Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness 6/2020

Open Access 07-04-2020 | ORIGINAL PAPER

The Process of Science: A Critical Evaluation of more than 15 Years of Research on Self-Compassion with the Self-Compassion Scale

Auteurs: Peter Muris, Henry Otgaar

Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness | Uitgave 6/2020

Abstract

Objectives

Falsification is regarded as a cornerstone of science. Anomalous data—even when highly credible—do not always lead to an adjustment of theory. In this paper, we discuss reasons for why current theories are not modified despite a corpus of work falsifying (parts of) the theory, using the case of self-compassion as an example. During more than 15 years, this psychological construct has been heralded as a protective factor against stress and emotional adversity.

Methods

A search in the Web of Science database using [SELF-COMPASSION in title] as the search term was conducted and found empirical papers were critically evaluated.

Results

Good evidence abounds indicating that the theoretical definition of self-compassion is limited and that as a consequence its proper assessment with the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS) is obscured as the measure is contaminated by psychopathological characteristics. Surprisingly, few researchers take these critical notes into account when conducting and reporting new studies on this topic. We argue that this might be due to all kinds of (conscious and unconscious) tendencies and cognitive biases of researchers and clinicians.

Conclusion

Research lines are not always guided by solid data but strongly determined by social factors. We close by providing a recommendation for researchers regarding the assessment of self-compassion including the continued use of the SCS.
Opmerkingen

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Science refers to a process that builds and structures knowledge in the form of testable explanations and predictions. In psychology, the comprehension of (human) behavior and mental processes is the main spearpoint of scientific inquiry, and in the case of clinical psychology, the main theme of research is psychopathology, its phenomenology, associated etiological mechanisms, and their amplification or abridgment through interventions. Science is highly dependent on the definition and operationalization of the constructs under investigation, and this is true for psychology and clinical psychology in particular, which are dominated by concepts such as emotion and cognition that have been conceptualized in a variety of ways (e.g., Barrett 2017; Bayne et al. 2019). The multiform definition of constructs in this branch of science may give rise to debate and controversy, which oftentimes—after substantial empirical inquiry—results in a coexistence of diverse perspectives (e.g., Toomela 2019). However, occasionally, it happens that even in spite of convincing evidence, researchers maintain a certain perspective that is plain wrong and no longer in keeping with the main premises of their theory.
According to Popper (1963), science reflects a data-driven process that commences with a theoretical framework on the basis of which testable hypotheses can be formulated. With appropriate measurement instruments, one can assess the relevant constructs and examine the validity of a hypothesis, thereby falsifying the theory. As long as the hypothesis is confirmed, the theory is supported and can be considered as valid. However, if the hypothesis is rejected, the theory can no longer be viewed as valid and hence needs to be adjusted or even discarded. By this view, science reflects a logical, rational process that is fully driven by acquired empirical data. Oftentimes, science indeed operates according to Popperian principles. Within the field of clinical psychology, examples can be found of theories that after appropriate scientific inquiry had to be refuted or corrected. A case in point has been the hyperventilation theory of panic, which assumed that the typical physiological arousal symptoms (e.g., palpitations, breathlessness, chest pain, dizziness) as seen in patients with panic disorder are produced by overbreathing producing a metabolic state of respiratory alkalosis (e.g., Ley 1985). Experimental studies, however, showed that not the hyperventilation-induced physical symptoms per se but the catastrophic interpretations of such symptoms (e.g., palpitations interpreted as a sign of an impending heart attack) are the vehicle behind the development of panic attacks (Hornsveld 1996; Salkovskis and Clark 1990). This work on the interpretation of such symptoms has made researchers abandon the hyperventilation theory of panic.
Meanwhile, it should be noted that science does not always progress in a logical and rational way, and oftentimes, new evidence that does not accord well with the original data is disputed and rejected, leading to a consolidation of a perhaps objectively non-valid theory. This process of hampering scientific progress is well described by Kuhn (1962) in The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Kuhn noted that empirical findings that are in disagreement with a current theory (so-called anomalies) do not immediately lead to a change in theory but rather could result in a period of “crisis” during which new methods and approaches of inquiry are permitted. These new scientific endeavors could then result in a “paradigm shift”, a significant adjustment or even complete replacement of the original theory. Such shifts do not take place readily and automatically; they are obstructed by social factors such as scientists’ interest in publishing (preferably high-impact) papers, various types of cognitive biases such as confirmation bias which can lead researchers to continue to defend and refuse to let go of a theory in spite of non-fitting new evidence. If this unfolds, a research program might enter a degenerative phase which could ideally lead to an abandonment of a certain theory (Lakatos and Musgrave 1974).
Scholars have repeatedly noted that the choice of observation and choice of attention are key to science, which means that researchers can be quite selective in what comes into their scientific lens and investigation (e.g., McComas 1996; Schwartz et al. 2004). Sometimes, however, there is undeniable proof that a theory is wrong or that the way a theory is currently investigated is seriously flawed. In our view, the latter is truly the case with the research program of self-compassion, a concept that was introduced more than 15 years ago as part of the positive psychology wave in clinical psychology. This research program has fueled the development of self-compassion related interventions as a viable branch of third-generation cognitive-behavioral therapies (Wilson et al. 2019). Like many other researchers, we became interested in self-compassion as a possible protective factor against the development of mental health problems and conducted a number of empirical studies on the topic. From the beginning of our research efforts, we were critical about the proper assessment of this construct by means of the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS) (Neff 2003a). We noted that half of its items were fused with symptoms of psychopathology, and so we decided to remove them from the scale. However, during the peer review of our first paper (Muris et al. 2016a), we started to discover that a critical view on self-compassion and its scale was embraced with a skeptic eye. This increased our skeptical scientific attitude regarding the SCS: we did not understand why researchers did not see the obvious point that we were making regarding this measure, and hence, we began to share our critical thoughts with the scientific community (Muris 2016; Muris and Petrocchi 2017; Muris et al. 2016b, 2019b). However, the developer of the SCS (Neff 2016a, b, 2019) maintained that the scale in its original form provides a good index of self-compassion.
In the present article, we do not want to reiterate the entirety of the arguments that have been brought forward in these publications but rather provide a description of the main controversy and use this as an illustration for the process (and lack of progress) of science. We are specifically focusing on personal, cognitive, and social mechanisms at work preventing the correction and adjustment of theoretical notions and applied assessment instruments. Following this, we highlight new advancements in the field of self-compassion and come with clear recommendations regarding the assessment of this possibly protective construct.

Neff’s Theory and Assessment of Self-Compassion

A healthy attitude to oneself is considered to be one of the linchpins of resilience and the preservation of mental health (Baumeister and Vohs 2004). For a long time, the research on the role of self-related characteristics in people’s psychological functioning was predominantly focused on self-esteem, which refers to a person’s subjective evaluation of his/her own worth (Rosenberg 1965). Although research has shown that high self-esteem may buffer against mental health problems while low levels of self-esteem might increase the risk for the development of such difficulties, it has also been noted that self-esteem is quite resistant to change (e.g., Josephs et al. 2003) and thus seems to be a less suitable target for intervention. Around the beginning of this century, Western psychology started to exhibit interest in self-compassion, an alternative positive self-related concept originating from Buddhist culture. Pioneering empirical work was conducted by Neff (2003b) who defined self-compassion as “being touched by and open to one’s own suffering, not avoiding or disconnecting from it, generating the desire to alleviate one’s suffering and to heal oneself with kindness. [It] also involves offering nonjudgmental understanding to one’s pain, inadequacies and failures, so that one’s experience is seen as part of the larger human experience” (p. 87). In a more recent elaboration of this definition, Neff conceptualized self-compassion as a “balance between increased compassionate and decreased uncompassionate self-responding to personal struggle” (Neff et al. 2018b, p. 371), which involves three key elements on bipolar ends: (1) being kind and supportive to oneself rather than harsh and judgmental, (2) recognizing that such difficulties constitute a normal part of humans’ life rather than feeling isolated from other people as a result of one’s imperfection, and (3) keeping the personal suffering in rational awareness rather than becoming fully absorbed by one’s problems (see Neff 2003a, b).
In line with this conceptualization, Neff (2003a) developed the SCS, a 26-item self-report questionnaire for measuring individual differences in “the three main components of self-compassion on separate subscales (self-kindness versus self-judgment, common humanity versus isolation, and mindfulness versus over-identification) with the intention of summing the subscale scores to create a total score that would represent a participants’ overall level of self-compassion” (p. 226). Table 1 shows examples of SCS items on the three dimensions, each contrasting compassionate (positive) and uncompassionate (negative) ways of self-responding. A shortened version of the scale (the SCS-SF) has also been created (Raes et al. 2011), which includes the 12 best loading items, 4 for each dimension with again an equal number for compassionate and uncompassionate self-directed responses.
Table 1
Item examples for each of the three self-compassion dimensions of Neff’s (2003a) Self-Compassion Scale (SCS)
SCS dimensions
Compassionate self-responding
Uncompassionate self-responding
1. Self-kindnessa versus self-judgmentb
aI’m kind to myself when I’m experiencing suffering
bWhen I see aspects of myself that I do not like, I get down on myself
2. Common humanityc versus isolationd
cWhen I feel inadequate in some way, I try to remind myself that feelings of inadequacy are shared by most people
dWhen I fail at something that’s important to me I tend to feel alone in my failure
3. Mindfulnesse versus over-identificationf
eWhen something upsets me, I try to keep my emotions in balance
fWhen something painful happens, I tend to blow the incident out of proportion
Solid evidence exists for the basic psychometric properties of the SCS. The initial evaluation of the test by Neff (2003a) showed that the reliability of the scale was good, and this was true for both the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha was .92 for the total score and ranged between .75 and .81 for various subscales) and test-retest stability (test-retest correlations over a 3-week period were .93 for the total score and between .80 and .88 for the subscales). Furthermore, support was found for the validity of the scale. Specifically, the total SCS score was positively related to scores on other positive self-related traits (i.e., self-esteem, self-acceptance, self-determination) while it was negatively associated with symptom levels of anxiety and depression, which is of course in line with the hypothesized protective qualities of self-compassion. In addition, it was found that Buddhists—who practice a type of meditation enhancing mindfulness and compassion—displayed, as expected, a higher SCS score than a comparison group of undergraduate students.
The only aberrant finding in the Neff (2003a) study was that no support could be documented for the dimensional nature of the three self-compassion components. Instead, confirmatory factor analysis showed that for each component, a two-factor solution provided a better fit for the data than the one-factor solution, which made Neff conclude that six separate but correlated factors in the SCS existed. In her view, these factors jointly constitute the overarching construct of self-compassion, which as such still justifies the employment of the total score.
The SCS has become highly popular in the academic field. The SCS was translated and validated in at least 17 countries. In most cases, the psychometric qualities of the scale have been reported to be just as favorable as those of the original English version. This is also true for the SCS-SF: the shortened scale is reliable and has a near-perfect correlation with the original 26-item scale (Raes et al. 2011) suggesting that the use of this economic measure is likely to produce the same results as obtained with its full-length counterpart.

Flourishing Research on Self-Compassion

Since its introduction in the scientific literature, the construct of self-compassion has garnered considerable attention of the research community. A search of the literature conducted on December 31, 2019 in the Web of Science database using [SELF-COMPASSION in title] as the search term yielded 927 publications, of which 597 were empirical studies. Most of these investigations (n = 571, 95.6%) used the SCS or the SCS-SF (which became only available in 2011) to measure self-compassion, showing that these are the dominant instruments in the field. Alternative scales such as the Fear of Compassion for the Self (FCS) (Gilbert et al. 2011), Self-Other Four Immeasurables (SOFI) (Kraus and Sears 2009), and the Self-Compassion and Self-Criticism Scales (SCCS) (Falconer et al. 2015) are available but are less frequently used.
As can be seen in the top panel of Fig. 1, the number of publications has continuously increased over the years, and this also counts for the number of citations which is growing exponentially. The full-length version of the SCS is still most popular, although the percentage of studies employing the SCS-SF is steadily increasing (from 8.3% in 2011 to 31.9% in 2019). Most researchers (72.0%) use the total score of the SCS or SCS-SF to obtain an overall indicator of self-compassion, and only in a minority of studies (28.0%) other scores are derived from these measures, such as scores on individual subscales or combined positive and negative subscales, and this modus operandi does not seem to change over time (bottom panel of Fig. 1). The trend to employ the total score is more prominent with the SCS-SF (89.8%) than with the SCS (65.1%), which is not surprising given the limited number of items included in the form which precludes the measurement of reliable factor/subscale scores.
The attraction of self-compassion lies in the fact that this construct may be relevant for understanding people’s adjustment to life adversity and personal problems (Neff 2003b). It is clearly advocated as a protective mechanism, as we read in a recent writing by Neff and Germer (2017): “Self-compassion is a powerful way to enhance intrapersonal and interpersonal well-being. When we are mindful of our suffering and respond to it with kindness, remembering that suffering is part of the shared human condition, it appears that we are able to better cope with life’s struggles” (p.382). In other words, it is assumed that self-compassion promotes psychological resilience by enabling the person to use more adaptive coping and emotion regulation strategies (Inwood and Ferrari 2018), and this could help the individual to maintain healthy psychological functioning and shield against the development of mental health problems. Many studies have focused on the protective function of self-compassion, and although most of them are cross-sectional (correlational) in nature (but see Donald et al. 2018; Kirschner et al. 2019), this research has generally indicated that this trait is positively related to indices reflecting personal well-being (e.g., happiness, life satisfaction; Zessin et al. 2015) and negatively associated with measures tapping symptoms of psychopathology (e.g., anxiety, depression, stress; MacBeth and Gumley 2012). Furthermore, while most studies have concentrated on the relevance of self-compassion for people’s adaptation within a clinical psychology context, the trait is also increasingly investigated as a positive psychological characteristic in stressful work-related (e.g., burn-out, motivation, procrastination), sports, and medical (e.g., HIV, chronic pain, cancer) settings (see for an overview: https://​self-compassion.​org/​).
An additional appealing feature of self-compassion is that it also has much potential for therapy and interventions: unlike self-esteem, this trait appears to be amenable to change (Germer and Neff 2019). Specific treatments have been developed with the purpose to bolster compassionate self-responding (Kirby et al. 2017; Wilson et al. 2019), and indeed, evidence has been obtained that suggests that these interventions not only successfully increase self-compassion but also promote personal well-being and reduce psychopathology. Not surprisingly, many clinicians are enthusiastic and view self-compassion as a potent vehicle along which psychological functioning can be improved and human suffering can be eliminated.

Critique on the SCS

At first sight, the research on self-compassion is in good shape: a vast amount of evidence seems to indicate that this trait acts as a protective individual difference variable that seems to have more explanatory power than other positive psychology constructs (e.g., self-esteem, e.g., Neff and Vonk 2009; mindfulness, e.g., Van Dam et al. 2011) and also provides a lead for (improving) treatment in clinical settings. A weak point, however, is that the main body of knowledge is solely based on the SCS and that questions have been raised regarding the validity of this instrument. We argue that this criticism is far from trivial and seriously undermines the scientific foundation of the self-compassion concept.
The main point of critique regarding the SCS concerns the inclusion of items referring to ways of uncompassionate self-responding. These items measuring self-judgment, isolation, and over-identification were initially included in the scale as reversed items of the three key components of self-kindness, common humanity, and mindfulness. However, as is often the case with reversed items, they can form separate factors and unintentionally introduce new dimensions and/or unwanted method variance in a measure (e.g., Wong et al. 2003). A recent study has indeed shown that this also applies to the SCS (Montero-Marin et al. 2018). The inclusion of the uncompassionate self-responding items is especially problematic because they are clearly in contrast with the protective nature of the self-compassion construct. Face validity checks performed on SCS items have revealed that whereas the compassionate self-responding components are regarded as aspects of positive cognitive coping and healthy psychological functioning, the uncompassionate self-responding components are mainly seen as indicators of vulnerability, psychopathological symptoms, and mental illness (Muris et al. 2018; see also Fig. 2). This result has also been confirmed by empirical data showing that while the compassionate SCS components of self-kindness, common humanity, and mindfulness are connected to adaptive personality features (e.g., optimism), positive mood states (e.g., happiness), and aspects of well-being (e.g., quality of life), their uncompassionate counterparts of self-judgment, isolation, and over-identification are more clearly linked to negative affect and symptoms of anxiety and depression (e.g., Neff et al. 2018a). Thus, the SCS contains multiple dimensions that are differentially related to external constructs (see also Brenner et al. 2017, 2018; Lopez et al. 2015, 2016; Muris et al. 2018, 2019a). It is clear that this has important consequences when researchers use and only report the total score of such a measure as one cannot know the nature of the different dimensions’ contributions to that score as well as their unique predictive value for external variables (Smith et al. 2009).
Within the context of psychopathology, the inclusion of uncompassionate self-responding items in the SCS total score is questionable for another and perhaps more important reason. Several authors have argued that the components of self-judgment, isolation, and over-identification reflect a number of toxic processes that can be commonly observed in a wide range of mental health problems, especially those of an internalizing nature. Specifically, self-judgment shows clear similarity with harsh self-criticism and self-punishment, isolation shares features with social withdrawal and loneliness, whereas over-identification matches with self-absorption and self-focused rumination (Körner et al. 2015; Lopez et al. 2018; Muris 2016). Whether the uncompassionate self-responding components of the SCS reflect some underlying vulnerability factor (e.g., neuroticism; Pfattheicher et al. 2017) or directly reflect symptoms of psychopathology (Muris et al. 2016b) is still a matter of debate, but the fact is that there is (too) little awareness in the research on self-compassion that this type of “measurement confounding” (see, e.g., Lemery et al. 2002) occurs, in particular when using the measure as a “predictor” of mental health problems. As noted earlier, the vast majority of researchers in the field of self-compassion appears to use the total SCS score (which includes the reversed uncompassionate self-responding items) and without further consideration treat this as a valid indicator of a protective trait.

A Scientific Smoke Curtain to Defend the Use of the SCS Total Score

The most important argument put forward to defend the use of the SCS total score is based on the factor analytic finding that there seems to be a common overarching factor that serves as an umbrella for the six compassionate and uncompassionate components included in the measure (Neff 2016b, 2019). An exemplary study was conducted by Neff et al. (2019) who used sophisticated statistical methodology to explore the factor structure of the SCS using the data of 11,685 participants from 20 diverse samples. The analysis revealed that a single bi-factor model, in which each SCS item not only loaded on its corresponding factor but also on an overarching factor, provided the best fit for the data, thereby justifying the use of the total score (see also Cleare et al. 2018; Neff et al. 2018b; Toth-Kiraly et al. 2017).
However, there has been more research investigating the factor structure of the SCS, and the outcomes have been mixed: quite a number of studies obtained support for a correlated six-factor model (Benda and Reichova 2016; Castilho et al. 2015; Cunha et al. 2016; Garcia-Campayo et al. 2014; Hupfeld and Ruffieux 2011; Kotsou and Leys 2016; Kumlander et al. 2018; Petrocchi et al. 2014; Pfattheicher et al. 2017; Ursic et al. 2019) while there are also other investigations pointing in the direction of a solution with two overarching factors representing compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding (Brenner et al. 2017; Coroiu et al. 2018; Costa et al. 2016; Halamova et al. 2018; Hayes et al. 2016; Zhang et al. 2019). Taken together, it can be concluded that the exact factor structure of the SCS is far from clear and tends to differ across studies. It is important to note though that all studies have in common that a simple one-factor model does never provide the best fit for the SCS. The structure of the scale is complex, and the compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding components need to be split while at the same time allowing them in some way to share variance (Williams et al. 2014).
Examination of the factor structure of a scale is certainly important as it enables us to determine whether the instrument adequately captures the various components that constitute a theoretical construct. However, it is important to note that this type of analysis only covers one aspect of validity, namely construct validity, and that it should not be used as a scientific smoke curtain to obscure the fact that there are other problems with a measure. The divergent correlations between the compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding components of the SCS and external measures, and the confounding of the uncompassionate components with vulnerability and psychopathology, simply cannot be ignored.

Implications of Using the SCS Total Score for Research on Self-Compassion

We signal at least four problems for the scientific inquiry of self-compassion with this sole focus on the SCS total score. First of all, the true protective value of self-compassion is obscured. On the basis of meta-analytic evidence, Muris and Petrocchi (2017) concluded that the uncompassionate self-responding components of the SCS are more strongly related to mental health problems than the compassionate self-responding components, implying that when using the total score, the link with psychopathology will be inflated. Direct support for this idea is still sparse, but in a recent paper, we demonstrated that when statistically correcting for uncompassionate self-responding, the contribution of compassionate self-responding to symptoms of anxiety, depression, stress, and related constructs was significantly reduced, explaining only a marginal proportion in the variance of these psychopathology indicators (Muris et al. 2019b; see Fig. 3).
A second problem is that the inclusion of the uncompassionate self-responding components in the SCS total score will hinder researchers from investigating the precise role of self-compassion in the multifactorial origins of psychopathology and from making a fair comparison with other predictor variables. For example, in one of our own studies (Muris et al. 2016a), we examined the value of self-compassion as a protective variable against symptoms of anxiety and depression as compared to other positive self-related factors such as self-esteem and self-efficacy. We used a SCS version from which the uncompassionate self-responding items had been discarded; if we had not done so, this would probably have resulted in the finding that self-compassion had incremental validity in predicting psychopathological symptoms over the other positive factors (cf. Neff and Vonk 2009), which now did not turn out to be the case (i.e., self-esteem and self-efficacy—and not self-compassion—were the variables found to have unique explanatory power). Thus, the employment of the SCS total score will obscure to what extent a protective trait like self-compassion really stands the competition with other etiological factors of mental health problems.
A third problem with the employment of the total score is that it may lead researchers to delve into issues that are perhaps trivial. A good example is the investigation of gender differences in self-compassion. A meta-analysis by Yarnell et al. (2015) has indicated that—when considering the SCS total score—women are in general less self-compassionate than men, on the basis of which it has been recommended to explore in future studies whether proper training may help women foster more compassion toward the self and make them more resilient to the development of mental health problems. In a subsequent study, Yarnell et al. (2019) examined whether the gender difference in self-compassion can be explained by differences in gender role orientation. The results were quite complex, but the overall picture suggests that “masculinity was the most consistent positive predictor of self-compassion” (p.1147, meaning that in both men and women, participants with higher levels of masculine traits displayed higher total SCS scores) while the role of femininity was less clear. To explain the intricate findings, Yarnell et al. (2019) point at the multiplistic nature of gender roles and the way these are assessed, but they forget to note that self-compassion as measured with the SCS also has multiple faces. Studies that did explore gender differences for SCS subscales indicated that women only scored higher on self-judgment, isolation, and over-identification (e.g., Bluth and Blanton 2015), implying that their lower total self-compassion scores do not truly reflect lower levels of compassionate self-responding but are mainly due to their higher endorsement of uncompassionate self-responding items. The latter is obviously less groundbreaking as it is already well known in the literature that women generally score higher on negative, internalizing psychopathology-related features than men (e.g., Kramer et al. 2008).
A fourth and final problem is that the use of the SCS total score will conceal the precise and unique role of self-compassion during psychological treatment. As noted earlier, an important assumption is that self-compassion is a modifiable characteristic, and in the past years, various types of interventions have been developed to examine whether these might reduce psychopathology (Kirby et al. 2017; Wilson et al. 2019). However, as rightly noted by Kirby and Gilbert (2019), by lumping together compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding into one score, it will remain unclear what is actually happening during therapy. Obviously, the abolishment of uncompassionate self-responding is of less interest: given its commonality with psychopathology, this seems to merely reflect symptom reduction, which is of course an important target of almost any therapeutic intervention. It seems more crucial and interesting to focus on the change in self-compassionate responding and to study whether this represents the mechanism responsible for the observed treatment effects (Wadsworth et al. 2018).
Altogether, it is inevitable to conclude that, especially within a context of mental health problems and stress, the inclusion of the uncompassionate self-responding components in the SCS is problematic. And although we and several other scholars (Brenner et al. 2018; Kirby and Gilbert 2019; Lopez et al. 2018) have repeatedly emphasized this issue, most researchers appear to show little appreciation for these critical notes and continue to use the SCS total score including the (reversely scored) uncompassionate self-responding items as the preferred index of self-compassion.

The Process of Science

Why do the critical points raised regarding the SCS and the empirical data collected to substantiate this criticism not lead to an adjustment in the theory of self-compassion and more specifically an altered employment of the scale in research? Or in other words: why is science apparently not operating as a Popperian process? The short answer is that this is because science is conducted by human beings who do not always operate in a logical, rational way but rather are driven by personal interests, cognitive biases, and social influences.
First of all, it is not uncommon that scientists continue to defend a theory against contrasting evidence and arguments. A famous example was Charles Darwin, a brilliant researcher who early in his career as a geologist was puzzled by the “parallel roads” of Glen Roy, three perfectly horizontal terraces along the mountainsides in the Lochaber area of the Scottish Highlands (Rudwick 1974). Darwin advanced the theory that the “parallel roads” were raised beaches of marine origin, and although he could not find any evidence in support of this account (e.g., fossils of marine animals and plants), he vigorously defended it, in spite of the fact that a more plausible theory had been formulated stating that the terraces in the landscape were cut by the waxing and waning shores of a glacier lake during the ice age. Only short before his death, Darwin recognized that he had it all wrong and admitted that his account of the “parallel roads” of Glen Roy was “one long gigantic blunder”.
Interestingly, Chinn and Brewer (1993, 1998, 2001) have described a framework describing how researchers deal with data that are not in keeping or even in contrast with their theory. In this framework, eight possible ways are specified of how scientists respond to such anomalous data. The most common responses reflect defensive maneuvers such as ignoring, rejection, exclusion, abeyance, and reinterpretation, all of which have the net effect that the initial theory remains unchanged. There are a number of reasons why scientists choose to preserve a theory in spite of anomalous data (Chinn and Brewer 1993). The first reason has to do with a person’s beliefs with regard to the theory. If a theoretical belief is deeply entrenched and associated with a strong personal commitment, it will be challenging to change this notion. Many researchers in the self-compassion research field are scientist-practitioners who operate in science because they are invested in developing diagnostic tools and treatment methods with the ultimate goal of helping patients more effectively in clinical practice. However, researchers with a scientist-practitioner focus often show less concern regarding validity checks and other psychometric issues, although this putatively is a threat to science (Lilienfeld et al. 2015). Thus, researchers tend to “blindly” employ a measure simply because it is continuously advocated as the ideally suited index for measuring a construct not only by a leading researcher (authority bias—the tendency to attribute greater value to the opinion of an authority figure; Milgram 1963) but also by many coworkers in this research field (groupthink or bandwagon effect—the tendency to do or believe things because many other people do or believe the same; Mailoo 2015).
Another ground for why aberrant data tend to be neglected is concerned with the lack of a solid alternative theory. Meanwhile, it is important to note that Gilbert, who is another well-respected researcher in the self-compassion field, has formulated an alternative theoretical account that might be able to effectively deal with the main problem of the SCS. This account, known as the theory of social mentalities (Gilbert 2000, 2005), proposes that there are three separate brain-based systems guiding people’s social behavior of which two are of particular importance for the current discussion: (1) the (parasympathetic) safeness system that elicits thoughts, feelings, and actions promoting positive relationships with others and the self, which is thought to be involved in compassionate self-responding, and (2) a (sympathetic) threat-defense system that prompts thoughts, feelings, and actions that mainly serve to reduce threat, which is related to ways of uncompassionate self-responding. Thus, there is a solid alternative theory (but see Khoury 2019) proposing that compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding indeed reflect different processes that are moderated by different brain systems (for further reading, see Klimecki and Singer 2017), which—as noted earlier—implies that it is not appropriate to combine them in a single score of self-compassion.
A further reason for theory-preserving responses to anomalous data relates to the credibility of the anomalous data. In the past years, multiple researchers have put effort in demonstrating that compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding components included in the SCS are dissimilar and do not represent a single protective trait (e.g., Brenner et al. 2017, 2018; Lopez et al. 2015; Muris et al. 2018, 2019a). That is, various types of methods were used to substantiate critiques regarding the scale (i.e., meta-analysis, face validity checks, empirical research), but so far, this has not led to a notable change in the way the SCS has been employed. Of course, we do not want to call into question the autonomy and self-governance of researchers, but the fact is that scientists, like all other people, are prone to various kinds of biases. For instance, the resistance to modification is likely to be guided by cognitive biases such as anchoring (i.e., relying too heavily on the first piece of information on a subject; Epley and Gilovich 2006), congruence bias (i.e., the tendency to test a hypothesis exclusively in one particular way instead of testing possible alternative hypotheses; Iverson et al. 2008), and confirmation bias (i.e., the tendency to search for, interpret, focus on, and remember information in a way that confirms one’s preconceptions; Oswald and Grosjean 2004). In this light, it is also important to note that the SCS has been the cornerstone of the self-compassion literature, and so, an acknowledgement of the questionable validity of the scale would lead to a re-analysis and reinterpretation of most of the collected data (with an unknown, probably less advantageous outcome for the self-compassion construct), which of course feeds cognitive distortions such as conservatism (i.e., the tendency to revise one’s belief insufficiently when presented with new evidence; Iverson et al. 2008), the Ostrich effect (i.e., ignoring an obvious negative situation; Karlsson et al. 2009), and status quo bias (i.e., the tendency to like things to stay the same; Nebel 2015).
Most of the abovementioned cognitive biases operate at an automatic level, which means that scientists make these types of information processing errors without being (fully) aware of them. However, scientists are also human in the sense that they consciously behave in a way that is not in line with ethical regulations. Science can be a hypercompetitive activity requiring researchers to publish a good quantity of papers in high-impact journals and to acquire research grants, thereby creating a climate that may tempt them to cut corners, exaggerate findings, and overstate the importance of their research (see Tijdink et al. 2016). In the case of self-compassion, it is more profitable to employ the SCS total score and treat the construct as a pure protective factor rather than to present a more nuanced (although more honest) picture including the role of compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding. Obviously, the second option carries the risk that results will show that, in the context of psychopathology, the uncompassionate ways of self-responding have more predictive power than their compassionate counterparts, a finding that has less scientific news value. There are examples of researchers in the self-compassion field who in spite of their apparent awareness of the potential problem with the SCS (as they previously published a paper displaying separate results for the compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding components) completely neglect this point when writing their next research report. This comes close to a phenomenon that has been labeled as “cherry-picking” (Murphy and Aguinis 2019).
Peer review is another element in science that may be associated with various types of biases serving to preserve an existing theoretical account and hinder the assimilation of new points of view (Haffar et al. 2019). As noted in the introductory section of this paper, reviewers are also prone to processes that distort their judgment when evaluating the empirical papers of researchers (who are often colleagues and scientific peers). Here too, anchoring, confirmation bias, and conservatism are at work in conjunction with more specific distortions such as the availability cascade (i.e., a notion that is increasingly repeated in the scientific literature will also be considered as more plausible; Kuran and Sunstein 1999). In the case of the SCS, the law-of-the-instrument effect may also be operating, which refers to an over-reliance on a familiar tool and its prescribed scoring method (Kaplan 1964). Journal editors have a decisive role in the peer review process but are of course susceptible to the same set of biases as the reviewers. They should be aware of these distortions, try to make objective judgments, and make the effort to encourage critical scientific dialog, which will ultimately lead to an advancement in knowledge.

New Insights in Self-Compassion

It is important to note that Neff’s definition of self-compassion—on the basis of which she developed the SCS—was based on her own observations and Buddhist readings. There is nothing wrong to adopt such an approach, and her conceptualization has proven to be powerful enough to attract attention from other researchers and clinicians. In the meantime, theoretical notions on compassion in general and self-compassion in specific have considerably advanced during the past years (Seppälä et al. 2018). After an extensive review of the literature on the various ways that (self-)compassion has been conceptualized, Strauss et al. (2016) came to the conclusion that the construct essentially consists of five elements: (1) recognizing suffering, (2) understanding the universality of human suffering, (3) feeling for the person suffering, (4) tolerating uncomfortable feelings, and (5) acting or motivation to act to alleviate suffering. Acknowledging the fact that most existing measures do not comprehensively measure self-compassion, these researchers developed a new questionnaire, the Sussex-Oxford Compassion Scales (SOCS), of which the 20-item self-related version measures self-compassion. In a first psychometric test (Gu et al. 2020), the scale was demonstrated to possess good reliability and validity. In support of its concurrent validity, scores on the new measure were found to correlate positively with the total score of the short form of the SCS: the SOCS subscales referring to feeling for the person suffering, tolerating uncomfortable feelings, and acting or motivation to act to alleviate suffering showed the most substantial correlations (>.50) with the SCS, indicating that these aspects are reasonably well represented by Neff’s operationalization of self-compassion.
A post hoc analysis performed on these data investigating the unique relations between compassionate and uncompassionate self-responding components of the SCS and various SOCS subscales revealed that compassionate self-responding was a significantly better predictor of various SOCS self-compassion elements than uncompassionate self-responding (Fig. 4). This suggests once more that if one wants to assess the true nature of self-compassion by means of the SCS, it is preferable to rely on the compassionate self-responding components. A similar conclusion was reached by Montero-Marin et al. (2018) who adopted a multitrait-multimethod analytical procedure analyzing the SCS data from 4120 participants in 11 samples with different cultural backgrounds. Their analysis indicated that “the positively valenced [compassionate self-responding] items, compared with the negative [uncompassionate self-responding] ones (which suffered more from method effects), were better explained by the corresponding trait factors of self-compassion” (p. 10), which led them to propose that the assessment of self-compassion with the SCS can best be confined to the compassionate items.

Concluding Remarks

Science involves the observation, identification, description, experimental investigation, and theoretical explanation of phenomena occurring in the real world. Via publications in peer-reviewed journals, scientists continuously inform the world about new insights and advancements they have made. According to Popper (1963), if the new data presented in a paper are in line with a theory, that theory is confirmed and consolidated. However, when the data are not in agreement with a theory, that should result in either a modification or rejection of the theory. In Popper’s view, science operates as an objective rational process, but the reality is that this is certainly not always the case. Science is a socially driven enterprise conducted by human beings who—especially when dealing with personally relevant topics—are prone to all kinds of biases. As a result, progress in science is not occurring readily and automatically. It really takes time for a research field to let go of a firmly established theory because of non-fitting evidence.
We have taken the research field of self-compassion as an illustration of the true process of science. It is apparent that there is irrefutable proof that the current conceptualization of self-compassion and the way this trait is currently assessed with the SCS are inappropriate and not in keeping with the true nature of this positive psychology construct (see also Khoury 2019). Nonetheless, the researchers in this domain do not seem ready for what Kuhn (1962) called a “paradigm shift”. Should it occur, such a shift would imply a theoretical change as well as a change in the measurement of self-compassion. The work by Strauss et al. (2016) has led to a more comprehensive description of the construct, on the basis of which a new scale (the SOCS; Gu et al. 2020) has been developed. This measure reliably assesses various aspects of self-compassion, the relevance of which can be further investigated in future studies. In the case of a continued use of the SCS, the solution is obvious: researchers should only use the compassionate self-responding components of the scale in case they are interested in studying the protective nature of the construct or consistently make a distinction between compassionate and uncompassionate ways of self-responding to study their unique and interactive effects within the context of psychopathology, thereby treating them for what they really are: separate concepts of protection and vulnerability.

