outcomes during school closures.
However, contradictory to the hypothesis, ethnic minority adolescents also reported
higher quality of online instruction than their ethnic majority peers; and this protected their school adjustment during school closure. Teachers may be aware of pupils’ lack of resources or difficult situation at home (Kim and Asbury,
2020). Consequently, they may try to provide instrumental support as reflected by higher quality of online instruction. This is also supported by the fact that quality of online instruction was higher among adolescents in vocational (vs. academic) school tracks, where ethnic minority youth are overrepresented (Baysu et al.
2018). Classes in vocational tracks are generally smaller. It might therefore have been easier for teachers to provide high-quality online instruction to vulnerable pupils in these classes (vs. larger classes in academic tracks). These findings combined demonstrate that adolescents from disadvantaged backgrounds might be more likely to benefit from positive school (and home) contexts but might also be lacking some critical support structures (Baysu et al.
2021). Although ethnic minority youth received higher quality of online instruction, this only compensated their higher experienced rejection from teachers. They still showed poorer school adjustment compared to their ethnic majority peers during school closure nevertheless, even when taking into account initial differences in their school adjustment before school closure. Moreover, these effects also endured until (at least) 1 year later, so reopening the schools did not fully recover some of the damage that had been done.
Overall, results were more pronounced among youth from ethnic minority backgrounds than from lower SES backgrounds. Measuring SES among adolescents remains a challenge (Svedberg et al.
2016). The present study used two different indicators of adolescents’ SES backgrounds, namely parental education and subjective SES. Although parental education is most commonly used, adolescents are generally not well-equipped to answer these questions (Hammond et al.
2021). The lack of significant findings of parental education in the present study might be attributed to this. Although there were some effects of subjective SES, they were also less pronounced than those of ethnic origin. Interestingly, parental education and subjective SES are not strongly related until late adolescence or young adulthood (Hammond et al.
2021). This points toward developmental changes in understandings of SES. Although more age-appropriate, subjective SES might relate to a separate subdimension in adolescence than more objective indicators like parental education (Svedberg et al.
2016). For example, objective and subjective SES measures were found to relate to adolescents’ substance use in different ways (Hammond et al.
2021). Arguably, early adolescents, like in the present study, might be better equipped to answer questions regarding their ethnic origin than their SES backgrounds. It could be that these effects were consequently more pronounced. Future research should aim to develop new methods to capture adolescents’ SES backgrounds to shed more light on developmental pathways across different SES groups.
Limitations
This study is not without limitations. First, the sample size was rather small and adolescents came from only one school. The design of the study prevented the addition of participants (and other schools). Post-hoc sensitivity analyses showed that it was possible to detect small to medium effects. Accordingly, it is possible that (very) small effects might not have been detected and were missed out on in the present study. As in all convenience samples, it is unclear how representative or generalizable the findings are. However, the results are very much in line with previous studies that used larger samples and were conducted in different countries. This strengthens the confidence in the current study’s conclusions. Second, the difficult circumstances of the data collection resulted in relatively high attrition rates. This was particularly the case for the three-wave data; and to a lesser extent for the first two waves - that were the focus of most analyses. Attrition analyses showed that ethnic minority youth (and older adolescents and adolescents in vocational school tracks) were more likely to drop out. Since there was no selective attrition in the outcome variables and the findings do not go against the Missing at Random assumption, FIML is an unbiased and robust method to handle these missing data (Enders and Bandalos
2009). Third, some of the measures had a somewhat low internal consistency. The factor structure of all measures was confirmed and showed proper factor loadings. Additionally, most of these measures such as teacher rejection (α between 0.70 and 0.88; Baysu et al.
2021; Brondolo et al.
2005) and academic self-esteem (
α = 0.92; Heatherton and Polivy,
1991) have been used in previous research and had higher internal consistencies. Their slightly lower internal consistencies in the present study could be due to the smaller sample size (Bujang et al.
2018). Even though the results were meaningful, they should still be interpreted with caution. Finally, the study could not distinguish between different ethnic origin groups. Potentially, adolescents from some origin groups are harder affected by school closures than others (Bayrakdar and Guveli,
2020). This might particularly be the case for those from lower SES backgrounds. Future research should incorporate intersections between adolescents’ ethnic and SES backgrounds to further disentangle their relative and joint contributions.