Discussion
During the last two decades Goth has developed into a highly visible youth cultural scene for young people attracted to black-romantic music and esthetics. Earlier studies have shown that identifying with of being part of the Goth scene (in the UK) may indicate serious internalizing problem behaviors such as depression, self-harm and suicide attempts. This study set out to explore the potential association between, specifically, liking Goth music and depressive symptoms, as adolescent music preference is easier to measure (both for adolescents and researchers alike) than the more abstract concept of youth culture. An advantage of assessing Goth music preference as opposed to Goth youth culture may be that a group is captured that may not be “hardcore” Goth stylistically, but attracted to the themes present in Goth music, its lyrics, and visualization. Furthermore, the understudied potential moderating role of gender in the link between Goth and depressive symptoms was analyzed. Results show that, indeed, the adolescent group reporting to like Goth music is larger than the one indicating that Goth youth culture suits them. The most important finding is that Goth music preference indicated depressive symptoms across adolescence, but that gender did not moderate this relation.
Analysis of the data of a four-wave longitudinal study brought forward evidence that at the onset of adolescence, mean age of the respondents 12.4 years, Goth was not a well-known music genre. But during the two years thereafter, the obscurity of Goth music vanished at a fast pace. At the time of the second wave of the study, the respondents were on average 14.4 years of age and Goth was not only a well-known genre, it was also relatively popular. In mid-adolescence the popularity of Goth music peaked with about 1 in 10 adolescents reporting (somewhat) liking this music. Two years later, at T4, mean age 16.6 years, only about 1 in 20 (still) liked Goth.
More important in the context of this study is the finding that across adolescence Goth preference becomes associated with depression. In the correlational study no relation between liking Goth and depressive symptoms was found at T1, but this association became more salient across adolescence, i.e. in later waves of the study. In addition, results of the GLMM analysis showed a clear Goth * years interaction indicating that across time youths into Goth music were more inclined to depressive tendencies than their peers who did not particularly value or even disliked this type of music. These results, thus, corroborate earlier research showing that a subcultural identification with Goth—implying liking the music that is a vital part of this subculture—is associated with serious internalizing distress. This association is present in the UK (Bowes et al.
2015; Young et al.
2006) and now shown to be also present in the cultural context of the current study, The Netherlands. Goth music may have an equal appeal cross-culturally and by attracting the same type of cultural “radicals”, relates to similar internalizing problems regardless of cultural context (Bešić and Kerr
2009). In addition, the results show that depressive tendencies are more prevalent in girls, a finding that has been brought forward in a large number of other studies (Hilt and Nolen-Hoeksema
2013), but no salient Goth * gender * years interaction was discerned. In other words, liking Goth does not increase depressive symptoms more in girls when compared to boys.
Earlier research suggests that depressive adolescents may be drawn disproportionally to Goth youth subculture. A selection effect may therefore explain the relation between Goth and depression (Bowes et al.
2015). This interpretation is in line with the finding that in early adolescence the link between liking Goth music and depressive symptoms is not present but develops across adolescence. Indeed, troubled young people may be fascinated by the dark symbolism and gloominess of Goth music and find relief in the fact that, for example, one of its most expressive proponents, Marilyn Manson, is not shy to expose his self-inflicted wounds and wade in misery in his lyrics, artwork and his biography
The Long Hard Road out of Hell (Manson and Strauss
1998). Goth music may be comforting as it addresses difficulties that they may face themselves. They may be attracted by the colorful clothes and hairdos of Goths and even their outsider status, signaled by their appearance, may be appealing, as it reflects their own feelings of not fitting in the regular crowd system.
It must be noted though that this study started from another vantage point. Already early in adolescence most young people know different types of music well. They further have developed more or less outspoken preferences for specific music types. Due to their choice of music, adolescents may be attracted to and befriend other young people who like the same music. Thus, a selection effect is at work, but now on the basis of music preference, and not on the basis of depression. Goth music preference may lead to Goth crowd membership. Once part of the Goth crowd, an altogether different mechanism may occur. Goths may become more similar; the Goth crowd is a basis for socialization and members may model or dictate the looks, ideas and feelings of their peers. Thus, not only selection but also socialization is relevant for social anxiety in Goth and punk subcultures. It has been noted furthermore that adolescents in these crowds became isolated from the rest of the social network over time, as they not only befriended others outside their crowd to a lesser degree but were also being targeted less as a friend (Van Zalk et al.
2011). Our results also hint at the possibility that a similar mechanism is at work for depressive symptoms. Across adolescence, the popularity of Goth music decreases, but among its fans depressive symptoms increase. This substantiates the assumption that Goth music fans become more “hardcore” as time passes, with remaining fans ever more similar, not only in looks and values, but also in subculturally socialized and legitimized depressive feelings.
These results corroborate a crucial element of the Music Marker Theory. A preference for Goth indeed predicts later differences in adolescent internalizing distress. In that sense the results further validate a more general rule: stronger and more persistent identifications with non-mainstream music and non-conventional crowds are associated with more problem behaviors throughout adolescence (Prinstein and La Greca
2002; Doornwaard et al.
2012). In contrast, stronger and more persistent identifications with mainstream music and conventional crowds are generally associated with fewer problems (Ter Bogt et al.
2013). In future studies it would be worthwhile to track other non-mainstream types of music and their function as an early marker for internalizing problem behavior. Now that Goth seems past its heyday in the late nineties and early new century, other non-mainstream crowds should be the object of study. Emo, another music type and subcultural scene within the rock spectrum, dating back to American punk and hardcore of the eighties and nineties, is a likely candidate (Kiuru et al.
2012; Baker et al.
2013). Less black in appearance with fans wearing neon dyed hair and fantasy-movie inspired outfits, Emo subculture and its key elements Emo music and lyrics is characterized by a similar romantic out-of-place-ness as Goth and, potentially, corresponding health risks. More recently Billie Eilish has become a superstar with looks and attitudes referring back to Goth subculture, addressing teenage angst, loneliness and depression in her lyrics (Haring
2019).
Clinicians and professionals working with youth should, thus, be aware that non-mainstream preferences may indicate problems. This does not only apply to non-mainstream music in the rock, hip-hop or dance music traditions. For example, in a correlation study on music and internalizing distress a link between on the one hand liking complex non-mainstream music (classical music, jazz) or liking a broad range of types of music (omnivorous music preference) and, on the other hand, withdrawn behavior and anxiousness was found (Mulder et al.
2007).
This study regarded a large number of young people whose development was assessed longitudinally. Still, it did not directly address socialization of depressive symptoms (Conway et al.
2011) or convergence to group standards in depressive behavior (Kiuru et al.
2012). Finding the exact mechanics behind an adolescent’s choice of a subcultural group on the basis of his/her music preferences and the influence that the social norms and culture of that group on the adolescent is a valid topic for future studies. Additionally, it was not addressed whether the Goth-depressive symptoms link was due to Goth music socializing adolescents into depressive symptoms or depressed adolescents selecting Goth music (Young et al.
2006). However, this study’s modeling strategy was based on findings from earlier studies that found music predicting later adolescent problem behavior and not vice versa (Franken et al.
2017; Selfhout et al.
2008; Slater and Henry
2013; Ter Bogt et al.
2013). Therefore, Goth music was introduced as a predictor in the models. Third, no extended set of confounding variables was introduced. Obviously, gender was included as this study focused on differences between girls and boys in the link between music preferences and depression. Furthermore, educational level was tested, but including this factor in the models did not change or attenuate the results. It has to be noted though, that other studies included a strong set of confounders such as social class, substance use, maternal depression, exposure to life events and childhood peer victimization, but found that these factors did not (strongly) attenuate the link between belonging to the Goth scene and internalizing distress (Bowes et al.
2015; Young et al.
2006). In addition, early depressive symptoms have been shown to be a solid predictor of later depressive symptoms in adolescence (Garber et al.
2002). In the current study, earlier depressive episodes were accounted for, hence, it teased out Goth music preferences’ unique contribution in predicting depressive feelings later in adolescence. Fourth, a single item measure for Goth music preference was used instead of a scale. However, the music preference questions were straight forward and well understood by respondents. Repeating them in a slightly different format, with thirteen preferences to be measured with the MPQ, may irritate the adolescent respondents who might find these additional questions to be redundant. The MPQ items have low numbers of missing values even though the MPQ questions were located at the end of the survey. Cross-wave consistency in answering patterns on music items also indicates strong construct validity and test-retest reliability. Fifth, a statistical framework was utilized that is sensitive to the longitudinal data being nested within persons and the data were analyzed with state-of-the-art Bayesian methods. The final model showing that Goth is indeed linked to depression across time is plausible, still, the coefficients do not depart from zero dramatically, suggesting weak effect sizes. It must be noted though that these effect sizes are rather common in psychological research (Stanley et al.
2018). It must furthermore be noted that these weak associations may be accounted for by the fact that most Goth fans listen to Goth as they simply like the music, and that among these music fans internalizing problems may not be present. These weak, but significant associations hint at a subgroup within the Goth music fanbase showing more depressive symptoms than their Goth or non-Goth peers.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.