The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.21121207

Abstract

Objective:

Craving is a central construct in the study of motivation and human behavior and is also a clinical symptom of substance and non-substance-related addictive disorders. Thus, craving represents a target for transdiagnostic modeling.

Methods:

The authors applied connectome-based predictive modeling (CPM) to functional connectivity data in a large (N=274) transdiagnostic sample of individuals with and without substance use–related conditions, to predict self-reported craving. Functional connectomes derived from three guided imagery conditions of personalized appetitive, stress, and neutral-relaxing experiences were used to predict craving rated before and after each imagery condition. The generalizability of the “craving network” was tested in an independent sample using functional connectomes derived from a cue-induced craving task collected before and after fasting to predict craving rated during fasting.

Results:

CPM successfully predicted craving, thereby identifying a transdiagnostic “craving network.” Anatomical localization of model contribution suggested that the strongest predictors of craving were regions of the salience, subcortical, and default mode networks. As external validation, in an independent sample, the “craving network” predicted food craving during fasting using data from a cue-induced craving task.

Conclusions:

These data provide a transdiagnostic perspective to a key phenomenological feature of addictive disorders—craving—and identify a common “craving network” across individuals with and without substance use–related disorders, thereby suggesting a neural signature for craving or urge for motivated behaviors.

Craving—the strong desire or urge to engage in motivated action, such as substance use (1)—is fundamental to addiction (2) and predicts future substance use and treatment outcomes. More broadly, craving is considered central to understanding motivation for appetitive behavior (3, 4) and is widely accepted as a construct for behavioral intent in both animal models and humans (e.g., 5, 6). Self-reported craving has been included as a testing paradigm for constructs (e.g., expectancy/reward prediction error) that are primary to understanding addictive behavior across substance and behavioral addictions (7). Furthermore, behavior motivated by reward or appetite, mediated by “wanting,” is experienced by individuals with and without substance use disorders (e.g., food craving [8], non-disordered substance use), with addictive behavior motivated by excessive “wanting” of a substance, or substance craving (4). There is ample evidence that self-reported craving relates to intake behavior across drugs (9), food (10), and other non-disordered craving such as craving for coffee. Therefore, craving represents a target for transdiagnostic modeling that includes comparator samples (i.e., spanning normal and abnormal craving) (11).

The clinical significance of craving is evidenced by its inclusion in DSM-5 and ICD-10 diagnostic criteria for substance use disorders. Support for the clinical relevance of craving is bolstered by neuroimaging meta-analyses showing similar neural substrates underlying substance cue reactivity and craving across substance use disorders (12, 13). Self-reported craving is consistently correlated with activation in brain regions such as the ventral striatum in response to drug and natural reward cues (1214). However, despite identifying multiple brain regions related to craving, these correlations do not offer a clinically viable biomarker for craving report (9). This may be in part because the included studies typically rely on traditional statistical inference (e.g., analyses of variance, correlations) to identify neuroimaging measures related to craving measures. These approaches aim to explain the variance between two or more variables and often do not generalize to novel individuals (15). There has been very little predictive research on brain-phenotype associations, including with craving, across substance use disorders.

Connectome-based predictive modeling (CPM) is a well-validated machine learning approach for generating predictive models of brain-phenotype associations from whole-brain functional connectivity data, or “connectomes” (16). In contrast to correlations or regressions, machine learning models, like CPM, protect against overfitting by being defined and validated in independent samples, leading to more accurate effect sizes and increasing the generalizability of findings (17, 18). CPM is also data driven, and it is therefore used to identify neural “signatures” in functional connectivity data related to a specific phenotype (16). Importantly, CPM has been used to predict treatment outcomes in substance use disorders (19, 20), whereas previous approaches had not yet identified reliable brain-based biomarkers of treatment response or abstinence (21), which is consistent with a neurobiology of successful abstinence involving complex interactions between multiple neural networks (22).

Our aim in this study was to identify a transdiagnostic “craving network” by applying CPM to functional connectivity data in individuals with and without substance use–related conditions to predict a continuous measure of self-reported craving. We used harmonized functional MRI (fMRI) data collected in a diverse set of participants with and without substance use–related conditions performing a guided imagery task. We also tested the generalizability of the “craving network” in an independent cohort using fMRI data from a cue-induced craving task. Based on previous work implicating the default mode network (DMN) in internally directed attention and self-referential processing related to craving (14, 23, 24) and in appetitive cue reactivity (14), we hypothesized that functional connectivity in the DMN would predict individual differences in self-reported craving.

