Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a000010

Zusammenfassung. Die Bedeutung der Exekutiven Funktionen im Vorschulalter rückt zunehmend in den Fokus entwicklungspsychologischer Forschungsliteratur. Diese exekutiven Kontrollprozesse sind nicht nur zentral für die kognitive, soziale und motorische Entwicklung der Kinder im späten Vorschulalter, sondern gelten ebenso als bedeutsame Prädiktoren für den Schulerfolg. Die vorliegende Studie versucht auf der Grundlage der Daten von insgesamt 410 Kindern (61–88 Monate) die Frage von Entwicklungsveränderungen und Beziehungsmuster verschiedener Prozesse Exekutiver Funktionen zu klären. Die Befunde sprechen für eine wichtige Entwicklungsphase der Exekutiven Funktionen im späten Vorschulalter und bedeutsame Zusammenhänge zwischen den Exekutiven Funktionen und den Individualfaktoren Sprache, Intelligenz, Motorik und Selbstregulation sowie dem Umweltfaktor sozioökonomischer Status. Kein Zusammenhang wurde allerdings zwischen den Exekutiven Funktionen und der elterlichen Unterstützung bzw. Förderung gefunden. Die Ergebnisse werden bezüglich der Struktur des Konstrukts der Exekutiven Funktionen und hinsichtlich der Relevanz der Exekutiven Funktionen im Rahmen der Entwicklungsvoraussetzungen für die Schulbereitschaft diskutiert.


Executive functions: Underlying cognitive processes and their correlates in late preschool children

Abstract. In the developmental literature, there is a growing interest in executive functioning, especially of preschool children. Executive control processes are now widely recognized to play an essential role not only for children’s cognitive, social, and motor development, but have also been shown to constitute significant predictors for later school success. The present study addresses developmental progression in executive functions, its internal structure, and well as associations with other central developmental constructs. A total of N = 410 children (61–88 months) participated in this study. Results speak for pronounced developmental progression in the late preschool years and for significant interrelations between executive function and the individual characteristics such as language, intelligence, motor coordination skills, self-regulation, as well as parents’ socioeconomic background. However, no significant relationship between children’s executive functioning and parental fostering was found. Results are discussed with regard to the theoretical framework of executive function and in the context of the discussion about preschoolers’ school readiness.

Literatur

  • Alloway, T. P. , Gathercole, S. E. , Kirkwood, H. & Elliott, J. (2009). The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Development, 80, 606–621. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Archibald, S. J. , Kerns, K. A. (1999). Identification and description of new tests of executive functioning in children. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 115–129. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111–127. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, C. , Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brocki, K. C. , Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 571–593. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bull, R. , Espy, K. A. , Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205–228. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burrage, M. S. , Ponitz, C. C. , McCready, E. A. , Shah, P. , Sims, B. C. , Jewkes, A. M. , Morrison, F. J. (2008). Age- and schooling related effects on executive functions in young children: A natural experiment. Child Neuropsychology, 14, 510–524. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carlson, S. M. (2005) Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 595–616. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carlson, S. M. , Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in Inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carlson, S. M. , Mandell, D. , Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Casey, B. J. , Galvan, A. , Hare, T. (2005). Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 239–244. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cattell, R. B. , Weiss, R. H. , Osterland, J. (1997). CFT 1: Grundintelligenztest Skala 1 (5. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Daseking, M. , Oldenhage, M. , Petermann, F. (2008). Der Übergang vom Kindergarten in die Grundschule – eine Bestandsaufnahme. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 55, 84–99. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, M. C. , Amso, D. , Anderson, L. C. , Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2037–2078. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Smeth, B. , Janssen, R. , Bouwens, K. , Verschaffel, L. , Boets, B. , Ghesquière, P. (2009). Working memory and individual differences in mathematics achievement: A longitudinal study from first to second grade. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 186–201. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71, 44–56. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D. Stuss, R. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466–503). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, A. , Carlson, S. M. , Beck, D. M. (2005). Preschool children’s performance in task switching on the dimensional change card sort task: Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 689–729. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dowsett, S. , Livesey, D. (2000). The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: Effects of executive skills training. Developmental Psychology, 36, 161–174. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, G. J. , Claessens, A. , Huston, A. C. , Pagani, L. S. , Engel, M. , Sexton, H. , Dowsett, C. J. , Magnuson, K. , Klebanov, P. , Feinstein, L. , Brooks-Gunn, J. , Duckworth, K. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1426–1446. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Durston, S. , Thomas, K. M. , Yang, Y. , Ulug, A. M. , Zimmermann, R. D. , Casey, B. J. (2002). A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 5, 9–16. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gerstadt, C. L. , Hong, Y. J. , Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of 3½–7 years old on Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129–153. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garon, N. , Bryson, S. E. , Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 1, 31–60. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hasselhorn, M. , Lohaus, A. (2008). Entwicklungsvoraussetzungen und Herausforderungen des Schuleintritts. In M. Hasselhorn, R. K. Silbereisen (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Themenbereich C, Theorie und Forschung, Serie V, Entwicklungspsychologie, Band 4, Entwicklungspsychologie des Säuglings- und Kindesalter (S. 409–428). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Henderson, S. E. , Sugden, D. A. , Barnett, A. L. (2007) M-ABC-2: Movement Assessment Battery for Children - 2. London: Harcourt. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hongwanishkul, D. , Happaney, K. R. , Lee, W. , Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Hot and cool executive functions: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huizinga, M. , Dolan, C. , van der Molen, M. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kail, R. (1991). Processing time declines exponentially during childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 259–266. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kloo, D. , Perner, J. (2003). Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: Helping children apply conflicting descriptions. Child Development, 74, 1823–1839. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Korkman, M. , Kirk, U. , Kemp, S. L. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lehto, J. E. , Juujärvi, P. , Kooistra, L. & Pulkkinnen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59–80. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Livesey, D. , Keen, J. , Rouse, J. , White, F. (2006). The relationship between measures of executive function, motor performance and externalising behavior in 5- and 6-year-old children. Human Movement Science, 25, 50–64. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mandich, A. , Buckholz, E. , Polatajko, H. (2003). Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and their inability to disengage ongoing attentional focus: More on inhibitory function. Brain and Cognition, 51, 346–356. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marzocchi, G. M. , Oosterlaan, J. , Zuddas, A. , Cavolina, P. , Geurts, H. , Redigolo, D. , Vio, C. , Sergeant, J. S. (2008). Contrasting deficits on executive functions between ADHD and reading disabled children. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 543–552. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McClelland, M. M. , McDonald Connor, C. , Jewkes, A. M. , Cameron, C. E. , Farris, C. L. , Morrison, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology, 43, 947–959. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Michel, E. , Roebers, C. M. (2008). Children in regular and special needs classes: Cognitive and non-cognitive aspects. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67, 249–259. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A. , Friedman, N. , Emerson, M. , Witzki, A. H. , Howerter, A. , Wagner, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex „frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morton, J. B. , Munakata, Y. (2002). Active vs. latent representations: A neural network model of perseveration, dissociation, and decalage. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 255–263. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neuenschwander, R. , Röthlisberger, M. , Michel, E. , Roebers, C. M. (in Druck). Unterschiede in verschiedenen Bereichen der Schulbereitschaft in Abhängigkeit des vorschulischen Kontextes: Ein systematischer Vergleich von Kindergarten und einem neuen Schuleingangsmodel in der Schweiz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2003). Do children’s attention processes mediate the link between family predictor and school readiness? Developmental Psychology, 39, 581–593. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Noble, K. G. , Norman, M. F. , Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 74–87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Noble, K. G. , McCandliss, B. D. , Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science, 10, 464–480. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Overman, W. H. (2004). Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex. Brain Cognition, 55, 134–147. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pennington, B. F. , Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 51–87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pickering, S. J. , Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C). London: The Psychological Corporation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Piek, J. P. , Dyck, M. J. , Nieman, A. , Anderson, M. , Hay, D. , Smith, L. M. , McCoy, M. & Hallmayer, J. (2004). The relationship between motor coordination, executive functioning and attention in school aged children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 1063–1076. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riebel, J. , Jäger, R. S. (2008). Kompetenzen von Schulanfängern: Was sollten Schulanfänger können? Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 55, 132–142. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rimm-Kaufman, S. E. , Pianta, R. C. , Cox, M. J. (2000).Teacher’s judgements of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 147–166. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roebers, C. M. , Kauer, M. (2009). Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Developmental Science, 12, 175–181. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ricken, G. , Fritz, A. , Schuck, K. D. , Preuss, U. (2007) HAWIVA-III:. Hannover-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für das Vorschulalter – III. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rueda, M. R. , Fan, J. , McCandliss, B. D. , Halparin, J. D. , Gruber, D. B. , Lercari, L. P. , Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in chidhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–1040. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rueda, M. R. , Rothbart, M. K. , McCandliss, B. D. , Saccomanno, L. , Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. PNAS, 41, 14931–14936. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sabbagh, M. A. , Xu, F. , Carlson, S. M. , Moses, L. J. & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind. Psychology Science, 17, 74–81. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmid, C. , Zoelch, C. , Roebers, C. M. (2008). Das Arbeitsgedächtnis von 4-bis 5-jährigen Kindern: Theoretische und empirische Analyse seiner Funktionen. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 40, 2–12. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Thorell, L. B. , Lindqvist, S. , Bergman Nutley, S. , Bohlin, G. & Klingberg, T. (2008). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12, 106–113. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Sluis, S. , de Jong, P. F. , van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427–449. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegener, B. (1988). Kritik des Prestiges. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Welsh, M. C. (2002). Developmental and clinical variations in executive functions. In D. L. Molfese, V. J. Molfese (Eds.), Developmental variations in learning (pp. 139–185). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wright, I. , Waterman, M. , Prescott, H. & Murdoch-Eaton, D. (2003). A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: Typical developmental trends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 561–575. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. , Müller, U. , Frye, D. , Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, vii–137. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. , Carlson, S. M. , Kesek, A. (2008). The development of executive function in childhood. In C. A. Nelson, M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 553–574). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, P. , Gondan, M. , Fimm, B. (2002). KITAP Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung der Kinder. Herzogenrath: Vera Fimm, Psychologische Testsysteme. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zoelch, C. , Seitz, K. , Schumann-Hengsteler, R. (2005). From rag(bag)s to riches: measuring the developing central executive. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development. Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability and theory of mind (pp. 39–69). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar