skip to main content
research-article

Walking improves your cognitive map in environments that are large-scale and large in extent

Authors Info & Claims
Published:01 July 2011Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

This study investigated the effect of body-based information (proprioception, etc.) when participants navigated large-scale virtual marketplaces that were either small (Experiment 1) or large in extent (Experiment 2). Extent refers to the size of an environment, whereas scale refers to whether people have to travel through an environment to see the detail necessary for navigation. Each participant was provided with full body-based information (walking through the virtual marketplaces in a large tracking hall or on an omnidirectional treadmill), just the translational component of body-based information (walking on a linear treadmill, but turning with a joystick), just the rotational component (physically turning but using a joystick to translate) or no body-based information (joysticks to translate and rotate). In large and small environments translational body-based information significantly improved the accuracy of participants' cognitive maps, measured using estimates of direction and relative straight line distance but, on its own, rotational body-based information had no effect. In environments of small extent, full body-based information also improved participants' navigational performance. The experiments show that locomotion devices such as linear treadmills would bring substantial benefits to virtual environment applications where large spaces are navigated, and theories of human navigation need to reconsider the contribution made by body-based information, and distinguish between environmental scale and extent.

References

  1. Avraamides, M. N., Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., and Golledge, R. G. 2004. Use of cognitive versus perceptual heading during imagined locomotion depends on the response mode. Psych. Sci. 15, 403--408.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  2. Bremmer, F. and Lappe, M. 1999. The use of optical velocities for distance discrimination and reproduction during visually simulated self motion. Exper. Brain Resear. 127, 33--42.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Brooks, F. P., Airey, J., Alspaugh, J., Bell, A., Brown, R., Hill, C., Nimscheck, U., Rheingans, P., Rohlf, J., Smith, D., Turner, D., Varshney, A., Wang, Y., Weber, H., and Yuan, X. 1992. Six generations of building walkthrough: Final technical report to the National Science Foundation. Tech. rep. #TR92-026. Department of Computer Science, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  4. Brotons-Mas, J. R., O'Mara, S., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. 2006. Neural processing of spatial information: What we know about place cells and what they can tell us about presence. Presence: Teleop. Virt. Enviro. 15, 485--499. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  5. Buchanan, G., Blandford, A., Thimbleby, H., and Jones, M. 2004. Integrating information seeking and structuring: Exploring the role of spatial hypertexts in a digital library. In Proceedings of the 15th ACM Conference on Hypertext and Hypermedia. ACM, New York, 225--234. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  6. Campos, J. L., Byme, P., and Sun, H. J. 2010. The brain weights body-based cues higher than vision when estimating walked distances. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1889--1898.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  7. Chance, S. S., Gaunet, F., Beall, A. C., and Loomis, J. M. 1998. Locomotion mode affects the updating of objects encountered during travel: The contribution of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs to path integration. Presence: Teleop. Virt. Enviro. 7, 168--178. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  8. Cockburn, A. and Mckenzie, B. 2002. Evaluating the effectiveness of spatial memory in 2D and 3D physical and virtual environments. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM, New York, 203--210. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  9. Darken, R., Cockayne, W., and Carmein, D. 1997. The omni-directional treadmill: A locomotion device for virtual worlds. In Proceedings of the 10th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology. ACM, New York, 213--221. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  10. De Luca, A., Mattone, R., Giordano, P. R., and BüLthoff, H. H. 2009. Control design and experimental evaluation of the 2D CyberWalk platform. In Proceedings of the IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS'09). IEEE, Los Alamitos: CA, 5051--5058. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  11. Doeller, C. F., Barry, C., and Burgess, N. 2010. Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network. Nature 463, 657--661.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  12. Feasel, J., Whitton, M. C., and Wendt, J. D. 2008. LLCM-WIP: Low-latency, continuous-motion walking-in-place. In Proceedings of the 2008 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces. IEEE, Los Alamitos, CA, 97--104. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  13. Foo, P., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A., and Tarr, M. J. 2005. Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. J. Exper. Psych.: Learn. Mem. Cogni. 31, 195--215.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  14. Grant, S. C. and Magee, L. E. 1998. Contributions of proprioception to navigation in virtual environments. Human Factors 40, 489--497.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. Hollerbach, J., Checcacci, D., Noma, H., Yanagida, Y., and Tetsutani, N. 2003. Simulating side slopes on locomotion interfaces using torso forces. In Proceedings of the 11th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environments and Teleoperation. IEEE, Los Alamitos, CA, 91--98. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  16. Hölscher, C., Schnee, A., Dahmen, H., Setia, L., and Mallot, H. A. 2005. Rats are able to navigate in virtual environments. J. Exper. Biol. 208, 561--569.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. Jansen-Osmann, P. and Fuchs, P. 2006. Wayfinding behavior and spatial knowledge of adults and children in a virtual environment: The role of landmarks. Exper. Psych. 5, 171--181.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. Kearns, M. J., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A. P., and Tarr, M. J. 2002. Path integration from optic flow and body senses in a homing task. Perception 31, 349--374.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  19. Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Beall, A. C., Chance, S. S., and Golledge, R. G. 1998. Spatial updating of self-position and orientation during real, imagined, and virtual locomotion. Psych. Sci. 9, 293--298.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  20. Lessels, S. and Ruddle, R. A. 2004. Changes in navigational behaviour produced by a wide field of view and a high fidelity visual scene. In Proceedings of the 10th Eurographics Symposium on Virtual Environments. Eurographics Association, Aire-la-Ville, Switzerland, 71--78. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  21. Lessels, S. and Ruddle, R. A. 2005. Movement around real and virtual cluttered environments. Presence: Teleop. Virt. Environ. 14, 580--596. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  22. Loomis, J., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., and Philbeck, J. W. 1999. Human navigation by path integration. In Wayfinding: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes, R. Golledge Ed., John Hopkins, Baltimore, MD, 125--151.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  23. Mcnaughton, B. L., Battaglia, F. P., Jensen, O., Moser, E. I., and Moser, M.-B. 2006. Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map'. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 7, 663--678.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  24. Montello, D. R. 1991. The measurement of cognitive distance: Methods and construct validity. J. Environ. Psych. 11, 101--122.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Montello, D. R. 1998. A new framework for understanding the acquisition of spatial knowledge in large-scale environments. In Spatial and Temporal Reasoning in Geographic Information Systems, R. Golledge and M. Egenhofer Eds., Oxford University, New York, 143--154.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. O'hara, K. and Payne, S. 1998. The effects of operator implementation cost on planfulness of problem solving and learning. Cogn. Psych. 35, 34--70.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. O'Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. 1978. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford University, Oxford.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  28. Peck, T. C., Fuchs, H., and Whitton, M. C. 2009. Evaluation of reorientation techniques and distractors for walking in large virtual environments. IEEE Trans. Visualiza. Comput. Graph. 15, 383--394. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  29. Riecke, B. E., Van Veen, H. A. H. C., and BüLthoff, H. H. 2002. Visual homing is possible without landmarks: A path integration study in virtual reality. Presence: Teleop. Virt. Environ. 11, 443--473. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  30. Robertson, G., Czerwinski, M., Larson, K., Robbins, D. C., Thiel, D., and Van Dantzich, M. 1998. Data mountain: Using spatial memory for document management. In Proceedings of the 11th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology. ACM, New York, 153--162. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  31. Ruddle, R. A. 2001. Navigation: Am I really lost or virtually there? In Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics, D. Harris Ed., Ashgate, Burlington, VT, 135--142.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  32. Ruddle, R. A. 2010. INSPIRE: A new method for mapping information spaces. In Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on Information Visualisation (IV'10). IEEE, Los Alamitos, CA, 273--279. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  33. Ruddle, R. A., Howes, A., Payne, S. J., and Jones, D. M. 2000. The effects of hyperlinks on navigation in virtual environments. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud. 53, 551--581. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  34. Ruddle, R. A. and Lessels, S. 2009. The benefits of using a walking interface to navigate virtual environments. ACM Trans. Comput.-Hum. Interact. 16, article 5. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  35. Ruddle, R. A., Payne, S. J., and Jones, D. M. 1997. Navigating buildings in “desk-top” virtual environments: Experimental investigations using extended navigational experience. J. Exper. Psychol. Appl. 3, 143--159.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  36. Ruddle, R. A., Payne, S. J., and Jones, D. M. 1999. Navigating large-scale virtual environments: What differences occur between helmet-mounted and desk-top displays? Presence: Teleop. Virt. Environ. 8, 157--168. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  37. Ruddle, R. A. and Péruch, P. 2004. Effects of proprioceptive feedback and environmental characteristics on spatial learning in virtual environments. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud. 60, 299--326. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  38. Ruddle, R. A., Volkova, E., Mohler, B., and Bülthoff, H. H. in press. The effect of landmark and body-based sensory information on route learning. Memory Cognition.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  39. Slater, M., Usoh, M., and Steed, A. 1995. Taking steps: The influence of a walking technique on presence in virtual reality. ACM Trans. Comput.-Hum. Interact. 2, 201--219. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  40. Souman, J. L., Frissen, I., Sreenivasa, M. N., and Ernst, M. O. 2009. Walking straight into circles. Current Biology 19, 1538--1542.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  41. Souman, J. L., Giordano, P. R., Frissen, I., De Luca, A., and Ernst, M. O. 2010. Making virtual walking real: Perceptual evaluation of a new treadmill control algorithm. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 7, article 11. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  42. Suma, E. A., Finkelstein, S. L., Reid, M., Babu, S. V., Ulinski, A. C., and Hodges, L. F. 2010. Evaluation of the cognitive effects of travel technique in complex real and virtual environments. IEEE Trans. Visualiza. Comput. Graph. 16, 690--702. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  43. Sun, H. J., Campos, J. L., Young, M., Chan, G. S. W., and Ellard, C. G. 2004. The contributions of static visual cues, nonvisual cues, and topic flow in distance estimation. Perception 33, 49--65.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  44. Tan, D. S., Gergle, D., Scupelli, P., and Pausch, R. 2006. Physically large displays improve performance on spatial tasks. ACM Trans. Comput.-Hum. Interact. 13, 71--99. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  45. Templeman, J., Denbrook, P., and Sibert, L. 1999. Virtual locomotion: Walking in place through virtual environments. Presence: Teleop. Virtual Environ. 8, 598--617. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  46. Thorndyke, P. and Hayes-Roth, B. 1982. Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cogn. Psych. 14, 560--589.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  47. Trutoiu, L. C., Mohler, B. J., Schulte-Pelkum, J., and BüLthoff, H. H. 2009. Circular, linear, and curvilinear vection in a large-screen virtual environment with floor projection Comput. Graph. 33, 47--58. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  48. Tversky, B., Morrison, J. B., Franklin, N., and Bryant, D. J. 1999. Three spaces of spatial cognition. Profess. Geog. 51, 516--524.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  49. Waller, D., Hunt, E., and Knapp, D. 1998. The transfer of spatial knowledge in virtual environment training. Presence: Teleop. Virt. Environ. 7, 129--143. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  50. Waller, D., Loomis, J. M., and Haun, D. B. M. 2004. Body-based senses enhance knowledge of directions in large-scale environments. Psychonomi. Bull. Rev. 11, 157--163.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  51. Weatherford, D. L. 1985. Representing and manipulating spatial information from different environments: Models to neighborhoods. In The development of spatial cognitionus, R. Cohen Ed., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, 41--70.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  52. Whitton, M. C., Cohn, J. V., Feasel, J., Zimmons, P., Razzaque, S., Poulton, S. J., Mcleod, B., and Brooks, F. P. 2005. Comparing VE locomotion interfaces. In Proceedings of the IEEE Virtual Reality Conference. IEEE, Los Alamitos, CA, 123--130. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  53. Witmer, B., Bailey, J., Knerr, B., and Parsons, K. 1996. Virtual spaces and real-world places: Transfer of route knowledge. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud. 45, 413--428. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  54. Zanbaka, C., Lok, B., Babu, S., Ulinski, A., and Hodges, L. 2005. Comparison of path visualizations and cognitive measures relative to travel techniques in a virtual environment. IEEE Trans. Visualiza. Comput. Graph. 11, 694--705. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library

Index Terms

  1. Walking improves your cognitive map in environments that are large-scale and large in extent

        Recommendations

        Comments

        Login options

        Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

        Sign in

        Full Access

        • Published in

          cover image ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction
          ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction  Volume 18, Issue 2
          June 2011
          149 pages
          ISSN:1073-0516
          EISSN:1557-7325
          DOI:10.1145/1970378
          Issue’s Table of Contents

          Copyright © 2011 ACM

          Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

          Publisher

          Association for Computing Machinery

          New York, NY, United States

          Publication History

          • Published: 1 July 2011
          • Accepted: 1 February 2011
          • Revised: 1 December 2010
          • Received: 1 June 2010
          Published in tochi Volume 18, Issue 2

          Permissions

          Request permissions about this article.

          Request Permissions

          Check for updates

          Qualifiers

          • research-article
          • Research
          • Refereed

        PDF Format

        View or Download as a PDF file.

        PDF

        eReader

        View online with eReader.

        eReader