Methods

Participants

All participants provided written informed consent, approved by the Yale University Human Investigation Committee. Participants included individuals with alcohol use disorder (N=38) or cocaine use disorder (N=28), as determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV and self-report of alcohol or cocaine use, verified by toxicology testing; obesity (body mass index >30; N=19); adolescents with prenatal cocaine exposure (N=41) as well as control subjects (N=148), including adults (N=131) and adolescents without prenatal cocaine exposure (N=17). The participants’ median age was 23 years (range, 14–45 years); 96 were male and 178 were female. Participants were included if they had complete data for self-reported craving and all three fMRI guided-imagery conditions, with an average head motion less than 0.2 mm.

Neuroimaging Data Acquisition

fMRI data were acquired during a personalized guided imagery task in which participants were presented with six imagery scripts: two appetitive (i.e., drug, favorite food), two stress (e.g., romantic break-up), and two neutral-relaxing (e.g., at the beach). Before and after each imagery script, participants rated their craving (their “desire to [use substance (e.g., drink alcohol)] at that moment” or their wanting of the specified food at that moment [“How much do you want (favorite-food) right now?”]) on a scale from 1, “not at all,” to 10, “more than ever” or “extremely high,” thus providing concurrent neural and subjective responses per trial. (For complete task details, see the online supplement.) Participants in studies with food cues were asked to eat ∼2 hours prior to scanning; those with cocaine use disorder were abstinent for at least 2 weeks prior to scanning; all were asked not to consume alcohol for 72 hours prior to scanning; and those with alcohol use disorder were abstinent for 4–8 weeks (for more details, see the online supplement).

Connectome-Based Predictive Modeling (CPM)

fMRI data were processed for each imagery condition with a validated pipeline and parcellated into 268 nodes using a whole-brain functional atlas (Figure 1A; see also the online supplement). Predictive modeling used a multidimensional approach, combining connectomes and craving measures into a single predictive model (Figure 1B) (25). A principal component analysis was used to summarize the six craving measures in a data-driven manner for predictive modeling, given that a single behavioral measurement represents a noisy approximation of any behavioral construct, and we did not have an a priori expectation of the best way to combine the measures. Principal component analysis is a common method to reduce the dimensionality of data to increase interpretability and minimize information loss. To maintain separate train and test groups, for each iteration, each analysis was limited to the training data sets and the principal component analysis coefficients were applied to the test data set.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Processing of fMRI data and combining of connectomes and craving measures into a single predictive modela

aAs illustrated in panel A, the Shen 268 functional parcellation consists of 200 cortical nodes, 15 subcortical nodes, nine nodes in the brainstem, and 39 nodes in the cerebellum. Panel B is a schematic of the study’s multidimensional connectome-based predictive modeling (CPM) pipeline. This approach combines three guided-imagery functional MRI connectomes and six craving ratings into a single predictive model using ridge regression and principal component analysis (PCA). (Panel B is modified, with permission, from reference 25.)

To generate predictive models of craving, all task connectomes were included as independent inputs for a single predictive model using ridge regression (26). Using multiple connectomes improves predictive modeling and better characterizes brain-behavior associations (26, 27). However, there is a high degree of similarity between task connectomes, and the edges are not independent. Ridge regression accounts for these dependencies in a principled manner. For feature selection, a statistical significance threshold of <0.05 was used to select edges that are positively and negatively associated with craving in the training data. Potential confounders (head motion, age, group, gender, anxiety, heart rate, smoking status) were controlled for at this step using partial correlation (25).

Model Validation and Performance

First, to train transdiagnostic models of craving, 10-fold cross-validation was used by dividing the total sample—regardless of diagnostic group—into 10 approximately equal-sized groups; on each fold, the model was trained on nine groups and tested on the excluded 10th group. This was repeated for 100 random divisions. Group membership was not balanced across iterations. Model performance was evaluated with a cross-validated coefficient of determination (q2), and the median q2 for 100 random 10-fold divisions is reported, along with Pearson’s correlation (r), Spearman’s rank correlation (ρ), and mean square error. To generate null distributions for significance testing, we randomly shuffled the correspondence between behavioral variables and connectivity matrices 1,000 times and reran the CPM analysis with the shuffled data. Based on these null distributions, the p values for predictions were calculated as in prior work. Because only a positive association between predicted and actual values can indicate prediction above chance (with negative associations indicating a failure to predict), one-tailed p values are reported.

