Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research 1/2024

Open Access 08-09-2023

Time perspective profile and self-reported health on the EQ-5D

Auteurs: Fanni Rencz, Mathieu F. Janssen

Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research | Uitgave 1/2024

Abstract

Objectives

Time perspective (TP) is a psychological construct that is associated with several health-related behaviours, including healthy eating, smoking and adherence to medications. In this study, we aimed to examine the associations of TP profile with self-reported health on the EQ-5D-5L and to detect which domains display response heterogeneity (cut-point shift) for TP.

Methods

We conducted a secondary analysis of EQ-5D-5L data from a representative general population sample in Hungary (n = 996). The 17-item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory was used to measure individuals' TP on five subscales: past-negative, past-positive, present-fatalist, present-hedonist and future. The associations between TP subscales and EQ-5D-5L domain scores, EQ VAS and EQ-5D-5L index values were analysed by using partial proportional odds models and multivariate linear regressions.

Results

Respondents that scored higher on the past-negative and present-fatalist and lower on the present-hedonist and future subscales were more likely to report more health problems in at least one EQ-5D-5L domain (p < 0.05). Adjusting for socio-economic and health status, three EQ-5D-5L domains exhibited significant associations with various TP subscales (usual activities: present-fatalist and future, pain/discomfort: past-negative and future, anxiety/depression: past-negative, present-fatalist, present-hedonist and future). The anxiety/depression domain showed evidence of cut-point shift.

Conclusions

This study identified response heterogeneity stemming from psychological characteristics in self-reported health on the EQ-5D-5L. TP seems to play a double role in self-reported health, firstly as affecting underlying health and secondly as a factor influencing one’s response behavior. These findings increase our understanding of the non-health-related factors that affect self-reported health on standardized health status measures.
Opmerkingen

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
It is far more important to know what person
the disease has than what disease the person has.”
Hippocrates

Introduction

The belief that psychological dispositions are related to health dates back to Hippocrates (‘the theory of the four humours’) in the 5th century B.C. and has since been generating substantial interest. Over the past decades, an increasing body of evidence demonstrated that personality characteristics are linked to a wide spectrum of health outcomes, including longevity, predicting the development and course of various chronic physical conditions and self-reported health status [14]. Time perspective (TP) is a psychological construct that describes how one subjectively focuses on the past, present and future [5]. Some authors consider it to be a trait, while others argue that it is a flexible cognitive structure that may change over the life course, or in response to life events (e.g. traumatic exposure), psychological interventions or social environment [5, 6]. In their seminal work, Zimbardo and Boyd distinguished two main aspects of TP, the directionality of one’s thoughts towards time (i.e. past, present or future orientation) and their emotional valence (i.e. positive or negative) [7]. TP has gained increasing attention in the contexts of health and healthcare over the past 30 years. Prior work suggests that persons with past negative view are more likely to experience depression [7], whereas people having a present TP more commonly report using alcohol, drugs, and tobacco [8]. Future TP, in contrast, demonstrated a positive effect on medication adherence and negative effect on partaking in risky sexual behaviour [9, 10].
A few previous studies using the general population or smaller patient samples identified a relationship between TP and self-reported health as measured by a single item health question, the SF-36, SF-12 and WHOQOL-HIV [1115]. To date, no studies have investigated the association between TP and self-reported health using the EQ-5D. The EQ-5D is the most widely used generic preference-accompanied health status measure with a variety of economic (e.g. cost-utility analysis) and non-economic applications (e.g. observational clinical studies, clinical trials, population health surveys and measuring health inequalities) [1620]. Previous streams of research with the EQ-5D mostly concentrated on the associations between self-reported health and certain personality traits and lifestyle-related attitudes. In these studies, self-reporting less health problems was related to conscientiousness and internal locus of control, while neuroticism, openness, type D personality, ‘live-for-today’ and ‘unconfident fatalist’ attitudes were related to reporting more health problems on the EQ-5D [2125].
A major measurement issue related to self-reporting own health is that, in addition to the probable link between different psychological factors and health outcomes; for example, as a result of variation in health behaviours or lifestyle choices, psychological characteristics such as TP profile, may also lead to systematic variations in self-reporting own health across respondents with the same health status. It is therefore possible that two people with different psychological traits and the same health status perceive and rate their health differently. This latter variation is commonly referred to as response heterogeneity [26], which may lead to differential item functioning in health status measures [27]. Guided by the framework outlined by Lindeboom and van Doorslaer, two forms of response heterogeneity may be distinguished: cut-point shift and index shift [26]. Cut-point shift occurs when the relative positions of the level thresholds change for certain subgroups of respondents directly influencing the shape of the distribution of responses [28]. Index shift refers to a parallel shift in all of the reporting thresholds for certain subgroups of respondents that leads to a shift in the distribution of responses either to the right or left [28]. An extensive body of studies provided evidence of the presence of cut-point or index shift in self-reported health mainly using a single health question [26, 2830]; however, none of these have investigated individuals’ psychological characteristics as a source of response heterogeneity in self-reported health.
This study seeks to explore the possible link between individuals’ TP profile and self-reported health on the five dimensions of EQ-5D-5L, EQ VAS and index values. We aim to go beyond merely demonstrating the association between TP and self-reported health by attempting to detect which EQ-5D-5L domains possibly display response heterogeneity for TP. Among the two forms of response heterogeneity, our sample enabled to investigate the presence of cut-point shift. We hypothesized that respondents with future, present-hedonistic and past-positive TP reported fewer health problems and respondents with present-fatalistic and past-negative TP reported more health problems [11, 12]. We expected that the pain/discomfort and anxiety/depression domains would be more likely to exhibit response heterogeneity for TP given the more subjective nature of these domains [31].

Methods

Study design and population

We conducted a secondary analysis of the cross-sectional data of the EQ-5D-Y-3L (youth) valuation study in Hungary [32]. Respondents were recruited from a large online panel in April and May 2021. The target population for the online panel survey was the Hungarian adult general population aged 18 years or over, and quota sampling methods were used to achieve a representative sample in terms of gender and age (across seven age groupings: 18–24, 25–34, 35–44, 45–54, 55–64, 65–74 and 75+). Ethical approval to conduct the data collection was granted by the Research Ethics Committee of the Corvinus University of Budapest (no. KRH/31/2021). All respondents entering the survey were asked to provide informed consent. To ensure high quality of DCE responses, two quality control criteria were used in the survey regarding completion time and dominant pairs [32]. Respondents that failed to meet either one or both of these quality control criteria were excluded and did not continue with the remaining sections of the survey. Participants that successfully met both quality control criteria proceeded to complete the EQ-5D-5L, 17-item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI) and socio-demographic and health-related questions in a fixed order. For the latter, a list of 12 common chronic health conditions was provided for respondents. The question specifically asked respondents to report those health conditions that had been diagnosed by a physician.

EQ-5D-5L

The EQ-5D-5L is a generic preference-accompanied health status measure that comprises two parts, a descriptive system and a vertical visual analogue scale (EQ VAS) ranging from ‘the worst imaginable health state’ (0) to ‘the best imaginable health state’ (100) [33]. The descriptive system is composed of the following five health domains: mobility, self-care, usual activities, pain/discomfort and anxiety/depression. Each domain has five response levels: no problems (1), slight problems (2), moderate problems (3), severe problems (4) and extreme problems/unable to (5). These five domains describe overall 3125 unique health profiles, with 11111 being the best (full health) and 55555 being the worst possible health state (pits). Index values (i.e. utilities) may be assigned to each profile using a value set that reflects societal preferences. In this study, we computed index values using the Hungarian EQ-5D-5L value set that had been developed using composite time trade-off method [34].

17-item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI)

To measure respondents’ TP profile, we used the validated Hungarian version of the 17-item ZTPI that is a shorter version of the original 56-item questionnaire [7, 35]. ZTPI is a multidimensional TP scale that is based on the considerations proposed by Zimbardo and Boyd [7]. Figure 1 presents the 17 items of the scale, with each being represented by a statement and assessed on a five-point scale with the endpoints of ‘very untrue’ and ‘very true’. Item scores were summed into subscale scores (past-negative, past-positive, present-fatalistic, present-hedonistic and future) following the official scoring of ZTPI (range of subscale scores 1–5, where a higher score indicates more of the trait being measured).

Statistical analyses

There were no missing values as all questions were mandatory in the online survey. Descriptive statistics were used to provide an overview of the characteristics of the study population. Mean, standard deviation (SD), median, interquartile range, minimum and maximum were computed for continuous variables (EQ VAS, EQ-5D-5L index values and each TP subscale).