Acknowledgements

The authors kindly thank Dr. Clara Strauss and Dr. Jenny Gu (University of Sussex) for conducting the post hoc analysis on the relation between the Sussex-Oxford Compassion Scale and the Self-Compassion Scale.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures were in accordance with ethical standards. The research protocol was approved by the Ethical Review Committee Psychology and Neuroscience at Maastricht University, The Netherlands.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
go back to reference Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: the secret life of the brain. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: the secret life of the brain. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Handbook of self-regulation. Research, theory, and applications. New York: The Guilford press. Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Handbook of self-regulation. Research, theory, and applications. New York: The Guilford press.
go back to reference Bayne, T., Brainard, D., Byrne, R. W., Chittka, L., Clayton, N., Heyes, C., Mather, J., Ölveczky, B., Shadlen, M., Suddendorf, T., & Webb, B. (2019). What is cognition? Current Biology, 29, R608–R615.PubMed Bayne, T., Brainard, D., Byrne, R. W., Chittka, L., Clayton, N., Heyes, C., Mather, J., Ölveczky, B., Shadlen, M., Suddendorf, T., & Webb, B. (2019). What is cognition? Current Biology, 29, R608–R615.PubMed
go back to reference Benda, J., & Reichova, A. (2016). Psychometricke charakteristiky Ceske verze Self-Compassion Scale (SCS-CZ). Ceskoslovenska Psychologie, 60, 120–136. Benda, J., & Reichova, A. (2016). Psychometricke charakteristiky Ceske verze Self-Compassion Scale (SCS-CZ). Ceskoslovenska Psychologie, 60, 120–136.
go back to reference Bluth, K., & Blanton, P. W. (2015). The influence of self-compassion on emotional well-being among early and older adolescent males and females. Journal of Positive Psychology, 10, 219–230. Bluth, K., & Blanton, P. W. (2015). The influence of self-compassion on emotional well-being among early and older adolescent males and females. Journal of Positive Psychology, 10, 219–230.
go back to reference Brenner, R. E., Heath, P. J., Vogel, D. L., & Crede, M. (2017). Two is more valid than one: examining the factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64, 696–707.PubMed Brenner, R. E., Heath, P. J., Vogel, D. L., & Crede, M. (2017). Two is more valid than one: examining the factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64, 696–707.PubMed
go back to reference Brenner, R. E., Vogel, D. L., Lannin, D. G., Engel, K. E., Seidman, A. J., & Heath, P. J. (2018). Do self-compassion and self-coldness distinctly relate to distress and well-being? A theoretical model of self-relating. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 65, 346–357.PubMed Brenner, R. E., Vogel, D. L., Lannin, D. G., Engel, K. E., Seidman, A. J., & Heath, P. J. (2018). Do self-compassion and self-coldness distinctly relate to distress and well-being? A theoretical model of self-relating. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 65, 346–357.PubMed
go back to reference Castilho, P., Pinto-Gouveia, J., & Duarte, J. (2015). Evaluating the multifactor structure of the long and short versions of the Self-Compassion Scale in a clinical sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71, 856–870.PubMed Castilho, P., Pinto-Gouveia, J., & Duarte, J. (2015). Evaluating the multifactor structure of the long and short versions of the Self-Compassion Scale in a clinical sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71, 856–870.PubMed
go back to reference Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: a theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63, 1–49. Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: a theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63, 1–49.
go back to reference Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1998). An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous data in science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35, 623–654. Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1998). An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous data in science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35, 623–654.
go back to reference Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (2001). Models of data: a theory of how people evaluate data. Cognition and Instruction, 19, 323–393. Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (2001). Models of data: a theory of how people evaluate data. Cognition and Instruction, 19, 323–393.
go back to reference Cleare, S., Gumley, A., Cleare, C. J., & O’Connor, R. C. (2018). An investigation of the factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 9, 618–628.PubMed Cleare, S., Gumley, A., Cleare, C. J., & O’Connor, R. C. (2018). An investigation of the factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 9, 618–628.PubMed
go back to reference Coroiu, A., Kwakkenbos, L., Moran, C., Thombs, B., Albani, C., Bourkas, S., Zenger, M., Brahler, E., & Körner, A. (2018). Structural validation of the Self-Compassion Scale with a German general population sample. PLoS One, 13, e0190771.PubMedPubMedCentral Coroiu, A., Kwakkenbos, L., Moran, C., Thombs, B., Albani, C., Bourkas, S., Zenger, M., Brahler, E., & Körner, A. (2018). Structural validation of the Self-Compassion Scale with a German general population sample. PLoS One, 13, e0190771.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Corsius, N. (2018). Further exploration of the face validity of the Self-Compassion Scale (unpublished master’s thesis). Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands. Corsius, N. (2018). Further exploration of the face validity of the Self-Compassion Scale (unpublished master’s thesis). Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
go back to reference Costa, J., Maroco, Pinto-Gouveia, J., Ferreira, C., & Castilho, P. (2016). Validation of the psychometric properties of the Self-Compassion Scale. Testing the factorial validity and factorial invariance of the measure among borderline personality disorder, anxiety disorder, eating disorder, and general populations. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 23, 460–468.PubMed Costa, J., Maroco, Pinto-Gouveia, J., Ferreira, C., & Castilho, P. (2016). Validation of the psychometric properties of the Self-Compassion Scale. Testing the factorial validity and factorial invariance of the measure among borderline personality disorder, anxiety disorder, eating disorder, and general populations. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 23, 460–468.PubMed
go back to reference Cunha, M., Xavier, A., & Castilho, P. (2016). Understanding self-compassion in adolescents: validation study of the Self-Compassion Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 56–62. Cunha, M., Xavier, A., & Castilho, P. (2016). Understanding self-compassion in adolescents: validation study of the Self-Compassion Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 56–62.
go back to reference Donald, J. N., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P. D., Sahdra, B. K., Marshall, S. L., & Guo, J. (2018). A worthy self is a caring self: examining the developmental relations between self-esteem and self-compassion in adolescents. Journal of Personality, 86, 619–630.PubMed Donald, J. N., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P. D., Sahdra, B. K., Marshall, S. L., & Guo, J. (2018). A worthy self is a caring self: examining the developmental relations between self-esteem and self-compassion in adolescents. Journal of Personality, 86, 619–630.PubMed
go back to reference Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic. Psychological Science, 17, 311–318.PubMed Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic. Psychological Science, 17, 311–318.PubMed
go back to reference Falconer, C. J., King, J. A., & Brewin, C. R. (2015). Demonstrating mood repair with a situation-based measure of self-compassion and self-criticism. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory Research and Practice, 88, 351–365. Falconer, C. J., King, J. A., & Brewin, C. R. (2015). Demonstrating mood repair with a situation-based measure of self-compassion and self-criticism. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory Research and Practice, 88, 351–365.
go back to reference Garcia-Campayo, J., Navarro-Gil, M., Andres, E., Montero-Marin, J., Lopez-Artal, L., & Piva Demarzo, M. M. (2014). Validation of the Spanish versions of the long (26 items) and short (12 items) forms of the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS). Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 12, 4.PubMedPubMedCentral Garcia-Campayo, J., Navarro-Gil, M., Andres, E., Montero-Marin, J., Lopez-Artal, L., & Piva Demarzo, M. M. (2014). Validation of the Spanish versions of the long (26 items) and short (12 items) forms of the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS). Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 12, 4.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Germer, C., & Neff, K. D. (2017). Teaching the mindful self-compassion program. A guide for professionals. New York: The Guilford Press. Germer, C., & Neff, K. D. (2017). Teaching the mindful self-compassion program. A guide for professionals. New York: The Guilford Press.
go back to reference Gilbert, P. (2000). Social mentalities: internal ‘social’ conflict and the role of inner warmth and compassion in cognitive therapy. In P. Gilbert & K. G. Bailey (Eds.), Genes on the couch: exploration in evolutionary psychotherapy (pp. 118–150). Hove: Psychology Press. Gilbert, P. (2000). Social mentalities: internal ‘social’ conflict and the role of inner warmth and compassion in cognitive therapy. In P. Gilbert & K. G. Bailey (Eds.), Genes on the couch: exploration in evolutionary psychotherapy (pp. 118–150). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Gilbert, P. (2005). Compassion: conceptualizations, research, and use in psychotherapy. London: Routledge. Gilbert, P. (2005). Compassion: conceptualizations, research, and use in psychotherapy. London: Routledge.
go back to reference Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Matos, M., & Rivis, A. (2011). Fears of compassion: development of three self-report scales. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory Research and Practice, 84, 239–255. Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Matos, M., & Rivis, A. (2011). Fears of compassion: development of three self-report scales. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory Research and Practice, 84, 239–255.
go back to reference Gu, J., Baer, R., Cavanagh, K., Kuyken, W., & Strauss, C. (2020). Development and psychometric properties of the Sussex-Oxford Compassion Scales (SOCS). Assessment, 27, 3–20.PubMed Gu, J., Baer, R., Cavanagh, K., Kuyken, W., & Strauss, C. (2020). Development and psychometric properties of the Sussex-Oxford Compassion Scales (SOCS). Assessment, 27, 3–20.PubMed
go back to reference Haffar, S., Bazerbachi, F., & Murad, M. H. (2019). Peer review bias: a critical review. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 4, 670–676. Haffar, S., Bazerbachi, F., & Murad, M. H. (2019). Peer review bias: a critical review. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 4, 670–676.
go back to reference Halamova, J., Kanovsky, M., & Pacuchova, M. (2018). Self-Compassion Scale: IRT psychometric analysis, validation, and factor structure—Slovak translation. Psychologica Belgica, 57, 190–209.PubMedPubMedCentral Halamova, J., Kanovsky, M., & Pacuchova, M. (2018). Self-Compassion Scale: IRT psychometric analysis, validation, and factor structure—Slovak translation. Psychologica Belgica, 57, 190–209.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hayes, J. A., Lockard, A. J., Janis, R. A., & Locke, B. D. (2016). Construct validity of the Self-Compassion Scale-Short Form among psychotherapy clients. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 29, 405–422. Hayes, J. A., Lockard, A. J., Janis, R. A., & Locke, B. D. (2016). Construct validity of the Self-Compassion Scale-Short Form among psychotherapy clients. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 29, 405–422.
go back to reference Hornsveld, H. (1996). Farewell to the hyperventilation syndrome. Doctoral thesis, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Hornsveld, H. (1996). Farewell to the hyperventilation syndrome. Doctoral thesis, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
go back to reference Hupfeld, J., & Ruffieux, N. (2011). Validierung einer Deutschen version der Self-Compassion Scale (SCS-D). Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 40, 115–123. Hupfeld, J., & Ruffieux, N. (2011). Validierung einer Deutschen version der Self-Compassion Scale (SCS-D). Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 40, 115–123.
go back to reference Inwood, E., & Ferrari, M. (2018). Mechanisms of change in the relationship between self-compassion, emotion regulation, and mental health. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 10, 215–235. Inwood, E., & Ferrari, M. (2018). Mechanisms of change in the relationship between self-compassion, emotion regulation, and mental health. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 10, 215–235.
go back to reference Iverson, G. L., Brooks, B. L., & Holdnack, J. A. (2008). Misdiagnosis of cognitive impairment in forensic neuropsychology. In R. L. Heilbronner (Ed.), Neuropsychology in the courtroom: expert analysis of reports and testimony (pp. 243–266). New York: Guilford Press. Iverson, G. L., Brooks, B. L., & Holdnack, J. A. (2008). Misdiagnosis of cognitive impairment in forensic neuropsychology. In R. L. Heilbronner (Ed.), Neuropsychology in the courtroom: expert analysis of reports and testimony (pp. 243–266). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Josephs, R. A., Bosson, J. K., & Jacobs, C. G. (2003). Self-esteem maintenance processes: why low self-esteem may be resistant to change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 920–933.PubMed Josephs, R. A., Bosson, J. K., & Jacobs, C. G. (2003). Self-esteem maintenance processes: why low self-esteem may be resistant to change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 920–933.PubMed
go back to reference Kaplan, A. (1964). The conduct of inquiry: methodology for behavioral science. San Francisco: Chandler Publishing. Kaplan, A. (1964). The conduct of inquiry: methodology for behavioral science. San Francisco: Chandler Publishing.
go back to reference Karlsson, M., Loewenstein, G., & Seppi, D. (2009). The ostrich effect: selective attention to information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38, 95–115. Karlsson, M., Loewenstein, G., & Seppi, D. (2009). The ostrich effect: selective attention to information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38, 95–115.
go back to reference Khoury, B. (2019). Compassion: embodied and embedded. Mindfulness, 10, 2363–2374. Khoury, B. (2019). Compassion: embodied and embedded. Mindfulness, 10, 2363–2374.
go back to reference Kirby, J. N., & Gilbert, P. (2019). Commentary regarding Wilson et al. (2018) “Effectiveness of ‘self-compassion’ related therapies: a systematic review and meta-analysis”. All is not as it seems. Mindfulness, 10, 1006–1016. Kirby, J. N., & Gilbert, P. (2019). Commentary regarding Wilson et al. (2018) “Effectiveness of ‘self-compassion’ related therapies: a systematic review and meta-analysis”. All is not as it seems. Mindfulness, 10, 1006–1016.
go back to reference Kirby, J. N., Tellegen, C. L., & Steindl, S. R. (2017). A meta-analysis of compassion-based interventions: current state of knowledge and future directions. Behavior Therapy, 48, 778–792.PubMed Kirby, J. N., Tellegen, C. L., & Steindl, S. R. (2017). A meta-analysis of compassion-based interventions: current state of knowledge and future directions. Behavior Therapy, 48, 778–792.PubMed
go back to reference Kirschner, H., Kuyken, W., Wright, K., Roberts, H., Brejcha, C., & Karl, A. (2019). Soothing your heart and feeling connected: a new experimental paradigm to study the benefits of self-compassion. Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 545–565.PubMedPubMedCentral Kirschner, H., Kuyken, W., Wright, K., Roberts, H., Brejcha, C., & Karl, A. (2019). Soothing your heart and feeling connected: a new experimental paradigm to study the benefits of self-compassion. Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 545–565.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Klimecki, O. M., & Singer, T. (2017). The compassionate brain. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compassion science (pp. 109–120). New York: Oxford University Press. Klimecki, O. M., & Singer, T. (2017). The compassionate brain. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compassion science (pp. 109–120). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Körner, A., Coroiu, A., Copeland, L., Gomez-Garibello, C., Albani, C., Zenger, M., & Brähler, E. (2015). The role of self-compassion in buffering symptoms of depression in the general population. PLoS One, 10, e0136598.PubMedPubMedCentral Körner, A., Coroiu, A., Copeland, L., Gomez-Garibello, C., Albani, C., Zenger, M., & Brähler, E. (2015). The role of self-compassion in buffering symptoms of depression in the general population. PLoS One, 10, e0136598.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Kotsou, I., & Leys, C. (2016). Self-Compassion Scale (SCS): psychometric properties of the French translation and its relations with psychological well-being, affect, and depression. PLoS One, 11, e0152880.PubMedPubMedCentral Kotsou, I., & Leys, C. (2016). Self-Compassion Scale (SCS): psychometric properties of the French translation and its relations with psychological well-being, affect, and depression. PLoS One, 11, e0152880.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Kramer, M. D., Krueger, R. F., & Hicks, B. M. (2008). The role of internalizing and externalizing liability factors in accounting for gender differences in the prevalence of common psychopathological syndromes. Psychological Medicine, 38, 51–61.PubMed Kramer, M. D., Krueger, R. F., & Hicks, B. M. (2008). The role of internalizing and externalizing liability factors in accounting for gender differences in the prevalence of common psychopathological syndromes. Psychological Medicine, 38, 51–61.PubMed
go back to reference Kraus, S., & Sears, S. (2009). Measuring the immeasurables: development and initial validation of the Self-Other Four Immeasurables (SOFI) scale based on Buddhist teachings on loving kindness, compassion, joy, and equanimity. Social Indicators Research, 92, 169–181. Kraus, S., & Sears, S. (2009). Measuring the immeasurables: development and initial validation of the Self-Other Four Immeasurables (SOFI) scale based on Buddhist teachings on loving kindness, compassion, joy, and equanimity. Social Indicators Research, 92, 169–181.
go back to reference Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Kumlander, S., Lahtinen, O., Turunen, T., & Salmivalli, C. (2018). Two is more valid than one, but is six even better? The factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS). PLoS One, 13, e0207706.PubMedPubMedCentral Kumlander, S., Lahtinen, O., Turunen, T., & Salmivalli, C. (2018). Two is more valid than one, but is six even better? The factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS). PLoS One, 13, e0207706.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Kuran, T., & Sunstein, C. R. (1999). Availability cascades and risk regulation. Stanford Law Review, 51, 683–768. Kuran, T., & Sunstein, C. R. (1999). Availability cascades and risk regulation. Stanford Law Review, 51, 683–768.
go back to reference Lakatos, I., & Musgrave, A. (1974). Criticism and the growth of knowledge. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lakatos, I., & Musgrave, A. (1974). Criticism and the growth of knowledge. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Lemery, K. S., Essex, M. J., & Smider, N. A. (2002). Revealing the relation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms by eliminating measurement confounding: expert ratings and factor analyses. Child Development, 73, 867–882.PubMed Lemery, K. S., Essex, M. J., & Smider, N. A. (2002). Revealing the relation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms by eliminating measurement confounding: expert ratings and factor analyses. Child Development, 73, 867–882.PubMed
go back to reference Ley, R. (1985). Blood, breath, and fears: a hyperventilation theory of panic attacks and agoraphobia. Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 271–285. Ley, R. (1985). Blood, breath, and fears: a hyperventilation theory of panic attacks and agoraphobia. Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 271–285.
go back to reference Lilienfeld, S. O., Ritschel, L. A., Lynn, S. J., Cautin, R. L., & Latzman, R. D. (2015). Science-practice gap. In R. L. Cautin & S. O. Lilienfeld (Eds.), The encyclopedia of clinical psychology (pp. 2548–2555). Hoboken: Wiley and Sons. Lilienfeld, S. O., Ritschel, L. A., Lynn, S. J., Cautin, R. L., & Latzman, R. D. (2015). Science-practice gap. In R. L. Cautin & S. O. Lilienfeld (Eds.), The encyclopedia of clinical psychology (pp. 2548–2555). Hoboken: Wiley and Sons.
go back to reference Lopez, A., Sanderman, R., Smink, A., Zhang, Y., Van Sonderen, E., Ranchor, A., & Schroevers, M. J. (2015). A reconsideration of the Self-Compassion Scale’s total score: self-compassion versus self-criticism. PLoS One, 10, e0132940.PubMedPubMedCentral Lopez, A., Sanderman, R., Smink, A., Zhang, Y., Van Sonderen, E., Ranchor, A., & Schroevers, M. J. (2015). A reconsideration of the Self-Compassion Scale’s total score: self-compassion versus self-criticism. PLoS One, 10, e0132940.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lopez, A., Sanderman, R., & Schroevers, M. J. (2016). Mindfulness and self-compassion as unique and common predictors of affect in the general population. Mindfulness, 7, 1289–1296.PubMedPubMedCentral Lopez, A., Sanderman, R., & Schroevers, M. J. (2016). Mindfulness and self-compassion as unique and common predictors of affect in the general population. Mindfulness, 7, 1289–1296.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lopez, A., Sanderman, R., & Schroevers, M. J. (2018). A close examination of the relationship between self-compassion and depressive symptoms. Mindfulness, 9, 1470–1478.PubMedPubMedCentral Lopez, A., Sanderman, R., & Schroevers, M. J. (2018). A close examination of the relationship between self-compassion and depressive symptoms. Mindfulness, 9, 1470–1478.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference MacBeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: a meta-analysis on the association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 545–552.PubMed MacBeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: a meta-analysis on the association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 545–552.PubMed
go back to reference Mailoo, V. (2015). Common sense or cognitive bias and groupthink: does it belong in our clinical reasoning? British Journal of General Practice, 65, 27. Mailoo, V. (2015). Common sense or cognitive bias and groupthink: does it belong in our clinical reasoning? British Journal of General Practice, 65, 27.
go back to reference McComas, W. F. (1996). Ten myths of science: reexamining what we think about the nature of science. School Science and Mathematics, 96, 10–16. McComas, W. F. (1996). Ten myths of science: reexamining what we think about the nature of science. School Science and Mathematics, 96, 10–16.
go back to reference Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378. Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.
go back to reference Montero-Marin, J., Kuyken, W., Crane, C., Gu, J., Baer, R., Al-Awamleh, A. A., Akutsu, S., Araya-Veliz, C., Ghorbani, N., Chen, Z. J., Kim, M. S., Mantzios, M., Rolim dos Santos, D. N., Serramo Lopez, L. C., Teleb, A. A., Watson, P. J., Yamaguchi, A., Yang, E., & Garcia-Campayo, J. (2018). Self-compassion and cultural values: a cross-cultural study of self-compassion using a multitrait-multimethod (MTMM) analytical procedure. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2638.PubMedPubMedCentral Montero-Marin, J., Kuyken, W., Crane, C., Gu, J., Baer, R., Al-Awamleh, A. A., Akutsu, S., Araya-Veliz, C., Ghorbani, N., Chen, Z. J., Kim, M. S., Mantzios, M., Rolim dos Santos, D. N., Serramo Lopez, L. C., Teleb, A. A., Watson, P. J., Yamaguchi, A., Yang, E., & Garcia-Campayo, J. (2018). Self-compassion and cultural values: a cross-cultural study of self-compassion using a multitrait-multimethod (MTMM) analytical procedure. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2638.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Muris, P. (2016). A protective factor against mental health problems in youths? A critical note on the assessment of self-compassion. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 1461–1465.PubMed Muris, P. (2016). A protective factor against mental health problems in youths? A critical note on the assessment of self-compassion. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 1461–1465.PubMed
go back to reference Muris, P., & Petrocchi, N. (2017). Protection or vulnerability? A meta-analysis of the relations between the positive and negative components of self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 24, 373–383.PubMed Muris, P., & Petrocchi, N. (2017). Protection or vulnerability? A meta-analysis of the relations between the positive and negative components of self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 24, 373–383.PubMed
go back to reference Muris, P., Meesters, C., Pierik, A., & De Kock, B. (2016a). Good for the self: self-compassion and other self-related constructs in relation to symptoms of anxiety and depression in non-clinical youths. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 607–617.PubMed Muris, P., Meesters, C., Pierik, A., & De Kock, B. (2016a). Good for the self: self-compassion and other self-related constructs in relation to symptoms of anxiety and depression in non-clinical youths. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 607–617.PubMed
go back to reference Muris, P., Otgaar, H., & Petrocchi, N. (2016b). Protection as the mirror image of psychopathology: further critical notes on the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 7, 787–790. Muris, P., Otgaar, H., & Petrocchi, N. (2016b). Protection as the mirror image of psychopathology: further critical notes on the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 7, 787–790.
go back to reference Muris, P., Van den Broek, M., Otgaar, H., Oudenhoven, I., & Lennartz, J. (2018). Good and bad sides of self-compassion: a face validity check of the Self-Compassion Scale and an investigation of its relations to coping and emotional symptoms in non-clinical adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27, 2411–2421.PubMedPubMedCentral Muris, P., Van den Broek, M., Otgaar, H., Oudenhoven, I., & Lennartz, J. (2018). Good and bad sides of self-compassion: a face validity check of the Self-Compassion Scale and an investigation of its relations to coping and emotional symptoms in non-clinical adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27, 2411–2421.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Muris, P., Otgaar, H., Meesters, C., Heutz, A., & Van den Hombergh, M. (2019a). Self-compassion and adolescents’ positive and negative reactions to daily life problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28, 1433–1444. Muris, P., Otgaar, H., Meesters, C., Heutz, A., & Van den Hombergh, M. (2019a). Self-compassion and adolescents’ positive and negative reactions to daily life problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28, 1433–1444.
go back to reference Muris, P., Otgaar, H., & Pfattheicher, S. (2019b). Stripping the forest from the rotten trees: compassionate self-responding is a way of coping, but reduced uncompassionate self-responding mainly reflects psychopathology. Mindfulness, 10, 196–199. Muris, P., Otgaar, H., & Pfattheicher, S. (2019b). Stripping the forest from the rotten trees: compassionate self-responding is a way of coping, but reduced uncompassionate self-responding mainly reflects psychopathology. Mindfulness, 10, 196–199.
go back to reference Murphy, K. R., & Aguinis, H. (2019). HARKing: how badly can cherry-picking and question trolling produce bias in published results? Journal of Business and Psychology, 34, 1–17. Murphy, K. R., & Aguinis, H. (2019). HARKing: how badly can cherry-picking and question trolling produce bias in published results? Journal of Business and Psychology, 34, 1–17.
go back to reference Nebel, J. M. (2015). Status quo bias, rationality, and conservatism about value. Ethics, 125, 449–476. Nebel, J. M. (2015). Status quo bias, rationality, and conservatism about value. Ethics, 125, 449–476.
go back to reference Neff, K. D. (2003a). Development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223–250. Neff, K. D. (2003a). Development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223–250.
go back to reference Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: an alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85–101. Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: an alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85–101.
go back to reference Neff, K. D. (2016a). Does self-compassion entail reduced self-judgment, isolation, and over-identification? A response to Muris, Otgaar, and Petrocchi (2016). Mindfulness, 7, 791–797. Neff, K. D. (2016a). Does self-compassion entail reduced self-judgment, isolation, and over-identification? A response to Muris, Otgaar, and Petrocchi (2016). Mindfulness, 7, 791–797.
go back to reference Neff, K. D. (2016b). The Self-Compassion Scale is a valid and theoretically coherent measure of self-compassion. Mindfulness, 7, 264–274. Neff, K. D. (2016b). The Self-Compassion Scale is a valid and theoretically coherent measure of self-compassion. Mindfulness, 7, 264–274.
go back to reference Neff, K. D. (2019). Setting the record straight about the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 10, 200–202. Neff, K. D. (2019). Setting the record straight about the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 10, 200–202.
go back to reference Neff, K. D., & Germer, C. (2017). Self-compassion and psychological well-being. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compassion science (pp. 371–385). New York: Oxford University Press. Neff, K. D., & Germer, C. (2017). Self-compassion and psychological well-being. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compassion science (pp. 371–385). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Neff, K. D., & Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus global self-worth: two different ways of relating to oneself. Journal of Personality, 77, 23–50.PubMed Neff, K. D., & Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus global self-worth: two different ways of relating to oneself. Journal of Personality, 77, 23–50.PubMed
go back to reference Neff, K. D., Long, P., Knox, M. C., Davidson, O., Kuchar, A., Costigan, A., Williamson, Z., Rohleder, N., Toth-Kiraly, I., & Breines, J. G. (2018a). The forest and the trees: examining the association of self-compassion and its positive and negative components with psychological functioning. Self and Identity, 17, 627–645. Neff, K. D., Long, P., Knox, M. C., Davidson, O., Kuchar, A., Costigan, A., Williamson, Z., Rohleder, N., Toth-Kiraly, I., & Breines, J. G. (2018a). The forest and the trees: examining the association of self-compassion and its positive and negative components with psychological functioning. Self and Identity, 17, 627–645.
go back to reference Neff, K. D., Toth-Kiraly, I., & Colosimo, K. (2018b). Self-compassion is best measured as a global construct and is overlapping with but distinct from neuroticism: a response to Pfattheicher, Geiger, Hartung, Weiss, and Schindler (2017). European Journal of Personality, 32, 371–392. Neff, K. D., Toth-Kiraly, I., & Colosimo, K. (2018b). Self-compassion is best measured as a global construct and is overlapping with but distinct from neuroticism: a response to Pfattheicher, Geiger, Hartung, Weiss, and Schindler (2017). European Journal of Personality, 32, 371–392.
go back to reference Neff, K. D., Toth-Kiraly, I., Yarnell, L. M., Arimitsu, K., Castilho, P., Ghorbani, N., Guo, H. X., Hirsch, J. K., Hupfeld, J., Hutz, C. S., Kotsou, I., Lee, W. K., Montero-Marin, J., Sirios, F. M., De Souza, L. K., Svendsen, J. L., Wilkinson, R. B., & Mantzios, M. (2019). Examining the factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale in 20 diverse samples: support for use of a total score and six subscale scores. Psychological Assessment, 31, 27–45.PubMed Neff, K. D., Toth-Kiraly, I., Yarnell, L. M., Arimitsu, K., Castilho, P., Ghorbani, N., Guo, H. X., Hirsch, J. K., Hupfeld, J., Hutz, C. S., Kotsou, I., Lee, W. K., Montero-Marin, J., Sirios, F. M., De Souza, L. K., Svendsen, J. L., Wilkinson, R. B., & Mantzios, M. (2019). Examining the factor structure of the Self-Compassion Scale in 20 diverse samples: support for use of a total score and six subscale scores. Psychological Assessment, 31, 27–45.PubMed
go back to reference Oswald, M. E., & Grosjean, S. (2004). Confirmation bias. In R. F. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions: a handbook on fallacies and bias in thinking, judgment, and memory (pp. 79–96). Hove: Psychology Press. Oswald, M. E., & Grosjean, S. (2004). Confirmation bias. In R. F. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions: a handbook on fallacies and bias in thinking, judgment, and memory (pp. 79–96). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Petrocchi, N., Ottaviani, C., & Couyoumdjian, A. (2014). Dimensionality of self-compassion: translation and construct validation of the Self-Compassion Scale in an Italian sample. Journal of Mental Health, 23, 72–77.PubMed Petrocchi, N., Ottaviani, C., & Couyoumdjian, A. (2014). Dimensionality of self-compassion: translation and construct validation of the Self-Compassion Scale in an Italian sample. Journal of Mental Health, 23, 72–77.PubMed
go back to reference Pfattheicher, S., Geiger, M., Hartung, J., Weiss, S., & Schindler, S. (2017). Old wine in new bottles? The case of self-compassion and neuroticism. European Journal of Personality, 31, 160–169. Pfattheicher, S., Geiger, M., Hartung, J., Weiss, S., & Schindler, S. (2017). Old wine in new bottles? The case of self-compassion and neuroticism. European Journal of Personality, 31, 160–169.
go back to reference Popper, K. R. (1963). Conjectures and refutations: the growth of scientific knowledge. London: Routledge. Popper, K. R. (1963). Conjectures and refutations: the growth of scientific knowledge. London: Routledge.
go back to reference Raes, F., Pommier, E., Neff, K. D., & Van Gucht, D. (2011). Construction and factorial validation of a short form of the Self-Compassion Scale. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 18, 250–255.PubMed Raes, F., Pommier, E., Neff, K. D., & Van Gucht, D. (2011). Construction and factorial validation of a short form of the Self-Compassion Scale. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 18, 250–255.PubMed
go back to reference Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
go back to reference Rudwick, M. J. S. (1974). Darwin and Glen Roy: a ‘great failure’ in scientific method? Studies in the History and Philosophy of Science, 5, 97–185. Rudwick, M. J. S. (1974). Darwin and Glen Roy: a ‘great failure’ in scientific method? Studies in the History and Philosophy of Science, 5, 97–185.
go back to reference Salkovskis, P. M., & Clark, D. M. (1990). Affective responses to hyperventilation: a test of the cognitive model of panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 15–28. Salkovskis, P. M., & Clark, D. M. (1990). Affective responses to hyperventilation: a test of the cognitive model of panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 15–28.
go back to reference Schwartz, R. S., Lederman, N. G., & Crawford, B. A. (2004). Developing views of nature of science in an authentic context: an explicit approach to bridging the gap between nature of science and scientific inquiry. Science Education, 88, 610–645. Schwartz, R. S., Lederman, N. G., & Crawford, B. A. (2004). Developing views of nature of science in an authentic context: an explicit approach to bridging the gap between nature of science and scientific inquiry. Science Education, 88, 610–645.
go back to reference Seppälä, E. M., Simon-Thomas, E., Brown, S. L., Worline, M. C., Cameron, C. D., & Doty, J. R. (Eds.). (2018). The Oxford handbook of compassion science. New York: Oxford University Press. Seppälä, E. M., Simon-Thomas, E., Brown, S. L., Worline, M. C., Cameron, C. D., & Doty, J. R. (Eds.). (2018). The Oxford handbook of compassion science. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Smith, G. T., McCarthy, D. M., & Zapolski, T. C. B. (2009). On the value of homogeneous constructs for construct validation, theory testing, and the description of psychopathology. Psychological Assessment, 21, 272–284.PubMedPubMedCentral Smith, G. T., McCarthy, D. M., & Zapolski, T. C. B. (2009). On the value of homogeneous constructs for construct validation, theory testing, and the description of psychopathology. Psychological Assessment, 21, 272–284.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Strauss, C., Taylor, B. L., Gu, J., Kuyken, W., Baer, R., Jones, F., & Cavanagh, K. (2016). What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 15–27.PubMed Strauss, C., Taylor, B. L., Gu, J., Kuyken, W., Baer, R., Jones, F., & Cavanagh, K. (2016). What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 15–27.PubMed
go back to reference Tijdink, J. K., Bouter, L. M., Veldkamp, C., Van de Ven, P. M., Wicherts, J. M., & Smulders, Y. M. (2016). Personality traits are associated with research misbehavior in Dutch scientists: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One, 11(9), e0163251.PubMedPubMedCentral Tijdink, J. K., Bouter, L. M., Veldkamp, C., Van de Ven, P. M., Wicherts, J. M., & Smulders, Y. M. (2016). Personality traits are associated with research misbehavior in Dutch scientists: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One, 11(9), e0163251.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Toomela, A. (2019). The psychology of scientific inquiry. Cham: Springer. Toomela, A. (2019). The psychology of scientific inquiry. Cham: Springer.
go back to reference Toth-Kiraly, I., Bothe, B., & Orosz, G. (2017). Exploratory structural equation modeling analysis of the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 8, 881–892. Toth-Kiraly, I., Bothe, B., & Orosz, G. (2017). Exploratory structural equation modeling analysis of the Self-Compassion Scale. Mindfulness, 8, 881–892.
go back to reference Ursic, N., Kocjancic, D., & Zvelc, G. (2019). Psychometric properties of the Slovenian long and short version of the Self-Compassion Scale. Psihologija, 52, 107–125. Ursic, N., Kocjancic, D., & Zvelc, G. (2019). Psychometric properties of the Slovenian long and short version of the Self-Compassion Scale. Psihologija, 52, 107–125.
go back to reference Van Dam, N. T., Sheppard, S. C., Forsyth, J. P., & Earleywine, M. (2011). Self-compassion is a better predictor than mindfulness of symptom severity and quality of life in mixed anxiety and depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 123–130.PubMed Van Dam, N. T., Sheppard, S. C., Forsyth, J. P., & Earleywine, M. (2011). Self-compassion is a better predictor than mindfulness of symptom severity and quality of life in mixed anxiety and depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 123–130.PubMed
go back to reference Wadsworth, L. P., Forgeard, M., Hsu, K. J., Kertz, S., Treadway, M., & Bjorgvinsson, T. (2018). Examining the role of repetitive negative thinking in relations between positive and negative aspects of self-compassion and symptom improvement during intensive treatment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42, 236–249. Wadsworth, L. P., Forgeard, M., Hsu, K. J., Kertz, S., Treadway, M., & Bjorgvinsson, T. (2018). Examining the role of repetitive negative thinking in relations between positive and negative aspects of self-compassion and symptom improvement during intensive treatment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42, 236–249.
go back to reference Williams, M. J., Dalgleish, T., Karl, A., & Kuyken, W. (2014). Examining the factor structures of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and the Self-Compassion Scale. Psychological Assessment, 26, 407–418.PubMed Williams, M. J., Dalgleish, T., Karl, A., & Kuyken, W. (2014). Examining the factor structures of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire and the Self-Compassion Scale. Psychological Assessment, 26, 407–418.PubMed
go back to reference Wilson, A. C., Mackintosh, K., Power, K., & Chan, S. W. Y. (2019). Effectiveness of self-compassion related therapies: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mindfulness, 10, 979–995. Wilson, A. C., Mackintosh, K., Power, K., & Chan, S. W. Y. (2019). Effectiveness of self-compassion related therapies: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mindfulness, 10, 979–995.
go back to reference Wong, N., Rindfleish, A., & Burroughs, J. E. (2003). Do reverse worded items confound measures in cross-cultural consumer research? The case of the Material Values Scale. Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 72–91. Wong, N., Rindfleish, A., & Burroughs, J. E. (2003). Do reverse worded items confound measures in cross-cultural consumer research? The case of the Material Values Scale. Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 72–91.
go back to reference Yarnell, L. M., Stafford, R. E., Neff, K. D., Reilly, E. D., Knox, M. C., & Mullarkey, M. (2015). Meta-analysis of gender differences in self-compassion. Self and Identity, 14, 499–520. Yarnell, L. M., Stafford, R. E., Neff, K. D., Reilly, E. D., Knox, M. C., & Mullarkey, M. (2015). Meta-analysis of gender differences in self-compassion. Self and Identity, 14, 499–520.
go back to reference Yarnell, L. M., Neff, K. D., Davidson, O. A., & Mullarkey, M. (2019). Gender differences in self-compassion: examining the role of gender role orientation. Mindfulness, 10, 1136–1152. Yarnell, L. M., Neff, K. D., Davidson, O. A., & Mullarkey, M. (2019). Gender differences in self-compassion: examining the role of gender role orientation. Mindfulness, 10, 1136–1152.
go back to reference Zessin, U., Dickhäuser, O., & Garbade, S. (2015). The relationship between self-compassion and well-being: a meta-analysis. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-being, 7, 340–364. Zessin, U., Dickhäuser, O., & Garbade, S. (2015). The relationship between self-compassion and well-being: a meta-analysis. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-being, 7, 340–364.
go back to reference Zhang, H., Dong, L., Watson-Singleton, N. N., Tarantino, N., Carr, E. R., Niles-Carnes, L. V., Patterson, B., & Kaslow, N. J. (2019). Psychometric properties of the Self-Compassion Scale in an African American sample. Mindfulness, 10, 1395–1405. Zhang, H., Dong, L., Watson-Singleton, N. N., Tarantino, N., Carr, E. R., Niles-Carnes, L. V., Patterson, B., & Kaslow, N. J. (2019). Psychometric properties of the Self-Compassion Scale in an African American sample. Mindfulness, 10, 1395–1405.
Metagegevens
Titel
The Process of Science: A Critical Evaluation of more than 15 Years of Research on Self-Compassion with the Self-Compassion Scale
Auteurs
Peter Muris
Henry Otgaar
Publicatiedatum
07-04-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Mindfulness / Uitgave 6/2020
Print ISSN: 1868-8527
Elektronisch ISSN: 1868-8535
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01363-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2020

Mindfulness 6/2020 Naar de uitgave

ORTHOGONAL ROTATION IN CONSCIOUSNESS

“I Read the News Today, Oh Boy”