Second, leave-one-group-out cross-validation was used to test whether prediction results were driven by group differences in prediction performance from including both substance use–related and control groups in a single model. Models were trained on the control group and tested in the substance use–related group and vice versa. To measure prediction performance, Pearson’s correlation was tested between actual and predicted craving.

For additional details on the above methods and quantification of task and anatomical contributions to prediction, see the online supplement.

Group Differences in Connectivity

Group differences in task connectomes (appetitive, stress, neutral-relaxing) between the substance use–related and control groups and how these group differences overlapped with the identified “craving network” were investigated (see the online supplement).

External Validation

Finally, we tested the generalizability of the “craving network” in an independent cohort using fMRI data from six runs of a cue-induced craving task collected before (baseline) and after fasting (28). This data set consisted of 32 healthy participants who viewed images of individuals talking, laughing, and smiling, highly palatable foods (cake, pizza, chocolate), and attractive flowers (as a control). Participants fasted (except water) for 10 hours before the fasting fMRI scan. Every 2 hours during fasting, self-reported craving was assessed on a scale of 0 to 100, with 100 representing the strongest craving. (For more details, see https://osf.io/a3yfe/ and https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds003242/versions/1.0.0.)

fMRI data were processed as described above. One connectome per participant for each of the baseline and fasting scans was created by averaging connectomes for each of the six runs. The five craving measures collected during fasting were averaged to create a single measure of craving. All participants had an average head motion <0.2 mm. Pearson’s correlations between actual and predicted craving and parametric p values were used to measure prediction performance. As above, negative associations indicate a failure to predict, and only positive associations were tested for significance.

Results

Self-Reported Craving

Craving ratings are reported in the Supplementary Results section of the online supplement.

Transdiagnostic Prediction of Craving

In a transdiagnostic manner, the overall CPM model successfully predicted self-reported craving as defined by the first principal component derived from all craving ratings (r=0.41, ρ=0.37, q2=0.14, mean square error=34.22, p<0.001, permutation testing 1,000 iterations, one-tailed) (Figure 2). Follow-up post hoc comparisons adjusting for head motion (r=0.40, ρ=0.36, q2=0.14, mean square error=34.28), heart rate (r=0.41, ρ=0.37, q2=0.14, mean square error=34.16), anxiety ratings (r=0.40, ρ=0.37, q2=0.13, mean square error=34.62), group (r=0.38, ρ=0.36, q2=0.13, mean square error=34.73), smoking status (r=0.41, ρ=0.38, q2=0.14, mean square error=34.08), and gender (r=0.41, ρ=0.37, q2=0.14, mean square error=34.12) demonstrated similar prediction performances. Prediction performance was reduced after adjustment for age but remained significant (r=0.24, ρ=0.24, q2=0.05, mean square error=37.88). The results were similar when age, gender, and head motion were adjusted for in the same model (r=0.20, ρ=0.21, q2=0.05, mean square error=38.97). The first principal component explained 92.5% of the variance. Each of the six craving ratings contributed approximately equally to the first principal component (see Figure S2 in the online supplement). The results were therefore similar when predicting the mean craving, rather than using the first principal component (see Table S2 in the online supplement). We could not predict imagery-induced changes in craving (post- minus pre-craving; r=0.04, ρ=0.07, q2=0.00, mean square error=2.5). Finally, models were specific to craving, rather than arousal and alerting to a stimulus, as they did not predict self-reported measures of anxiety (r=0.18, ρ=−0.11, p=0.20) or imagery vividness (r=−0.004, ρ=−0.019, p=0.57).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Connectome-based predictive model performance for transdiagnostic prediction of cravinga

aPanel A is a scatterplot of the observed first principal component of the craving data on the x-axis and the predicted principal component generated using CPM on the y-axis. Panel B is a histogram of the model performance, indexed by q2, across 100 iterations of the actual data (green) and 1,000 iterations of randomly permuted data (white).

While the results did not appear to be driven by better prediction in one group (Figure 2; see also the online supplement), this was tested by training on the substance use–related group or control group and testing on the other, as the groups differed in craving ratings. CPM predicted craving in the substance use–related group from models trained on the control group (r=0.28, df=146, p=0.001) and vice versa (r=0.19, df=124, p=0.02), showing that prediction was possible regardless of the training data. These two models were also similar, with an edge correlation of r=0.39 (p<0.001) and a node-level correlation of r=0.78 (p<0.001) of model coefficients.

Finally, we tested several alternative models, none of which resulted in superior performance (see Tables S2–S4 in the online supplement), and additionally found no gender differences in prediction performance (see the online supplement).

Anatomical and Task Contribution to Craving Prediction

In line with previous CPM results, the identified “craving network” was complex, with contributions from every node and canonical brain network, consisting of 3,069 unique edges (out of 35,778). The top 5% of nodes contributing to craving prediction were located in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), hippocampus, visual cortex, and primary sensory areas (Figure 3). When these spatial maps were uploaded to the Neurosynth Image Decoder (https://neurosynth.org/decode/), “default mode network” was the best match to our “craving network.” Similarly, at the network level, the DMN, motor-sensory, visual, and subcortical networks were the most prominent (Figure 4; see also Figure S3 in the online supplement). Finally, virtual lesioning, where canonical networks are iteratively removed from CPM to assess how a single network contributes to prediction performance (see the online supplement), highlighted that the subcortical, salience, and default mode networks were the most important networks for prediction (see Table S5–S6 in the online supplement). However, all predictions using a single network were significant (p values<0.01; see Table S6 in the online supplement).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Node-level contribution to predicting craving valuesa

aThe top row shows contribution (defined as the sum of all edgewise ridge regression coefficients normalized by the standard deviation of edges across participants for any node) across all three imagery conditions. The next three rows exhibit these contributions separated by each imagery condition. While all conditions highlight the posterior cingulate cortex as a top contributor, the appetitive and neutral-relaxing conditions also highlighted contributions from primary sensory and visual networks as well as the hippocampus. The stress condition highlights regions in the brainstem and parahippocampus. Warmer colors represent nodes that contributed more toward the final prediction.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4. Network-level contribution to predicting craving valuesa

aNode-level contributions to predictions were further summarized by canonical functional networks. The diagonal represents the average contribution of edges within a single network, and off-diagonal elements represent the average contribution of edges between two network pairs. Warmer colors represent networks that contributed more toward the final prediction. Missing nodes (see Figure S1 in the online supplement) were primarily in the cerebellar network, which may impact the contribution of this network. MF=medial frontal network; FPN=frontal parietal network; DMN=default mode network; MOT=motor-sensory network; V1=visual network 1; V2=visual network 2; VA=visual association network; SAL=salience network; SC=subcortical network; CBL=cerebellar network.

Next, given that multiple imagery conditions (appetitive, stress, neutral-relaxing) were included in a single predictive model, the contribution of each imagery condition was tested. All task conditions contributed to predicting craving (appetitive, 29.5%; stress, 27.0%, neutral-relaxing, 43.5%), and the distribution of task contributions did not differ significantly from a uniform distribution (χ2=0.05, df=2, p=0.80). For the appetitive condition, nodes with the largest contribution to craving prediction were located in the PCC and primary sensory areas, with the motor-sensory network and DMN as the top contributing networks. Similarly, for the neutral-relaxing condition, nodes with the largest contribution to craving prediction were located in the PCC and primary sensory areas, plus the hippocampus and visual cortex, with the visual networks contributing most to prediction at the network level. In contrast, for the stress condition, nodes with the largest contribution to craving prediction were located in the parahippocampus and brainstem, as well as the PCC, with the subcortical (which includes the brainstem nodes) and motor-sensory networks as the top contributing networks.

Finally, node and network contributions for the appetitive condition were significantly correlated with node and network contributions for both the stress (node: r=0.34, df=245, p<0.001; network: r=0.47, df=53, p<0.001) and neutral-relaxing conditions (node: r=0.35, df=245, p<0.001; network: r=0.31, df=53, p=0.02). However, node and network contributions for the stress and neutral-relaxing conditions were not correlated (node: r=−0.03, df=245, p=0.63; network: r=−0.01, df=53, p=0.95). Overall, model features were similar when adolescents were excluded (r=0.53, df=245, p<0.001).

External Validation

In an independent cohort using a different fMRI task and measures of self-reported craving, the “craving network” successfully predicted self-reported food craving during 10 hours of fasting using both baseline (r=0.47, df=30, p=0.003) and fasting scans (r=0.31, df=30, p=0.04). The difference in prediction performance using baseline and fasting scans was not significant (z=1.01, p=0.31, Steiger’s test) (see Figure S5 in the online supplement).

Discussion

In this study, connectome-based predictive modeling was used to identify a neurofunctional “craving network” in a large transdiagnostic sample of individuals with and without substance use–related conditions. Functional connectivity in the identified “craving network” during an imagery task predicted a continuous measure of self-reported craving across individuals with alcohol or cocaine use disorders, individuals with obesity, adolescents with and without prenatal cocaine exposure, and adult control subjects. Importantly, the neuroanatomical nodes contributing to prediction and group differences were not correlated, suggesting that the “craving network” is neurobiologically distinct from group differences. Finally, for external validation, the “craving network” was used to predict self-reported food craving during fasting in an independent group. These data provide a transdiagnostic perspective to a key phenomenological feature of addictive disorders—craving—and identify a common “craving network” across individuals with and without substance use–related disorders, or across normal and pathological craving, thereby suggesting a neural signature for craving/urge for motivated behaviors.

Consistent with the CPM approach, the “craving network” was found to be complex and distributed throughout the brain. The top nodes contributing to predicting craving included the PCC, hippocampus, visual cortex, and primary sensory areas. The top networks contributing to predicting craving included the DMN, motor-sensory, visual, and subcortical networks. Furthermore, virtual lesion analysis indicated that the DMN, salience, and subcortical networks were the most important networks for predicting craving. These findings are largely consistent with meta-analyses of univariate brain activation studies of substance cue reactivity, which typically implicate the ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala and hippocampus, middle frontal gyrus, posterior cingulate and parietal cortex, and visual areas (12), and sometimes the insula (29), with the amygdala, inferior parietal cortex, and middle frontal gyrus correlated with self-reported craving (12). The findings are also largely consistent with a study that computed functional connectivity profiles of regions of interest from a cue reactivity meta-analysis to identify subnetworks of cue reactivity–related brain regions and identified visual, executive control, salience, DMN, and an emotion-related network (amygdala, insula, and orbitofrontal cortex) (14), although that study did not test self-reported craving.

Studies of psychiatric disorders more generally (30) and of substance use disorders (31, 32) are increasingly focused on understanding how distributed brain regions organized as large-scale brain networks contribute to dysfunction (30). The strongest predictor of craving in this study was functional connectivity in the DMN, and the DMN was a match to the “craving network” using the meta-analytic tool Neurosynth (33). Virtual lesion analysis additionally indicated that the DMN, salience network, and subcortical network were the most important in predicting craving. The DMN is a core functional network (34), implicated in internally directed thought processes—including self-referential thinking, such as autobiographical, self-monitoring, and social processes (30)—as opposed to externally directed processing (35). The salience network is considered to allocate attention to internal or external stimuli (36). The present finding is therefore consistent with a large-scale brain network model in which a lack of suppression of the DMN, with the salience network allocating attentional resources toward the DMN, results in altered self-referential processing (30), such as difficulty disengaging from the internal experiences of craving (23, 32). Mounting evidence suggests that alterations in the DMN are a common feature across psychiatric and neurological disorders (30, 37), including substance use disorders. We note that the anterior DMN (medial prefrontal cortex) was not found to be predictive of craving. The anterior and posterior midline DMN have distinct roles in self-referential processing, with the anterior DMN implicated in the attribution of personal value and the posterior DMN implicated in internally directed attention (38). Finally, the subcortical network comprises all subcortical structures, including reward regions (e.g., amygdala, thalamus, striatum), and typically differences in subcortical connectivity between control subjects and individuals with substance use disorders or obesity are interpreted as differences in reward processing (32, 39). Furthermore, functional connectivity in the salience, motor-sensory, and subcortical networks has been related to reward responsiveness in a model of cocaine abstinence derived using a similar CPM approach (19). Subcortical structures are also implicated in drug and food craving (12, 40, 41). Together, the present findings suggest that engagement of the DMN, salience network, and subcortical network in particular represent urge and intent for motivated behavior in response to various appetitive and stress cues that are key contexts that may drive motivated behavior. Increased connectivity in these networks appears to be a common feature of subjective craving that may influence motivated behavior in response to appetitive stimuli across individuals with or without substance use–related conditions.

Transdiagnostic studies such as this can be used to relate heterogeneous phenotypes (wanting/urges/craving) to specific brain networks (posterior DMN, salience network, subcortical network), which can then be related to clinically relevant variations and to genetic, molecular, and cellular factors (42). Such findings can also be used to evaluate how brain networks are uniquely altered in specific disorders (20). In this study, participants were grouped as individuals with and without substance use–related conditions, with several subgroups (substance use disorders, obesity, prenatal cocaine exposure) included in the former. Important differences in brain networks related to craving likely exist between these subgroups. However, the purpose of this study was to identify a common “craving network” across groups, or across normative and pathological craving. The diverse set of participants, as opposed to a well-controlled cohort, is needed to show that models are truly transdiagnostic across different subgroups of participants. Furthermore, each subgroup sample size was considered small for neuroimaging-based predictive modeling, where recommended sample sizes are in the range of >100–200 (43). The study was therefore underpowered for testing subgroups or between subgroups, which remains for future work.

The nodes contributing to prediction and group differences were not correlated, suggesting that the “craving network” is neurobiologically distinct from group differences and may therefore relate to craving/wanting as a more central construct driving human motivated behavior (4). Notably, the neutral-relaxing condition contributed the most to prediction (43.5%) but showed little between-group difference. This is consistent with transdiagnostic prediction, where the task condition in which groups are the most similar is the most predictive. Transdiagnostic predictive models aim to capture the common neural systems across groups, whereas group differences aim to capture the unique neural systems between groups.

Across both samples, there were numerical, but not statistically significant, differences between craving and connectomes collected at baseline or after cue induction. This may relate to a reported distinction between generalized and cue-induced craving. Generalized or background tonic craving may reflect the incentive or biological value of a drug and may be distinct from phasic, cue-induced craving reflecting drug-cue expectancy (44, 45). How these components of craving interact is not well understood, but it is likely that they are processed differently. For example, cigarette abstinence has been found to increase generalized craving but not cue-induced craving (44), and nicotine replacement therapy reduced overall craving but not cue-induced craving (46). Generalized craving is more similar across individuals with and without substance use–related conditions and might therefore be easier to predict. Likewise, drug cues and abstinence can have separate influences on craving (45), and drug cue reactivity can be modulated by individual differences in craving in response to abstinence (47).

Although there is no clear evidence that functional connectivity outperforms fMRI activation or other imaging measures in predicting clinical variables (48), functional connectivity does offer some advantages for brain-based models. In particular, functional connectivity better reflects the network structure of the brain, which is not represented by activation. Disturbances of network organization are a common thread in virtually all neurological and psychiatric disorders (49). Because task and rest connectomes are highly correlated, functional connectivity better generalizes to task conditions other than fMRI activation (50). Finally, methodologically, functional connectivity squares the number of features for machine learning (n nodes leads to n2 edges), thereby improving power for prediction. Indeed, recent work suggests that connectivity features better relate to phenotypic information than brain structure (51) and task activation (52). Nevertheless, these aspects must be tested empirically to assess whether connectivity is a better feature to predict craving than task activation.

This study had some limitations. The craving measure was self-reported and, as with all subjective measures, was therefore sensitive to response bias, introspective abilities or tendencies, and other factors. Some machine-learning studies have found higher prediction accuracy for objective measures than subjective ratings (53). However, there is no single accepted measure of craving, and nearly all models of craving assume that it can be measured at least in part by self-report (54, 55). The findings are also generalizable to much of the research on substance craving, which uses self-report (55). Furthermore, craving was measured in the moment, at multiple time points, and it was highly correlated across time and conditions, thereby reducing potential noise. It would be interesting to test the “craving network” related to objective measures of craving (physiological, behavioral) in future studies, although self-reported craving does relate to intake behavior with food (10) and drugs (9). Next, although the results indicated that prediction performance did not differ between genders, and models trained in one gender predicted the other, future studies might test how the “craving network” differs by gender, such as how it is related to clinical outcomes in substance use disorders. Additionally, despite consistencies in how the neuroanatomy of the craving network is situated in previous neuroimaging studies on craving, this literature is mainly based on task activation rather than functional connectivity. These considerations have clinical implications in measuring the “craving network,” with the caution that network activity does not imply the experience of craving. Lastly, while we robustly show that the results were not driven by certain subgroups, there may be effects of other participant characteristics, including clinical and cognitive variables, such as substance use severity and cognitive impairments.

In summary, this study used CPM to identify a transdiagnostic “craving network” in individuals with and without substance use–related conditions. The neuroanatomy of the “craving network” was consistent with previous meta-analyses of cue-reactivity and craving studies, and it critically extends that work to whole-brain functional connectivity using CPM, which is generalizable. This approach indicated that the DMN, the salience network, and the subcortical network were the most important networks in predicting craving. These findings have several potential clinical applications. The DMN has been suggested as a potential biomarker for substance use risk and treatment response and as a treatment target for substance use disorders (38). The identified “craving network” might be utilized similarly, in particular for individuals who report high levels of craving and for treatments that target craving.

Department of Psychiatry (Garrison, Sinha, Potenza), Child Study Center (Sinha, Potenza, Scheinost), and Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging (Lacadie, Scheinost), Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn.; Department of Neuroscience (Sinha, Potenza), Wu Tsai Institute (Potenza, Scheinost), Department of Biomedical Engineering (Gao, Liang, Scheinost), and Department of Statistics and Data Science (Scheinost), Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; Connecticut Mental Health Center, New Haven, Conn. (Potenza); Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling, Hartford, Conn. (Potenza).
Send correspondence to Dr. Garrison () and Dr. Scheinost ().

Supported by Mind and Life Institute grant A-36649338 to Dr. Garrison, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism grants R01-AA013892-15 to Dr. Sinha and RL1-AA017539 to Dr. Potenza, NIDA grants P50-DA016556 and PL1-DA024859 to Dr. Sinha and R01-DA039136 to Dr. Potenza, National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research grant UL1-DE019586 to Dr. Sinha, and NIMH grant R01-MH121095 to Dr. Scheinost.

Dr. Sinha is principal investigator for Yale research contracts with Aelis Pharma, Aptinyx, and CT Pharma. Dr. Potenza has served as a consultant or adviser for Addiction Policy Forum, AXA, Baria-Tek, Game Day Data, Idorsia, and Opiant/Lakelight Therapeutics and has served as a consultant for legal and gambling entities on issues related to impulse control disorders; he has been involved in a patent application with Novartis and Yale University; he has received unrestricted research support from the Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling and Mohegan Sun Casino; he has edited journals and journal sections and has given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events, and other clinical or scientific venues; and he has produced books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

REFERENCES

1. Kozlowski LT, Mann RE, Wilkinson DA, et al.: “Cravings” are ambiguous: ask about urges or desires. Addict Behav 1989; 14:443–445Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Drummond DC: Theories of drug craving, ancient and modern. Addiction 2001; 96:33–46Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Koob GF, Volkow ND: Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2016; 3:760–773Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Robinson TE, Berridge KC: The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1993; 18:247–291Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Bedi G, Preston KL, Epstein DH, et al.: Incubation of cue-induced cigarette craving during abstinence in human smokers. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 69:708–711Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Pickens CL, Airavaara M, Theberge F, et al.: Neurobiology of the incubation of drug craving. Trends Neurosci 2011; 34:411–420Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Yucel M, Oldenhof E, Ahmed SH, et al.: A transdiagnostic dimensional approach towards a neuropsychological assessment for addiction: an international Delphi consensus study. Addiction 2019; 114:1095–1109Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Meule A: The psychology of food cravings: the role of food deprivation. Curr Nutr Rep 2020; 9:251–257Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Tiffany ST, Wray JM: The clinical significance of drug craving. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2012; 1248:1–17Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Boswell RG, Kober H: Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review. Obes Rev 2016; 17:159–177Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Insel TR: The NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) project: precision medicine for psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:395–397LinkGoogle Scholar

12. Chase HW, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, et al.: The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70:785–793Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Kuhn S, Gallinat J: Common biology of craving across legal and illegal drugs: a quantitative meta-analysis of cue-reactivity brain response. Eur J Neurosci 2011; 33:1318–1326Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Hill-Bowen LD, Riedel MC, Poudel R, et al.: The cue-reactivity paradigm: an ensemble of networks driving attention and cognition when viewing drug-related and natural-reward stimuli. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 130:201–213CrossrefGoogle Scholar

15. Rosenberg MD, Casey BJ, Holmes AJ: Prediction complements explanation in understanding the developing brain. Nat Commun 2018; 9:589Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Shen X, Finn ES, Scheinost D, et al.: Using connectome-based predictive modeling to predict individual behavior from brain connectivity. Nat Protoc 2017; 12:506–518Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Yarkoni T, Westfall J: Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: lessons from machine learning. Perspect Psychol Sci 2017; 12:1100–1122Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Whelan R, Garavan H: When optimism hurts: inflated predictions in psychiatric neuroimaging. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75:746–748Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Yip SW, Scheinost D, Potenza MN, et al.: Connectome-based prediction of cocaine abstinence. Am J Psychiatry 2019; 176:156–164LinkGoogle Scholar

20. Lichenstein SD, Scheinost D, Potenza MN, et al.: Dissociable neural substrates of opioid and cocaine use identified via connectome-based modelling. Mol Psychiatry 2021; 26:4383–4393Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Yip SW, Carroll K, Potenza MN: An overview of translational approaches to the treatment of addictions, in Neuroimaging and Psychosocial Addiction Treatment: An Integrative Guide for Researchers and Clinicians. Edited by Feldstein Ewing SW, Witkiewitz K, Filbey FM. Palgrave, 2015 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