Partial proportional odds models: exploring response heterogeneity

We adopted an analytical strategy that aims to test the equivalence in response level thresholds controlling for a variety of individual characteristics, such as socio-demographics and health status [26, 29, 30]. We treated EQ-5D-5L domain scores as ordinal data due to the hierarchy of response levels. The associations between TP subscales and EQ-5D-5L domain scores were analysed using partial proportional odds models [36]. The self-care domain was omitted from the analysis due to limited variability of responses. For the other four domains, responses were collapsed into three categories (no problems, slight problems and moderate-to-extreme problems) to account for the low number of respondents reporting severe or extreme health problems. The three categories were divided by two response thresholds: level 1 vs. levels 2–5 (‘no problems’ vs. ‘slight-to-extreme problems’) and levels 1–2 vs. levels 3–5 (‘no or slight problems’ vs. ‘moderate-to-extreme problems’). The five ZTPI subscale scores, four socio-demographic characteristics (age, gender, education, income) and 12 health condition groups were included in the models as independent variables. These latter were considered as proxies for ‘true’ underlying health status. For all independent variables, the proportional odds assumption was tested using Brant test [37]. The model was sequentially refitted until no variables complied with this assumption. We report the results as odds ratios (ORs) and their 95% confidence intervals. Independent variables that satisfy the proportional odds assumption have a single OR for both response thresholds. Whereas, independent variables not meeting the proportional odds assumption have different ORs for the threshold of ‘no problems’ vs. ‘slight-to-extreme problems’ relative to ‘no or slight problems’ vs. ‘moderate-to-extreme problems’ providing evidence of response heterogeneity (cut-point shift).

Multivariate linear regressions

Multivariate linear regressions were performed to investigate the association between TP subscales and EQ VAS and EQ-5D-5L index values. Two separate regressions were run for both outcomes of interest to explore the contribution of TP profile to the explained variance in EQ VAS and EQ-5D-5L index value. In the first models (‘without TP’), EQ VAS and EQ-5D-5L index were regressed on four socio-demographic variables (age, gender, education, income) and 12 chronic health condition groups. In the second models (‘with TP’), the five ZTPI subscale scores were also added to the regression as independent variables in addition to respondents’ socio-demographic characteristics and chronic conditions. To ease interpretation of the coefficients, ZTPI subscale scores were rescaled to range from 0 to 4 before the regression analyses. The presence of heteroscedasticity was confirmed by the Breusch-Pagan test [38]. Robust standard errors were used to correct for any heteroscedasticity. The ‘without TP’ and ‘with TP’ models were compared with regard to the explained variance (R2 statistic). All analyses were performed in Stata 14 and p-values <0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Characteristics of the study population

Out of the 1251 participants, 255 (20.4%) did not meet either one or both quality control criteria in the DCE and were consequently excluded from the study. The final sample consisted of 996 respondents and showed an excellent representativeness for gender and age groups. There was a higher proportion of highly educated respondents compared to the adult general population in Hungary (Table 1). The majority reported overall good health status with mean EQ VAS of 78.03 and EQ-5D-5L index of 0.919 (Table 2). Overall, 72, 93, 80, 53 and 67% had no problems with mobility, self-care, usual activities, pain/discomfort and anxiety/depression, and 38% of the sample reported to be in full health (11111).
Table 1
Characteristics of the study population
Variables
Reference populationa
Total sample (n = 996)
Variables
Reference populationa
Total sample (n = 996)
%
n
%
%
n
%
Age (years)
   
Gender
   
 18–24
10
103
10
 Female
53
522
52
 25–34
15
157
16
 Male
47
474
48
 35–44
20
195
20
Education
   
 45–54
16
167
17
 Primary school or less
45
219
22
 55–64
17
172
17
 Secondary school
33
366
37
 65–74
13
134
13
 College/university degree
31
411
41
 75+
10
68
7
EQ-5D-5L domains
   
Household’s per capita net monthly income (HUF)
   
Mobility
   
 Quintile 1 (<= 87500.50)
n/a
161
16
 No problems
n/a
721
72
 Quintile 2 (87500.51–131250.25)
n/a
154
15
 Slight problems
n/a
198
20
 Quintile 3 (131250.26–175000.33)
n/a
145
15
 Moderate problems - unable to
n/a
77
8
 Quintile 4 (175000.34–225000.33)
n/a
165
17
Self-care
   
 Quintile 5 (225000.34+)
n/a
162
16
 No problems
n/a
930
93
 Don't know/refused to answer
n/a
209
21
 Slight problems
n/a
44
4
Chronic health conditionsb,c
   
 Moderate problems - unable to
n/a
22
2
 None
52
461
46
Usual activities
   
 Allergy
15
160
16
 No problems
n/a
800
80
 Anxiety
n/a
78
8
 Slight problems
n/a
146
15
 Asthma
5
56
6
 Moderate - extreme problems
n/a
50
5
 Cancer
2
33
3
Pain/discomfort
   
 Cardiovascular disease
>8
120
12
 No problems
n/a
526
53
 Depression
8
53
5
 Slight problems
n/a
380
38
 Diabetes
9
103
10
 Moderate - extreme problems
n/a
90
9
 Gastrointestinal disease
n/a
75
8
Anxiety/depression
   
 Hypertension
31
305
31
 No problems
n/a
664
67
 Musculoskeletal disease
>20
239
24
 Slight problems
n/a
250
25
 Osteoporosis
6
30
3
 Moderate - extreme problems
n/a
82
8
 Skin disease
n/a
78
8
11111 (full health)
n/a
378
38
aReference values: Hungarian Central Statistical Office: Microcensus 2016
bReference values: Hungarian Central Statistical Office: Health at a glance, 2019
cn=19 don’t know/refused to answer
Figures may not add up 100% due to rounding. n/a = not available
Table 2
Descriptive statistics of EQ VAS, EQ-5D-5L index and ZTPI subscale scores
Measure
Theoretical range
Observed range
Mean
SD
Median
Q1–Q3
EQ VAS
0–100
1–100
78.03
17.22
81
70–90
EQ-5D-5L index
−0.848 to 1
−0.393 to 1
0.919
0.130
0.957
0.907–1
ZTPI future
1–5
1.75–5
3.89
0.55
4.00
3.50–4.25
ZTPI present-fatalistic
1–5
1–5
2.94
0.83
3.00
2.33–3.58
ZTPI present-hedonistic
1–5
1–5
2.65
0.78
2.67
2.00–3.00
ZTPI past-positive
1–5
1–5
3.40
0.81
3.33
3.00–4.00
ZTPI past-negative
1–5
1.5–4.5
2.88
0.50
3.00
2.50–3.25
EQ VAS EuroQol visual analogue scale, ZTPI 17-item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory
The distribution of responses on each ZTPI item is presented in Fig. 1. The item ‘I meet my obligations to friends and authorities on time’ received the highest proportion of affirmative responses (true or very true: 88%), while the disapproval rate (very untrue or untrue) was the highest for the statement ‘I’ve taken my share of abuse and rejection in the past’ (60%). With respect to TP subscales, the highest mean scores were found for the future subscale (3.89), followed by the past-positive (3.40), while the lowest were observed for the present-hedonistic subscale (2.65) (Table 2).

The association between EQ-5D-5L domain responses and TP

As hypothesized, after adjusting for socio-demographic characteristics and health status, respondents that scored higher on the past-negative and present-fatalistic and lower on the present-hedonistic and future subscales were more likely to report more health problems in at least one EQ-5D-5L domain (Table 3). Three EQ-5D-5L domains exhibited significant associations with various TP subscales (usual activities: present-fatalistic and future [range ORs: 0.60–1.26], pain/discomfort: past-negative and future [range of ORs: 0.69–1.47], anxiety/depression: past-negative, present-fatalistic, present-hedonistic and future [range of ORs: 0.42–2.05]). The mobility domain showed no association with TP profile.
Table 3
Partial proportional odds models of the association between time perspective and EQ-5D-5L domains (odds ratio and 95% CI)
 
Mobility
Usual activities
Pain/discomfort
Anxiety/depression
Intercept
0.05 (0.01–0.31)**
0.24 (0.03–1.79)
0.44 (0.09–2.01)
0.20 (0.04–1.12)
Time perspective (ZTPI subscales)
    
 Future
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
0.76 (0.56–1.02)
0.60 (0.43–0.84)**
0.69 (0.54–0.89)**
0.75 (0.57–0.99)*
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
0.42 (0.26–0.69)**
 Present-hedonistic
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
0.98 (0.79–1.22)
1.00 (0.79–1.28)
0.96 (0.8–1.15)
0.90 (0.73–1.10)
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
0.58 (0.40–0.86)**
 Present-fatalistic
1.14 (0.93–1.4)
1.26 (1.00–1.58)*
1.14 (0.96–1.35)
1.59 (1.31–1.92)***
 Past-positive
1.11 (0.89–1.39)
1.09 (0.85–1.39)
1.05 (0.88–1.25)
0.96 (0.80–1.16)
 Past-negative
1.07 (0.76–1.49)
0.89 (0.62–1.29)
1.47 (1.12–1.94)**
2.05 (1.51–2.78)***
Age (years)
1.03 (1.02–1.04)***
1.01 (0.99–1.02)
0.99 (0.98–1.00)
0.97 (0.96–0.98)***
Gender (ref: male)
0.95 (0.68–1.33)
1.58 (1.08–2.29)*
1.56 (1.19–2.05)**
1.27 (0.94–1.71)
Education (ref: primary)
    
 Secondary
1.00 (0.66–1.52)
0.89 (0.56–1.40)
0.76 (0.53–1.09)
0.92 (0.61–1.39)
 Tertiary
1.00 (0.64–1.56)
0.84 (0.52–1.38)
0.79 (0.54–1.16)
1.02 (0.67–1.57)
Income (ref: quintile 1)
    
 Quintile 2
0.98 (0.58–1.65)
0.94 (0.54–1.63)
1.35 (0.85–2.15)
0.93 (0.56–1.56)
 Quintile 3
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
0.83 (0.46–1.50)
0.91 (0.50–1.68)
1.60 (0.99–2.60)
1.19 (0.70–2.01)
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
1.58 (0.72–3.47)
 Quintile 4
0.83 (0.47–1.45)
0.44 (0.23–0.85)*
1.01 (0.63–1.64)
0.75 (0.44–1.28)
 Quintile 5
0.71 (0.39–1.30)
0.60 (0.31–1.16)
1.31 (0.80–2.16)
1.24 (0.72–2.11)
 Don't know/refused to answer
0.72 (0.42–1.25)
0.58 (0.33–1.05)
1.03 (0.66–1.62)
0.81 (0.50–1.33)
Chronic conditions (ref: none)
    
 Allergy
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
1.47 (0.95–2.28)
2.09 (1.33–3.27)**
0.80 (0.54–1.19)
0.90 (0.60–1.36)
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
1.53 (0.86–2.72)
 Anxiety
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
1.75 (0.92–3.32)
1.48 (0.73–2.99)
2.18 (1.23–3.87)**
8.77 (4.92–15.65)***
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
0.46 (0.15–1.40)
 Asthma
0.94 (0.49–1.78)
1.83 (0.96–3.46)
1.58 (0.88–2.83)
0.79 (0.4–1.57)
 Cancer
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
1.52 (0.72–3.20)
1.35 (0.60–3.03)
1.62 (0.79–3.31)
0.99 (0.41–2.40)
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
3.24 (1.01–10.39)*
 Cardiovascular disease
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
1.39 (0.85–2.27)
2.26 (1.43–3.58)**
2.24 (1.48–3.38)***
1.30 (0.81–2.10)
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
2.92 (1.63–5.24)***
 Depression
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
1.55 (0.73–3.29)
3.25 (1.49–7.09)**
2.7 (1.34–5.44)**
4.83 (2.38–9.80)***
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
8.67 (3.26–23.07)***
 Diabetes
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
1.20 (0.74–1.96)
1.59 (0.93–2.72)
1.13 (0.71–1.79)
1.66 (0.97–2.84)
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
4.24 (1.95–9.21)***
 Gastrointestinal disease
0.61 (0.33–1.13)
0.90 (0.47–1.72)
1.32 (0.79–2.19)
2.52 (1.48–4.29)**
 Hypertension
1.72 (1.20–2.48)**
1.64 (1.09–2.47)*
1.61 (1.17–2.23)**
1.17 (0.81–1.71)
 Musculoskeletal disease
8.09 (5.69–11.50)***
4.23 (2.88–6.22)***
4.40 (3.17–6.13)***
1.12 (0.77–1.63)
 Osteoporosis
1.11 (0.50–2.44)
1.29 (0.55–3.04)
1.64 (0.78–3.45)
0.92 (0.39–2.18)
 Skin disease
    
  Level 1 vs. Levels 2–5
0.78 (0.44–1.39)
0.67 (0.35–1.28)
1.10 (0.67–1.8)
0.58 (0.31–1.08)
  Levels 1–2 vs. Levels 3–5
1.89 (0.83–4.34)
Model fit
χ2(28) = 348.86, < 0.001,
Pseudo R2 = 0.2326
χ2(28) = 242.15, < 0.001,
Pseudo R2 = 0.2000
χ2(27) = 273.02, < 0.001,
Pseudo R2 = 0.1486
χ2(31) = 329.07, p < 0.001,
Pseudo R2 = 0.2008
Note that modelling was not possible for the self-care dimension due to limited variability in responses. Variables that meet the proportional odds assumption exhibit a consistent odds ratio across response thresholds, i.e. comparing level 1 vs. levels 2–5 to levels 1–2 vs. levels 3–5. Conversely, variables that do not satisfy the proportional odds assumption demonstrate different odds ratios between the ‘level 1 vs. levels 2–5’ and the ‘levels 1–2 vs. levels 3–5’ thresholds, indicating the presence of response heterogeneity (cut-point shift).
ZTPI = 17-item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory
Level 1 = no problems, level 2 = slight problems, level 3–5 = moderate-to-extreme problems.
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001
Several TP subscales, socio-demographic and health status characteristics were found to be in a significant association with one or more EQ-5D-5L domains without evidence of cut-point shifting. For every one-year increase in age, the odds of reporting a one-level higher severity of problems was 1.03 (95% CI 1.02–1.04) for mobility and 0.97 (95% CI 0.96–0.98) for anxiety/depression. Women were 1.58 (95% CI 1.08–2.29) and 1.56 (95% CI 1.19–2.05) times more likely to report a one-level higher severity of problems with usual activities and pain/discomfort than men. Education was not associated with any EQ-5D-5L domain scores, but a higher level of income was related to a lower likelihood of reporting a one-level higher severity of problems with usual activities. The presence of different chronic conditions tended to increase the probability of reporting more problems in each EQ-5D-5L domain. Notably, the highest odds ratios were related to the association between having been diagnosed with anxiety and the anxiety/depression domain (OR 8.77, 95% CI 4.92–15.65) and having musculoskeletal disease and the mobility domain (OR 8.09, 95% CI 5.69–11.50).

Response heterogeneity

The anxiety/depression domain showed evidence of cut-point shift as demonstrated by the distinct ORs between the ‘no problems vs. slight-to-extreme problems’ and the ‘no or slight problems vs. moderate-to-extreme problems’ thresholds (Table 3). Individuals with higher present-hedonistic or future TP subscale scores were less likely to report moderate-to-extreme problems compared to no or slight problems (present-hedonistic: OR 0.58, 95% CI 0.40–0.86 and future: OR 0.42, 95% CI 0.26–0.69) relative to slight-to-extreme problems compared to no problems (present-hedonistic: OR 0.90, 95% CI 0.73–1.10 and future: OR 0.75, 95% CI 0.57–0.99). Age, gender and education showed no evidence of cut-point shift. One income quintile demonstrated cut-point shift for mobility; however, both separate coefficients were insignificant. An array of chronic condition categories indicated cut-point shift (mobility: cardiovascular diseases, usual activities: anxiety and depression, pain/discomfort: allergy, anxiety/depression: cancer, diabetes, skin disease). Note that only four of these seven chronic condition groups had a statistically significant association with the respective EQ-5D-5L domains.

The association between TP and EQ VAS and EQ-5D-5L index values

In the first EQ VAS model (‘without TP’), respondents with higher income had slightly higher EQ VAS scores and eight of 12 chronic health conditions were associated with a significant decrease in EQ VAS scores ranging from hypertension (2.55) to depression (10.42) (Table 4). In the second model (‘with TP’), after including respondents’ TP subscale scores in addition to their socio-demographic characteristics and health status, four of the five TP subscales had a significant effect on EQ VAS scores. A one-point increase in the past-negative and present-fatalist subscale scores, all else equal, decreased the EQ VAS score by 2.70 and 2.58 (p < 0.05). By contrast, a one-point increase in the future and present-hedonistic subscale scores, all else equal, resulted in a 3.00 and 1.25 increase in EQ VAS score (p < 0.05). Respondents’ TP profile (including all five TP subscale scores) increased the explained variance in EQ VAS score from 26.6% (‘without TP’) to 30.2% (‘with TP’).
Table 4
OLS regression of the association between time perspective and EQ VAS and EQ-5D-5L index values (regression coefficients and standard errors)
Variables
EQ VAS ‘without TP’
EQ VAS ‘with TP’
EQ-5D-5L index ‘without TP’
EQ-5D-5L index ‘with TP’
Intercept
79.478 (2.43)***
78.979 (4.166)***
0.934 (0.019)***
0.938 (0.031)***
Time perspective (ZTPI subscale score-1)
    
 Future
2.996 (0.935)**
0.016 (0.006)**
 Present-hedonistic
1.246 (0.619)*
0.003 (0.005)
 Present-fatalistic
−2.575 (0.639)***
−0.015 (0.005)**
 Past-positive
0.259 (0.647)
−0.001 (0.004)
 Past-negative
−2.700 (0.98)**
−0.009 (0.007)
Age (years)
0.013 (0.033)
0.011 (0.033)
0.000 (0.000)
0.000 (0.000)
Gender (ref: male)
0.426 (0.982)
0.635 (0.968)
−0.006 (0.008)
−0.005 (0.008)
Education (ref: primary)
   
 Secondary
2.363 (1.454)
2.058 (1.407)
0.008 (0.011)
0.006 (0.011)
 Tertiary
0.915 (1.459)
0.216 (1.419)
0.014 (0.01)
0.009 (0.01)
Income (ref: quintile 1)
   
 Quintile 2
1.343 (2.002)
1.116 (1.954)
0.013 (0.015)
0.011 (0.015)
 Quintile 3
1.781 (1.938)
0.989 (1.877)
0.014 (0.015)
0.009 (0.014)
 Quintile 4
4.736 (1.864)*
3.966 (1.798)*
0.027 (0.014)
0.023 (0.014)
 Quintile 5
4.042 (1.883)*
2.742 (1.832)
0.020 (0.014)
0.012 (0.013)
 Don't know/refused to answer
4.353 (1.765)*
3.683 (1.704)*
0.025 (0.013)
0.021 (0.013)
Chronic conditions (ref: none)
   
 Allergy
0.144 (1.347)
0.111 (1.353)
−0.004 (0.01)
−0.005 (0.01)
 Anxiety
−7.949 (2.129)***
−7.140 (2.141)**
−0.081 (0.019)***
−0.078 (0.019)***
 Asthma
−4.411 (2.138)*
−3.726 (2.189)
−0.018 (0.02)
−0.015 (0.02)
 Cancer
−9.753 (4.185)*
−8.918 (4.123)*
−0.014 (0.025)
−0.009 (0.024)
 Cardiovascular disease
−8.388 (1.831)***
−8.673 (1.781)***
−0.070 (0.019)***
−0.071 (0.018)***
 Depression
−10.416 (2.643)***
−9.385 (2.642)***
−0.101 (0.028)***
−0.095 (0.027)**
 Diabetes
−6.190 (1.74)***
−6.293 (1.686)***
−0.031 (0.017)
−0.032 (0.017)
 Gastrointestinal disease
−2.527 (1.932)
−2.184 (1.877)
−0.013 (0.017)
−0.011 (0.017)
 Hypertension
−2.548 (1.208)*
−2.600 (1.202)*
−0.026 (0.009)**
−0.026 (0.009)**
 Musculoskeletal disease
−7.316 (1.339)***
−7.039 (1.337)***
−0.075 (0.01)***
−0.074 (0.01)***
 Osteoporosis
−7.123 (3.686)
−6.844 (3.639)
−0.004 (0.024)
−0.003 (0.023)
 Skin disease
−0.567 (1.738)
−0.341 (1.786)
0.004 (0.014)
0.006 (0.014)
Model fit
F(21, 974) = 12.04 (< 0.001),
R2 = 0.266
F(26, 969) = 12.51 (< 0.001),
R2 = 0.302
F(21, 974) = 8.25 (< 0.001), R2 = 0.309
F(26, 969) = 7.66 (< 0.001), R2 = 0.326
EQ VAS EuroQol visual analogue scale, TP time perspective, ZTPI 17−item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory
* < 0.05; ** < 0.01; *** p < 0.001
In the first EQ-5D-5L index model (‘without TP’), no socio-demographic characteristics were associated with index values; however, five of 12 chronic health conditions were resulted in a significant decrease in index values ranging from hypertension (0.026) to depression (0.101) (Table 4). In the second model (‘with TP’), after including respondents’ TP subscale scores in addition to their socio-demographic characteristics and health status, two TP subscales had a significant effect on EQ-5D-5L index values. A one-point increase in the present-fatalistic and future TP subscale scores, was associated with a decrease of 0.015 and an increase of 0.016 in EQ-5D-5L index, all else equal (p < 0.05). Respondents’ TP profile increased the explained variance in EQ-5D-5L index from 30.9% (‘without TP’) to 32.6% (‘with TP’).

Discussion

This study contributes to the growing literature on the link between psychological dispositions and self-reported health on the EQ-5D. Using a large general population sample from Hungary, it provides an insight into the association between individuals’ TP profiles and self-reported health on the EQ-5D. Three EQ-5D domains (usual activities, pain/discomfort and anxiety/depression) as well as the EQ VAS and EQ-5D index values were associated with respondents’ TP profile. Furthermore, we demonstrated the presence of response heterogeneity in the anxiety/depression domain; the probability of reporting more problems in this domain decreased with having more future and present-hedonistic characteristics. As such, this is the first study that identified response heterogeneity on the EQ-5D arising from individual psychological factors. Other authors have used item response theory, Rasch-analysis, Mantel-Haenszel statistics and ordinal logistic regressions, and reported response heterogeneity (or differential item functioning) on the EQ-5D mainly across geographical regions, countries, age groups, sexes, ethnicities, patients vs. proxies and clinically relevant patient groups (e.g. types of cancer or psychosis) [3946].
Multiple TP subscales were significantly associated with EQ-5D-5L and EQ VAS values. However, the overall impact of TP on EQ VAS and EQ-5D-5L values appears to be relatively small. Findings of a large systematic review suggest that personality characteristics account for varying proportions of health (ranging from 0 to 39%), depending on the health status measure used [4]. In our study, TP explained 3.6% of the variance of EQ VAS and 1.7% of EQ-5D-5L index values. Although these findings are in the range of those reported in the abovementioned review, they fall towards the lower end. It is worth noting that the percentage of explained variance in EQ-5D-5L index values also depends on the value set used. It is likely that using a value set of another country, where anxiety/depression has a larger weight compared to the Hungarian value set, would result in slightly higher explained variance.
Our findings suggest that the impact of certain TP scales, such as future or present-fatalistic, on index values and EQ VAS scores may approximate previously reported MID estimates (0.03–0.10 for the EQ-5D-5L index and 7–11 for EQ VAS) [4755]. For example, compared to a respondent scoring the minimum on the future TP subscale, a respondent scoring one, two, three or four has, on average, higher EQ-5D-5L index values by 0.016, 0.032, 0.048 and 0.064 and higher EQ VAS scores by 3, 6, 9 and 12 points, respectively.
Respondents’ TP profile and a few chronic condition groups seem to display cut-point shift, a form of response heterogeneity. It is important to stress that for variables not producing any cut-point shift, but being significantly related to self-reported health (e.g. future TP to usual activities and pain/discomfort), an index shift may still occur. In our analytical framework, we accounted for ‘true’ health status by controlling for respondents’ chronic health conditions; however, response heterogeneity may also affect these variables through false reporting [56]. Future research is recommended to use different approaches (e.g. anchoring vignettes, performance measurements, objective clinical variables and item response theory) to isolate index shift as a reporting behaviour from variations in underlying health status [5762].
Another noteworthy finding from this study is that the EQ-5D showed no evidence of cut-point shift by age, gender and education. Notwithstanding, some domains exhibited significant associations with age or gender that may signal a possible index shift. This is in line with prior work on response heterogeneity on the EQ-5D, whereby older respondents were more likely to report problems with mobility and less likely with anxiety/depression [41, 45]. Even though we cannot rule out the possibility of having more mobility problems with age after controlling for specific chronic health conditions, it may also be possible that these findings are attributable to an index shift. Similarly, our findings suggest a possible index shift on the usual activities and pain/discomfort domains by gender, whereby women were more inclined to report problems than men. In a previous study with cancer patients, the mobility and usual activities domains showed large- and medium-size response heterogeneity by gender [41]. Among the two forms of response heterogeneity distinguished in our analytical framework, index shift is less concerning than cut-point shift due to its linear nature [26].
Possessing more future and present-hedonistic traits may be seen as desirable qualities leading to less health problems, whereas individuals with more past-negative and present-fatalistic characteristics appear to report more health problems. These results are broadly consistent with those of previous studies that identified an association between TP profile and self-reported health measured by various instruments [1113, 15]. As argued above, these associations must be treated with caution as they are presumably a result of both response heterogeneity and true health effects. A possible explanation for the latter is that TP profile has been found to be related to a number of health behaviours, such as exercising, alcohol, tobacco and substance use, attendance at health screenings and adherence to medications [8, 10, 6365]. The association between health outcomes and TP profiles is further supported by evidence of the effectiveness of TP-based psychological interventions, such as ‘Time Perspective Therapy’, which have successfully improved mental health in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder [66].
Our findings may have wider implications for patient management, clinical trials and economic evaluations. It seems that non-health factors, such as TP profile may affect one’s ‘true’ health as well as response behaviour on the EQ-5D. Understanding the relationship between TP and health status may help to identify barriers in treatment adherence and to improve patient self-management. Further research is needed to examine whether psychological characteristics, such as TP profile, may be considered a potential source of systemic differences between the treatment and control groups in clinical trials. Lastly, considering that the EQ-5D index values are used to estimate quality-adjusted life years, individual TP may also represent an uncertainty on the results of cost-effectiveness analyses and healthcare decisions based thereon. Exploring the potential impact of respondents’ TP on health preferences in valuation studies is another an important direction for future research.
This study has a number of limitations. First, we used a general population sample, and therefore, there was less variability in respondents’ health status that motivated us in collapsing response levels and excluding self-care from the domain-specific analyses. Secondly, more abundant information about the clinical status of respondents (e.g. severity/stage, symptoms, limitations in functioning) could have been useful to more adequately adjust our models for ‘true’ health. Thirdly, selection bias may have occurred not only because of the online mode of administration that excluded people without internet access or sufficient computer literacy, but also due to the study design. During the DCE tasks, 255 respondents were excluded based on quality control criteria, such as providing inconsistent responses on the dominant pairs [32]. As these tasks may be viewed as some kind of logical or cognitive test, it is likely that respondents with somewhat higher cognitive abilities accomplished them and therefore were selected to the final sample. Fourthly, the original 56-item ZTPI questionnaire has been subject to some criticisms with regard to its construct validity and dimensionality [5, 67]. In our study, we used a 17-item short version of this scale that performed well in most psychometric tests in an earlier study in Hungary [35]. However, its face validity may still be questioned; for example, some of the items may rather capture beliefs, values or preferences that do not directly relate to TP and therefore may represent alternative psychological constructs [5, 68, 69].
In conclusion, this is the first study to explore the association between individuals’ TP and self-reported health on the EQ-5D and also the first to identify response heterogeneity (cut-point shift) stemming from psychological characteristics on the EQ-5D. It seems that psychological factors may play a double role in self-reported health, firstly as affecting underlying health and secondly as a factor influencing one’s response behavior. These findings increase our understanding of the non-health-related factors that affect self-reported health on standardized health status measures.

Acknowledgement

The authors thank Fatima Al Sayah for her useful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

FR and MFJ are active members of the EuroQol Group. Views expressed in the article are those of the authors and are not necessarily those of the EuroQol Research Foundation.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Ethical approval was obtained from the Research Ethics Committee of the Corvinus University of Budapest (no. KRH/31/2021).
Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Podotherapeut Totaal

Binnen de bundel kunt u gebruik maken van boeken, tijdschriften, e-learnings, web-tv's en uitlegvideo's. BSL Podotherapeut Totaal is overal toegankelijk; via uw PC, tablet of smartphone.

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. American Psychologist, 55(1), 99–109.PubMed Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. American Psychologist, 55(1), 99–109.PubMed
2.
go back to reference Ferguson, E. (2013). Personality is of central concern to understand health: Towards a theoretical model for health psychology. Health Psychology Review, 7(Suppl 1), S32-s70.PubMedPubMedCentral Ferguson, E. (2013). Personality is of central concern to understand health: Towards a theoretical model for health psychology. Health Psychology Review, 7(Suppl 1), S32-s70.PubMedPubMedCentral
3.
go back to reference Jokela, M., Batty, G. D., Nyberg, S. T., Virtanen, M., Nabi, H., Singh-Manoux, A., & Kivimäki, M. (2013). Personality and all-cause mortality: Individual-participant meta-analysis of 3,947 deaths in 76,150 adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 178(5), 667–675.PubMedPubMedCentral Jokela, M., Batty, G. D., Nyberg, S. T., Virtanen, M., Nabi, H., Singh-Manoux, A., & Kivimäki, M. (2013). Personality and all-cause mortality: Individual-participant meta-analysis of 3,947 deaths in 76,150 adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 178(5), 667–675.PubMedPubMedCentral
4.
go back to reference Huang, I. C., Lee, J. L., Ketheeswaran, P., Jones, C. M., Revicki, D. A., & Wu, A. W. (2017). Does personality affect health-related quality of life? A systematic review. PLoS One, 12(3), e0173806.PubMedPubMedCentral Huang, I. C., Lee, J. L., Ketheeswaran, P., Jones, C. M., Revicki, D. A., & Wu, A. W. (2017). Does personality affect health-related quality of life? A systematic review. PLoS One, 12(3), e0173806.PubMedPubMedCentral
5.
go back to reference Mohammed, S., & Marhefka, J. T. (2020). How have we, do we, and will we measure time perspective? A review of methodological and measurement issues. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 41(3), 276–293. Mohammed, S., & Marhefka, J. T. (2020). How have we, do we, and will we measure time perspective? A review of methodological and measurement issues. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 41(3), 276–293.
6.
go back to reference Stolarski, M., Wojciechowski, J., & Matthews, G. (2021). Seeking the origins of time perspectives – Intelligence, temperament, or family environment? A one-year longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Differences, 169, 110080. Stolarski, M., Wojciechowski, J., & Matthews, G. (2021). Seeking the origins of time perspectives – Intelligence, temperament, or family environment? A one-year longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Differences, 169, 110080.
7.
go back to reference Zimbardo, P., & Boyd, J. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288. Zimbardo, P., & Boyd, J. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288.
8.
go back to reference Keough, K. A., Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Who’s smoking, drinking, and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(2), 149–164. Keough, K. A., Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Who’s smoking, drinking, and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(2), 149–164.
9.
go back to reference Kooij, D., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., & Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(8), 867–893.PubMed Kooij, D., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., & Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(8), 867–893.PubMed
10.
go back to reference Sansbury, B., Dasgupta, A., Guthrie, L., & Ward, M. (2014). Time perspective and medication adherence among individuals with hypertension or diabetes mellitus. Patient Education and Counseling, 95(1), 104–110.PubMedPubMedCentral Sansbury, B., Dasgupta, A., Guthrie, L., & Ward, M. (2014). Time perspective and medication adherence among individuals with hypertension or diabetes mellitus. Patient Education and Counseling, 95(1), 104–110.PubMedPubMedCentral
11.
go back to reference Griva, F., Tseferidi, S. I., & Anagnostopoulos, F. (2015). Time to get healthy: Associations of time perspective with perceived health status and health behaviors. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 20(1), 25–33. Griva, F., Tseferidi, S. I., & Anagnostopoulos, F. (2015). Time to get healthy: Associations of time perspective with perceived health status and health behaviors. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 20(1), 25–33.
12.
go back to reference Guthrie, L. C., Butler, S. C., & Ward, M. M. (2009). Time perspective and socioeconomic status: A link to socioeconomic disparities in health? Social Science & Medicine, 68(12), 2145–2151. Guthrie, L. C., Butler, S. C., & Ward, M. M. (2009). Time perspective and socioeconomic status: A link to socioeconomic disparities in health? Social Science & Medicine, 68(12), 2145–2151.
13.
go back to reference Laguette, V., Apostolidis, T., Dany, L., Bellon, N., Grimaud, J. C., & Lagouanelle-Simeoni, M. C. (2013). Quality of life and time perspective in inflammatory bowel disease patients. Quality of Life Research, 22(10), 2721–2736.PubMed Laguette, V., Apostolidis, T., Dany, L., Bellon, N., Grimaud, J. C., & Lagouanelle-Simeoni, M. C. (2013). Quality of life and time perspective in inflammatory bowel disease patients. Quality of Life Research, 22(10), 2721–2736.PubMed
14.
go back to reference Préau, M., Apostolidis, T., Francois, C., Raffi, F., & Spire, B. (2007). Time perspective and quality of life among HIV-infected patients in the context of HAART. AIDS Care, 19(4), 449–458.PubMed Préau, M., Apostolidis, T., Francois, C., Raffi, F., & Spire, B. (2007). Time perspective and quality of life among HIV-infected patients in the context of HAART. AIDS Care, 19(4), 449–458.PubMed
15.
go back to reference Oyanadel, C., & Buela-Casal, G. (2014). Time perception and psychopathology: Influence of time perspective on quality of life of severe mental illness. Actas Españolas de Psiquiatría, 42(3), 99–107.PubMed Oyanadel, C., & Buela-Casal, G. (2014). Time perception and psychopathology: Influence of time perspective on quality of life of severe mental illness. Actas Españolas de Psiquiatría, 42(3), 99–107.PubMed
16.
go back to reference Brooks, R. (1996). EuroQol: The current state of play. Health Policy, 37(1), 53–72.PubMed Brooks, R. (1996). EuroQol: The current state of play. Health Policy, 37(1), 53–72.PubMed
17.
go back to reference Kennedy-Martin, M., Slaap, B., Herdman, M., van Reenen, M., Kennedy-Martin, T., Greiner, W., Busschbach, J., & Boye, K. S. (2020). Which multi-attribute utility instruments are recommended for use in cost-utility analysis? A review of national health technology assessment (HTA) guidelines. European Journal of Health Economics, 21(8), 1245–1257. Kennedy-Martin, M., Slaap, B., Herdman, M., van Reenen, M., Kennedy-Martin, T., Greiner, W., Busschbach, J., & Boye, K. S. (2020). Which multi-attribute utility instruments are recommended for use in cost-utility analysis? A review of national health technology assessment (HTA) guidelines. European Journal of Health Economics, 21(8), 1245–1257.
18.
go back to reference Rencz, F., Gulácsi, L., Drummond, M., Golicki, D., Prevolnik Rupel, V., Simon, J., Stolk, E. A., Brodszky, V., Baji, P., Závada, J., Petrova, G., Rotar, A., & Péntek, M. (2016). EQ-5D in Central and Eastern Europe: 2000–2015. Quality of Life Research, 25(11), 2693–2710.PubMed Rencz, F., Gulácsi, L., Drummond, M., Golicki, D., Prevolnik Rupel, V., Simon, J., Stolk, E. A., Brodszky, V., Baji, P., Závada, J., Petrova, G., Rotar, A., & Péntek, M. (2016). EQ-5D in Central and Eastern Europe: 2000–2015. Quality of Life Research, 25(11), 2693–2710.PubMed
19.
go back to reference Wang, A., Rand, K., Yang, Z., Brooks, R., & Busschbach, J. (2021). The remarkably frequent use of EQ-5D in non-economic research. European Journal of Health Economics, 23(6), 1007–1014. Wang, A., Rand, K., Yang, Z., Brooks, R., & Busschbach, J. (2021). The remarkably frequent use of EQ-5D in non-economic research. European Journal of Health Economics, 23(6), 1007–1014.
20.
go back to reference Janssen, B., & Szende, A. (2014). Population Norms for the EQ-5D. In A. Szende, B. Janssen, & J. Cabases (Eds.), Self-reported population health: An international perspective based on EQ-5D (pp. 19–30). Springer. Janssen, B., & Szende, A. (2014). Population Norms for the EQ-5D. In A. Szende, B. Janssen, & J. Cabases (Eds.), Self-reported population health: An international perspective based on EQ-5D (pp. 19–30). Springer.
21.
go back to reference Pickard, A. S., Jalundhwala, Y. J., Bewsher, H., Sharp, L. K., Walton, S. M., Schumock, G. T., & Caskey, R. N. (2018). Lifestyle-related attitudes: Do they explain self-rated health and life-satisfaction? Quality of Life Research, 27(5), 1227–1235.PubMed Pickard, A. S., Jalundhwala, Y. J., Bewsher, H., Sharp, L. K., Walton, S. M., Schumock, G. T., & Caskey, R. N. (2018). Lifestyle-related attitudes: Do they explain self-rated health and life-satisfaction? Quality of Life Research, 27(5), 1227–1235.PubMed
22.
go back to reference Chapman, B. P., Franks, P., Duberstein, P. R., & Jerant, A. (2009). Differences between individual and societal health state valuations: Any link with personality? Medical Care, 47(8), 902–907.PubMedPubMedCentral Chapman, B. P., Franks, P., Duberstein, P. R., & Jerant, A. (2009). Differences between individual and societal health state valuations: Any link with personality? Medical Care, 47(8), 902–907.PubMedPubMedCentral
23.
go back to reference Jerant, A., Chapman, B. P., & Franks, P. (2008). Personality and EQ-5D scores among individuals with chronic conditions. Quality of Life Research, 17(9), 1195–1204.PubMedPubMedCentral Jerant, A., Chapman, B. P., & Franks, P. (2008). Personality and EQ-5D scores among individuals with chronic conditions. Quality of Life Research, 17(9), 1195–1204.PubMedPubMedCentral
24.
go back to reference Whynes, D. K. (2008). Correspondence between EQ-5D health state classifications and EQ VAS scores. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 6, 94.PubMedPubMedCentral Whynes, D. K. (2008). Correspondence between EQ-5D health state classifications and EQ VAS scores. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 6, 94.PubMedPubMedCentral
25.
go back to reference Israelsson, J., Thylén, I., Strömberg, A., Bremer, A., & Årestedt, K. (2018). Factors associated with health-related quality of life among cardiac arrest survivors treated with an implantable cardioverter-defibrillator. Resuscitation, 132, 78–84.PubMed Israelsson, J., Thylén, I., Strömberg, A., Bremer, A., & Årestedt, K. (2018). Factors associated with health-related quality of life among cardiac arrest survivors treated with an implantable cardioverter-defibrillator. Resuscitation, 132, 78–84.PubMed
26.
go back to reference Lindeboom, M., & van Doorslaer, E. (2004). Cut-point shift and index shift in self-reported health. Journal of Health Economics, 23(6), 1083–1099.PubMed Lindeboom, M., & van Doorslaer, E. (2004). Cut-point shift and index shift in self-reported health. Journal of Health Economics, 23(6), 1083–1099.PubMed
27.
go back to reference Hays, R. D., Morales, L. S., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory and health outcomes measurement in the 21st century. Medical Care, 38(9 Suppl), II28–II42.PubMedPubMedCentral Hays, R. D., Morales, L. S., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory and health outcomes measurement in the 21st century. Medical Care, 38(9 Suppl), II28–II42.PubMedPubMedCentral
28.
go back to reference Hernández-Quevedo, C., Jones, A. M., & Rice, N. (2004). Reporting bias and heterogeneity in self-assessed health. Evidence from the British Household Panel Survey. Health, Econometrics and Data Group (HEDG) Working paper 05, 4. Hernández-Quevedo, C., Jones, A. M., & Rice, N. (2004). Reporting bias and heterogeneity in self-assessed health. Evidence from the British Household Panel Survey. Health, Econometrics and Data Group (HEDG) Working paper 05, 4.
29.
go back to reference Pfarr, C., Schmid, A., & Schneider, U. (2012). Reporting heterogeneity in self-assessed health among elderly Europeans. Health Economics Review, 2(1), 21.PubMedPubMedCentral Pfarr, C., Schmid, A., & Schneider, U. (2012). Reporting heterogeneity in self-assessed health among elderly Europeans. Health Economics Review, 2(1), 21.PubMedPubMedCentral
30.
go back to reference Schneider, U., Pfarr, C., Schneider, B. S., & Ulrich, V. (2012). I feel good! Gender differences and reporting heterogeneity in self-assessed health. European Journal of Health Economics, 13(3), 251–265. Schneider, U., Pfarr, C., Schneider, B. S., & Ulrich, V. (2012). I feel good! Gender differences and reporting heterogeneity in self-assessed health. European Journal of Health Economics, 13(3), 251–265.
31.
go back to reference Rencz, F., & Janssen, M. F. (2022). Analyzing the pain/discomfort and anxiety/depression composite domains and the meaning of discomfort in the EQ-5D: A mixed-methods study. Value Health, 25(12), 2003–2016.PubMed Rencz, F., & Janssen, M. F. (2022). Analyzing the pain/discomfort and anxiety/depression composite domains and the meaning of discomfort in the EQ-5D: A mixed-methods study. Value Health, 25(12), 2003–2016.PubMed
32.
go back to reference Rencz, F., Ruzsa, G., Bató, A., Yang, Z., Finch, A. P., & Brodszky, V. (2022). Value set for the EQ-5D-Y-3L in Hungary. Pharmacoeconomics, 40, 205–215.PubMedPubMedCentral Rencz, F., Ruzsa, G., Bató, A., Yang, Z., Finch, A. P., & Brodszky, V. (2022). Value set for the EQ-5D-Y-3L in Hungary. Pharmacoeconomics, 40, 205–215.PubMedPubMedCentral
33.
go back to reference Herdman, M., Gudex, C., Lloyd, A., Janssen, M., Kind, P., Parkin, D., Bonsel, G., & Badia, X. (2011). Development and preliminary testing of the new five-level version of EQ-5D (EQ-5D-5L). Quality of Life Research, 20(10), 1727–1736.PubMedPubMedCentral Herdman, M., Gudex, C., Lloyd, A., Janssen, M., Kind, P., Parkin, D., Bonsel, G., & Badia, X. (2011). Development and preliminary testing of the new five-level version of EQ-5D (EQ-5D-5L). Quality of Life Research, 20(10), 1727–1736.PubMedPubMedCentral
34.
go back to reference Rencz, F., Brodszky, V., Gulácsi, L., Golicki, D., Ruzsa, G., Pickard, A. S., Law, E. H., & Péntek, M. (2020). Parallel valuation of the EQ-5D-3L and EQ-5D-5L by time trade-off in Hungary. Value Health, 23(9), 1235–1245.PubMed Rencz, F., Brodszky, V., Gulácsi, L., Golicki, D., Ruzsa, G., Pickard, A. S., Law, E. H., & Péntek, M. (2020). Parallel valuation of the EQ-5D-3L and EQ-5D-5L by time trade-off in Hungary. Value Health, 23(9), 1235–1245.PubMed
35.
go back to reference Orosz, G., Dombi, E., Tóth-Király, I., & Roland-Lévy, C. (2017). The less is more: The 17-Item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. Current Psychology, 36(1), 39–47. Orosz, G., Dombi, E., Tóth-Király, I., & Roland-Lévy, C. (2017). The less is more: The 17-Item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. Current Psychology, 36(1), 39–47.
36.
go back to reference Williams, R. (2006). Generalized ordered logit/partial proportional odds models for ordinal dependent variables. Stata Journal, 6(1), 58–82. Williams, R. (2006). Generalized ordered logit/partial proportional odds models for ordinal dependent variables. Stata Journal, 6(1), 58–82.
37.
go back to reference Brant, R. (1990). Assessing proportionality in the proportional odds model for ordinal logistic regression. Biometrics, 46, 1171–1178.PubMed Brant, R. (1990). Assessing proportionality in the proportional odds model for ordinal logistic regression. Biometrics, 46, 1171–1178.PubMed
38.
go back to reference Breusch, T. S., & Pagan, A. R. (1980). The Lagrange multiplier test and its applications to model specification in econometrics. The Review of Economic Studies, 47(1), 239–253. Breusch, T. S., & Pagan, A. R. (1980). The Lagrange multiplier test and its applications to model specification in econometrics. The Review of Economic Studies, 47(1), 239–253.
39.
go back to reference Knott, R. J., Lorgelly, P. K., Black, N., & Hollingsworth, B. (2017). Differential item functioning in quality of life measurement: An analysis using anchoring vignettes. Social Science & Medicine, 190, 247–255. Knott, R. J., Lorgelly, P. K., Black, N., & Hollingsworth, B. (2017). Differential item functioning in quality of life measurement: An analysis using anchoring vignettes. Social Science & Medicine, 190, 247–255.
40.
go back to reference Feng, Y., Herdman, M., van Nooten, F., Cleeland, C., Parkin, D., Ikeda, S., Igarashi, A., & Devlin, N. J. (2017). An exploration of differences between Japan and two European countries in the self-reporting and valuation of pain and discomfort on the EQ-5D. Quality of Life Research, 26(8), 2067–2078.PubMedPubMedCentral Feng, Y., Herdman, M., van Nooten, F., Cleeland, C., Parkin, D., Ikeda, S., Igarashi, A., & Devlin, N. J. (2017). An exploration of differences between Japan and two European countries in the self-reporting and valuation of pain and discomfort on the EQ-5D. Quality of Life Research, 26(8), 2067–2078.PubMedPubMedCentral
41.
go back to reference Smith, A. B., Cocks, K., Parry, D., & Taylor, M. (2016). A differential item functioning analysis of the EQ-5D in cancer. Value Health, 19(8), 1063–1067.PubMed Smith, A. B., Cocks, K., Parry, D., & Taylor, M. (2016). A differential item functioning analysis of the EQ-5D in cancer. Value Health, 19(8), 1063–1067.PubMed
42.
go back to reference Salomon, J. A., Patel, A., Neal, B., Glasziou, P., Grobbee, D. E., Chalmers, J., & Clarke, P. M. (2011). Comparability of patient-reported health status: Multicountry analysis of EQ-5D responses in patients with type 2 diabetes. Medical Care, 49(10), 962–970.PubMed Salomon, J. A., Patel, A., Neal, B., Glasziou, P., Grobbee, D. E., Chalmers, J., & Clarke, P. M. (2011). Comparability of patient-reported health status: Multicountry analysis of EQ-5D responses in patients with type 2 diabetes. Medical Care, 49(10), 962–970.PubMed
43.
go back to reference Whynes, D. K., Sprigg, N., Selby, J., Berge, E., & Bath, P. M. (2013). Testing for differential item functioning within the EQ-5D. Medical Decision Making, 33(2), 252–260.PubMed Whynes, D. K., Sprigg, N., Selby, J., Berge, E., & Bath, P. M. (2013). Testing for differential item functioning within the EQ-5D. Medical Decision Making, 33(2), 252–260.PubMed
44.
go back to reference Stochl, J., Croudace, T., Perez, J., Birchwood, M., Lester, H., Marshall, M., Amos, T., Sharma, V., Fowler, D., & Jones, P. B. (2013). Usefulness of EQ-5D for evaluation of health-related quality of life in young adults with first-episode psychosis. Quality of Life Research, 22(5), 1055–1063.PubMed Stochl, J., Croudace, T., Perez, J., Birchwood, M., Lester, H., Marshall, M., Amos, T., Sharma, V., Fowler, D., & Jones, P. B. (2013). Usefulness of EQ-5D for evaluation of health-related quality of life in young adults with first-episode psychosis. Quality of Life Research, 22(5), 1055–1063.PubMed
45.
go back to reference Penton, H., Dayson, C., Hulme, C., & Young, T. (2022). An investigation of age-related differential item functioning in the EQ-5D-5L using item response theory and logistic regression. Value Health, 25(9), 1566–1574.PubMed Penton, H., Dayson, C., Hulme, C., & Young, T. (2022). An investigation of age-related differential item functioning in the EQ-5D-5L using item response theory and logistic regression. Value Health, 25(9), 1566–1574.PubMed
46.
go back to reference Prieto, L., Novick, D., Sacristán, J. A., Edgell, E. T., & Alonso, J. (2003). A Rasch model analysis to test the cross-cultural validity of the EuroQoL-5D in the Schizophrenia Outpatient Health Outcomes Study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. Supplementum, 416, 24–29. Prieto, L., Novick, D., Sacristán, J. A., Edgell, E. T., & Alonso, J. (2003). A Rasch model analysis to test the cross-cultural validity of the EuroQoL-5D in the Schizophrenia Outpatient Health Outcomes Study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. Supplementum, 416, 24–29.
47.
go back to reference Coretti, S., Ruggeri, M., & McNamee, P. (2014). The minimum clinically important difference for EQ-5D index: A critical review. Expert Review of Pharmacoeconomics & Outcomes Research, 14(2), 221–233. Coretti, S., Ruggeri, M., & McNamee, P. (2014). The minimum clinically important difference for EQ-5D index: A critical review. Expert Review of Pharmacoeconomics & Outcomes Research, 14(2), 221–233.
48.
go back to reference Hoehle, L. P., Phillips, K. M., Speth, M. M., Caradonna, D. S., Gray, S. T., & Sedaghat, A. R. (2019). Responsiveness and minimal clinically important difference for the EQ-5D in chronic rhinosinusitis. Rhinology, 57(2), 110–116.PubMed Hoehle, L. P., Phillips, K. M., Speth, M. M., Caradonna, D. S., Gray, S. T., & Sedaghat, A. R. (2019). Responsiveness and minimal clinically important difference for the EQ-5D in chronic rhinosinusitis. Rhinology, 57(2), 110–116.PubMed
49.
go back to reference McClure, N. S., Sayah, F. A., Ohinmaa, A., & Johnson, J. A. (2018). Minimally important difference of the EQ-5D-5L Index Score in adults with type 2 diabetes. Value Health, 21(9), 1090–1097.PubMed McClure, N. S., Sayah, F. A., Ohinmaa, A., & Johnson, J. A. (2018). Minimally important difference of the EQ-5D-5L Index Score in adults with type 2 diabetes. Value Health, 21(9), 1090–1097.PubMed
50.
go back to reference McClure, N. S., Sayah, F. A., Xie, F., Luo, N., & Johnson, J. A. (2017). Instrument-defined estimates of the minimally important difference for EQ-5D-5L Index Scores. Value Health, 20(4), 644–650.PubMed McClure, N. S., Sayah, F. A., Xie, F., Luo, N., & Johnson, J. A. (2017). Instrument-defined estimates of the minimally important difference for EQ-5D-5L Index Scores. Value Health, 20(4), 644–650.PubMed
51.
go back to reference Pickard, A. S., Neary, M. P., & Cella, D. (2007). Estimation of minimally important differences in EQ-5D utility and VAS scores in cancer. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 5, 70.PubMedPubMedCentral Pickard, A. S., Neary, M. P., & Cella, D. (2007). Estimation of minimally important differences in EQ-5D utility and VAS scores in cancer. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 5, 70.PubMedPubMedCentral
52.
go back to reference Shiroiwa, T., Fukuda, T., Ikeda, S., Igarashi, A., Noto, S., & Shimozuma, K. (2016). Japanese population norms for preference-based measures: EQ-5D-3L, EQ-5D-5L, and SF-6D. Quality of Life Research, 25(3), 707–719.PubMed Shiroiwa, T., Fukuda, T., Ikeda, S., Igarashi, A., Noto, S., & Shimozuma, K. (2016). Japanese population norms for preference-based measures: EQ-5D-3L, EQ-5D-5L, and SF-6D. Quality of Life Research, 25(3), 707–719.PubMed
53.
go back to reference Nolan, C. M., Longworth, L., Lord, J., Canavan, J. L., Jones, S. E., Kon, S. S., & Man, W. D. (2016). The EQ-5D-5L health status questionnaire in COPD: Validity, responsiveness and minimum important difference. Thorax, 71(6), 493–500.PubMed Nolan, C. M., Longworth, L., Lord, J., Canavan, J. L., Jones, S. E., Kon, S. S., & Man, W. D. (2016). The EQ-5D-5L health status questionnaire in COPD: Validity, responsiveness and minimum important difference. Thorax, 71(6), 493–500.PubMed
54.
go back to reference Henry, E. B., Barry, L. E., Hobbins, A. P., McClure, N. S., & O’Neill, C. (2020). Estimation of an instrument-defined minimally important difference in EQ-5D-5L index scores based on scoring algorithms derived using the EQ-VT version 2 valuation protocols. Value Health, 23(7), 936–944.PubMed Henry, E. B., Barry, L. E., Hobbins, A. P., McClure, N. S., & O’Neill, C. (2020). Estimation of an instrument-defined minimally important difference in EQ-5D-5L index scores based on scoring algorithms derived using the EQ-VT version 2 valuation protocols. Value Health, 23(7), 936–944.PubMed
55.
go back to reference Chen, P., Lin, K. C., Liing, R. J., Wu, C. Y., Chen, C. L., & Chang, K. C. (2016). Validity, responsiveness, and minimal clinically important difference of EQ-5D-5L in stroke patients undergoing rehabilitation. Quality of Life Research, 25(6), 1585–1596.PubMed Chen, P., Lin, K. C., Liing, R. J., Wu, C. Y., Chen, C. L., & Chang, K. C. (2016). Validity, responsiveness, and minimal clinically important difference of EQ-5D-5L in stroke patients undergoing rehabilitation. Quality of Life Research, 25(6), 1585–1596.PubMed
56.
go back to reference Baker, M., Stabile, M., & Deri, C. (2004). What do self-reported, objective, measures of health measure? The Journal of Human Resources, 39(4), 1067–1093. Baker, M., Stabile, M., & Deri, C. (2004). What do self-reported, objective, measures of health measure? The Journal of Human Resources, 39(4), 1067–1093.
57.
go back to reference Bago d’Uva, T., Van Doorslaer, E., Lindeboom, M., & O’Donnell, O. (2008). Does reporting heterogeneity bias the measurement of health disparities? Health Economics, 17(3), 351–375.PubMed Bago d’Uva, T., Van Doorslaer, E., Lindeboom, M., & O’Donnell, O. (2008). Does reporting heterogeneity bias the measurement of health disparities? Health Economics, 17(3), 351–375.PubMed
58.
go back to reference Au, N., & Lorgelly, P. K. (2014). Anchoring vignettes for health comparisons: An analysis of response consistency. Quality of Life Research, 23(6), 1721–1731.PubMed Au, N., & Lorgelly, P. K. (2014). Anchoring vignettes for health comparisons: An analysis of response consistency. Quality of Life Research, 23(6), 1721–1731.PubMed
59.
go back to reference Grol-Prokopczyk, H., Freese, J., & Hauser, R. M. (2011). Using anchoring vignettes to assess group differences in general self-rated health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 52(2), 246–261.PubMedPubMedCentral Grol-Prokopczyk, H., Freese, J., & Hauser, R. M. (2011). Using anchoring vignettes to assess group differences in general self-rated health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 52(2), 246–261.PubMedPubMedCentral
60.
go back to reference Salomon, J. A., Tandon, A., & Murray, C. J. (2004). Comparability of self rated health: Cross sectional multi-country survey using anchoring vignettes. BMJ, 328(7434), 258.PubMedPubMedCentral Salomon, J. A., Tandon, A., & Murray, C. J. (2004). Comparability of self rated health: Cross sectional multi-country survey using anchoring vignettes. BMJ, 328(7434), 258.PubMedPubMedCentral
61.
go back to reference Melzer, D., Lan, T. Y., Tom, B. D., Deeg, D. J., & Guralnik, J. M. (2004). Variation in thresholds for reporting mobility disability between national population subgroups and studies. Journal of Gerontology A Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 59(12), 1295–1303. Melzer, D., Lan, T. Y., Tom, B. D., Deeg, D. J., & Guralnik, J. M. (2004). Variation in thresholds for reporting mobility disability between national population subgroups and studies. Journal of Gerontology A Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 59(12), 1295–1303.
62.
go back to reference Weisscher, N., Glas, C. A., Vermeulen, M., & De Haan, R. J. (2010). The use of an item response theory-based disability item bank across diseases: Accounting for differential item functioning. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 63(5), 543–549.PubMed Weisscher, N., Glas, C. A., Vermeulen, M., & De Haan, R. J. (2010). The use of an item response theory-based disability item bank across diseases: Accounting for differential item functioning. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 63(5), 543–549.PubMed
63.
go back to reference Henson, J. M., Carey, M. P., Carey, K. B., & Maisto, S. A. (2006). Associations among health behaviors and time perspective in young adults: Model testing with boot-strapping replication. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29(2), 127–137.PubMedPubMedCentral Henson, J. M., Carey, M. P., Carey, K. B., & Maisto, S. A. (2006). Associations among health behaviors and time perspective in young adults: Model testing with boot-strapping replication. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29(2), 127–137.PubMedPubMedCentral
64.
go back to reference Guthrie, L. C., Butler, S. C., Lessl, K., Ochi, O., & Ward, M. M. (2014). Time perspective and exercise, obesity, and smoking: Moderation of associations by age. American Journal of Health Promotion, 29(1), 9–16.PubMed Guthrie, L. C., Butler, S. C., Lessl, K., Ochi, O., & Ward, M. M. (2014). Time perspective and exercise, obesity, and smoking: Moderation of associations by age. American Journal of Health Promotion, 29(1), 9–16.PubMed
65.
go back to reference Griva, F., Anagnostopoulos, F., & Potamianos, G. (2013). Time perspective and perceived risk as related to mammography screening. Women Health, 53(8), 761–776.PubMed Griva, F., Anagnostopoulos, F., & Potamianos, G. (2013). Time perspective and perceived risk as related to mammography screening. Women Health, 53(8), 761–776.PubMed
66.
go back to reference Sword, R. M., Sword, R. K. M., Brunskill, S. R., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2014). Time perspective therapy: A new time-based metaphor therapy for PTSD. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 19(3), 197–201. Sword, R. M., Sword, R. K. M., Brunskill, S. R., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2014). Time perspective therapy: A new time-based metaphor therapy for PTSD. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 19(3), 197–201.
67.
go back to reference Perry, J. L., Temple, E. C., Worrell, F. C., Zivkovic, U., Mello, Z. R., Musil, B., Cole, J. C., & Mckay, M. T. (2020). Different version, similar result? A critical analysis of the multiplicity of shortened versions of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. SAGE Open, 10(2), 2158244020923351. Perry, J. L., Temple, E. C., Worrell, F. C., Zivkovic, U., Mello, Z. R., Musil, B., Cole, J. C., & Mckay, M. T. (2020). Different version, similar result? A critical analysis of the multiplicity of shortened versions of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. SAGE Open, 10(2), 2158244020923351.
68.
go back to reference Worrell, F. C., Temple, E. C., McKay, M. T., Živkovič, U., Perry, J. L., Mello, Z. R., Musil, B., & Cole, J. C. (2018). A theoretical approach to resolving the psychometric problems associated with the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory: Results from the USA, Australia, Slovenia, and the United Kingdom. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 34(1), 41–51. Worrell, F. C., Temple, E. C., McKay, M. T., Živkovič, U., Perry, J. L., Mello, Z. R., Musil, B., & Cole, J. C. (2018). A theoretical approach to resolving the psychometric problems associated with the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory: Results from the USA, Australia, Slovenia, and the United Kingdom. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 34(1), 41–51.
69.
go back to reference Shipp, A. J., Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2009). Conceptualization and measurement of temporal focus: The subjective experience of the past, present, and future. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110(1), 1–22. Shipp, A. J., Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2009). Conceptualization and measurement of temporal focus: The subjective experience of the past, present, and future. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110(1), 1–22.
Metagegevens
Titel
Time perspective profile and self-reported health on the EQ-5D
Auteurs
Fanni Rencz
Mathieu F. Janssen
Publicatiedatum
08-09-2023
Uitgeverij
Springer International Publishing
Gepubliceerd in
Quality of Life Research / Uitgave 1/2024
Print ISSN: 0962-9343
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2649
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-023-03509-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2024

Quality of Life Research 1/2024 Naar de uitgave