22. Garavan H, Brennan KL, Hester R, et al.: The neurobiology of successful abstinence. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2013; 23:668–674Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Lerman C, Gu H, Loughead J, et al.: Large-scale brain network coupling predicts acute nicotine abstinence effects on craving and cognitive function. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71:523–530Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Janes AC, Datko M, Roy A, et al.: Quitting starts in the brain: a randomized controlled trial of app-based mindfulness shows decreases in neural responses to smoking cues that predict reductions in smoking. Neuropsychopharmacology 2019; 44:1631–1638Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Barron DS, Gao S, Dadashkarimi J, et al.: Transdiagnostic, connectome-based prediction of memory constructs across psychiatric disorders. Cereb Cortex 2021; 31:2523–2533Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Gao S, Greene AS, Constable RT, et al.: Combining multiple connectomes improves predictive modeling of phenotypic measures. Neuroimage 2019; 201:116038Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Jiang R, Zuo N, Ford JM, et al.: Task-induced brain connectivity promotes the detection of individual differences in brain-behavior relationships. NeuroImage 2020; 207:116370Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Tomova L, Wang KL, Thompson T, et al.: Acute social isolation evokes midbrain craving responses similar to hunger. Nat Neurosci 2020; 23:1597–1605Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Naqvi NH, Bechara A: The hidden island of addiction: the insula. Trends Neurosci 2009; 32:56–67Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Menon V: Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci 2011; 15:483–506Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Ersche KD, Meng C, Ziauddeen H, et al.: Brain networks underlying vulnerability and resilience to drug addiction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2020; 117:15253–15261Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Sutherland MT, McHugh MJ, Pariyadath V, et al.: Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: lessons learned and a road ahead. Neuroimage 2012; 62:2281–2295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, et al.: Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods 2011; 8:665–670Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, et al.: Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106:13040–13045Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL: The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease, in The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2008. Edited by Kingstone A, Miller MB. Malden, Mass, Blackwell, 2008, pp 1–38 Google Scholar

36. Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V: A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008; 105:12569–12574Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM: Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2012; 8:49–76Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Zhang R, Volkow ND: Brain default-mode network dysfunction in addiction. Neuroimage 2019; 200:313–331Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. García-García I, Horstmann A, Jurado MA, et al.: Reward processing in obesity, substance addiction, and non-substance addiction. Obes Rev 2014; 15:853–869Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Schneider F, Habel U, Wagner M, et al.: Subcortical correlates of craving in recently abstinent alcoholic patients. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1075–1083LinkGoogle Scholar

41. Ravichandran S, Bhatt RR, Pandit B, et al.: Alterations in reward network functional connectivity are associated with increased food addiction in obese individuals. Sci Rep 2021; 11:3386Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, et al.: Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:748–751LinkGoogle Scholar

43. Sui J, Jiang R, Bustillo J, et al.: Neuroimaging-based individualized prediction of cognition and behavior for mental disorders and health: methods and promises. Biol Psychiatry 2020; 88:818–828Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44. Tiffany ST, Warthen MW, Goedeker KC: The functional significance of craving in nicotine dependence. Nebr Symp Motiv 2009; 55:171–197Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45. Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Duka T: The associative basis of cue-elicited drug taking in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010; 208:337–351Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46. Tiffany ST, Cox LS, Elash CA: Effects of transdermal nicotine patches on abstinence-induced and cue-elicited craving in cigarette smokers. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68:233–240Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47. McClernon FJ, Hiott FB, Huettel SA, et al.: Abstinence-induced changes in self-report craving correlate with event-related fMRI responses to smoking cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005; 30:1940–1947Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48. Zhang J, Kucyi A, Raya J, et al.: What have we really learned from functional connectivity in clinical populations? Neuroimage 2021; 242:118466Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49. van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O: A cross-disorder connectome landscape of brain dysconnectivity. Nat Rev Neurosci 2019; 20:435–446Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50. Cole MW, Bassett DS, Power JD, et al.: Intrinsic and task-evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron 2014; 83:238–251Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51. Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, et al.: Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature 2022; 603:654–660Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52. Scheinost D, Dadashkarimi J, Finn ES, et al.: Functional connectivity during frustration: a preliminary study of predictive modeling of irritability in youth. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021; 46:1300–1306Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53. Li J, Kong R, Liegeois R, et al.: Global signal regression strengthens association between resting-state functional connectivity and behavior. Neuroimage 2019; 196:126–141Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54. Sayette MA, Shiffman S, Tiffany ST, et al.: The measurement of drug craving. Addiction 2000; 95(suppl 2):S189–S210Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55. Sayette MA: The role of craving in substance use disorders: theoretical and methodological issues. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2016; 12:407–433Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar