The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Summer Birth and Deficit Schizophrenia in Dumfries and Galloway, Southwestern Scotland

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: An association between deficit schizophrenia and summer birth has previously been reported. Confirmation of a separate risk factor for this group of patients is potentially important, but the number of subjects with deficit schizophrenia in previous reports has been small. This analysis used data from an epidemiological study of incident cases of psychosis to test the hypothesis that deficit schizophrenia is associated with summer birth. METHOD: Data were drawn from records for the first year of clinical contact for all new patients coming into treatment for psychosis in the region of Dumfries and Galloway, Scotland, from 1979 to 1998. Patients with schizophrenia were classified as having deficit (N=65) or nondeficit (N=277) schizophrenia. Time of birth in the deficit and nondeficit groups was compared, and time of birth in the deficit group was compared with that for all births in Dumfries and Galloway during the study period. RESULTS: The deficit schizophrenia group had an excess of summer births, compared to both the nondeficit schizophrenia group and all births in Dumfries and Galloway. The difference between the deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia groups remained significant after accounting for demographic characteristics and symptoms of disorganization and hallucinations plus delusions. A measure of negative symptoms (as opposed to deficit schizophrenia) was a weaker predictor of summer birth. CONCLUSIONS: This study confirmed an association between deficit schizophrenia and summer birth in the nontropical regions of the Northern Hemisphere. The existence of a risk factor for deficit but not nondeficit schizophrenia is also consistent with other evidence that the pathophysiology of deficit schizophrenia differs from that for other types of the disorder.

Risk factors are often important clues to the etiology of a disease. Some risk factors for schizophrenia relate to very early events in the patient’s gestation and delivery, such as maternal infections during pregnancy, the mother’s exposure to other significant stresses, or a complicated labor (17). These findings have influenced research in other areas of schizophrenia by emphasizing the importance of very early brain development.

Winter birth is a widely replicated risk factor for schizophrenia (8). Some studies have found that winter-born patients had distinguishing clinical or demographic features, including paranoid subtype in men and a generally more benign course of illness (916). The clinical characteristics of winter-born patients suggested that patients with deficit schizophrenia (17), who generally have a poor outcome (18, 19), were likely not to have an excess of winter births. In two study groups that were not epidemiological samples, we noted an excess of summer births in patients with deficit schizophrenia, which was confirmed in epidemiological studies of incident cases of psychosis (20, 21) and in two studies of case prevalence (22, 23).

The association between deficit schizophrenia and summer birth is of considerable theoretical interest, as confirmation of a distinctive risk factor would suggest that there are important differences in the etiology of deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. A difference in risk factors would not be surprising in light of other studies showing biological and clinical differences between deficit and nondeficit groups, including studies of structural and functional imaging, cognitive processing, course of illness, and treatment response (18, 19, 24–32).

An important limitation of the previous studies was the lack of a comparison with all births in the same region of study. In a further test of the hypothesis that deficit schizophrenia is associated with summer birth in nontropical regions of the Northern Hemisphere, we used data from an epidemiological study of incident cases of psychosis in Scotland (33). We also compared the monthly pattern of births in the deficit group to that of all births in the same region.

Method

Dumfries and Galloway is a geographically well-defined area in southwestern Scotland. It has a stable population of around 147,000, of whom 99.5% are Caucasian. Psychiatric services are provided through one government-sponsored agency, and there is little or no private health care.

All patients were identified who had come into contact with the psychiatric services of Dumfries and Galloway between the years 1979 and 1998 and who had been given a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia; schizoaffective disorder; delusional disorder; mania; drug-induced psychotic disorder; or acute, transient, or unspecified psychotic disorder. ICD-9 was used to code diagnoses from 1979 to March 1996, when the clinical services began to use ICD-10. Patients were excluded if they did not reside in Dumfries and Galloway at the time of first clinical contact. The list of possible subjects was derived from two sources: 1) data for all inpatients, held centrally in Edinburgh by the Information and Statistical Division of the Scottish Office, and 2) locally held registers of outpatients, domiciliary visits, and “after hours” referrals. This study was approved by the Dumfries and Galloway Health Board Research Ethics Committee.

The Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders (34) was completed by using case records, correspondence, and all medical, nursing, social work, and occupational therapy notes. The instrument is a symptom checklist with 90 items relating to mental status and psychiatric history. It has a glossary with scoring instructions for each item and was developed with case note review in mind.

Two raters (J.A. and G.M.) completed the Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders for each patient for the year after initial contact with psychiatric services. These summaries allowed comparison among patients on the basis of similar information, irrespective of year of contact. Case notes were examined in random order of date of contact, and raters were blind to the clinically coded diagnosis. There was good interrater reliability for the ICD diagnosis of schizophrenia (33). The Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders was also used to generate DSM-IV and Research Diagnostic Criteria (35) diagnoses by means of the associated computer algorithm OPCRIT, version 3.4 (34).

There were 464 patients with a first episode of psychosis during the study period. Patients with an OPCRIT-based DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, or psychosis not otherwise specified were included in the present analyses (36). A total of 342 patients met these criteria: 113 with schizophrenia, 89 with schizophreniform disorder, and 140 with psychosis not otherwise specified.

We developed the Proxy for the Deficit Syndrome (2023, 37–40) to identify patients with deficit schizophrenia in data sets in which the Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome (17) was not applied. The proxy was initially developed for use with instruments such as the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, which quantify the severity of symptoms on 6- and 7-point scales. In contrast, the Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders records symptoms on 2- or 3-point scales, and many of the items that could be used with the Proxy for the Deficit Syndrome had little variance. Consequently, we used an algorithm based on the criteria for deficit schizophrenia (17) to define the deficit and nondeficit groups. Patients were categorized as having deficit schizophrenia if they 1) had restricted and/or blunted affect (two separate items in the Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders) but 2) did not exhibit dysphoria (a separate item in the Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders) (17, 37, 41). As in the studies in which the Proxy for the Deficit Syndrome was used, we tested the validity of assignments to the deficit and nondeficit groups by comparing their features to those of groups defined by using the Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome. In the validity testing, the deficit group did not differ significantly from the nondeficit group in age or in the severity of delusions, hallucinations, or formal thought disorder but had more severe negative symptoms, less severe dysphoria, poorer premorbid function, and a more insidious onset of the disorder (18, 24, 38, 39). Another research group has used a similar approach to define a deficit group (42).

This approach yielded 65 deficit patients (19%) and 277 (81%) nondeficit patients. The validity testing largely confirmed that these groups had clinical features that were similar to those diagnosed by using the Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome. The validity was further supported by the similarity between the prevalence in this deficit group and the prevalence estimate in another epidemiological study of first-episode schizophrenia (43), in which structured interview data, rating scales filled out by the patient, and hospital records were reviewed to make the deficit or nondeficit categorizations.

To test the summer-birth hypothesis, we used logistic regression models with summer birth as the dependent variable. To make our results comparable to those of previous studies of summer birth and deficit schizophrenia (2023), we used four definitions of summer: May to August, June to August, June-July, and July-August. Positive psychotic symptoms were used as covariates because psychotic symptoms are important sources of secondary negative symptoms and may contribute to miscategorizations. Earlier factor analysis studies of the symptoms of schizophrenia provided the basis for combining hallucinations and delusions as one variable and for considering disorganization as a separate variable (4446). In the logistic regression procedures, the hallucination and delusion items in the Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders were recoded so that patients with a history of at least 1 month’s duration of either symptom were given a score of 2; those with a definite history of either symptom, but of a duration of less than 1 month, were given a score of 1; and those without a history of either symptom were given a score of 0. A single disorganization score was derived by using the same strategy for the items measuring positive formal thought disorder and incoherence. Age and gender were also included as independent variables. Six patients were excluded from the season-of-birth analysis: for two patients information on the month of birth was not available, and four patients were born in the Southern Hemisphere.

In previous studies (2023), negative symptoms were not related to summer birth. We examined the relationship of summer birth to restricted affect (the negative symptom used to delineate the deficit and nondeficit groups) using four logistic regression procedures, with restricted affect in the place of deficit/nondeficit categorization as an independent variable. The other independent variables discussed earlier were also used.

We also tested the hypothesis that the relative frequency of summer births in the deficit group differed from that among all births in Dumfries and Galloway, using chi-square analysis and the four definitions of summer. Patients in the incidence study were born between 1891 and 1982 (the average year of birth was 1949). However, data on all births in Dumfries and Galloway by month were not available for the period before 1974. We compared summer births of the deficit group to those among all births in the area in 1974, using data from the General Register Office for Scotland in Edinburgh.

Because the data for all births were restricted to 1974, there was a possibility that any difference between deficit births and all births was due to a secular trend and did not reflect a consistent association between risk of deficit schizophrenia and summer birth. To assess this issue, we examined the pattern of deficit versus all births within each of two epochs, defined by a point in 1958 that approximately divided the deficit group in half, i.e., we made a median split of the deficit group based on year of birth and compared deficit births to all births within each of the two epochs.

Because the percentage of subjects with a diagnosis of psychosis not otherwise specified was relatively high, we also examined the association of summer birth and deficit schizophrenia after excluding the subjects with psychosis not otherwise specified.

Results

In general, the differences and similarities of the deficit and nondeficit groups were similar to those of such groups identified by using the Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome. By definition, the deficit group had significantly greater blunted affect and significantly less severe dysphoria than the nondeficit group; in addition, the two groups did not differ significantly relative to hallucinations plus delusions, and the deficit group had a significantly greater percentage of male subjects than the nondeficit group. As noted earlier, the prevalence of deficit schizophrenia was also similar to that in another epidemiological study of incident cases of psychosis (43). Because the deficit group also had significantly more severe disorganization than the nondeficit group and was significantly younger (Table 1), these two variables were used as covariates in the logistic regression procedures.

Figure 1 shows the percentages of births that occurred in each month in the deficit and nondeficit groups and among all births. In the multivariate logistic regression analyses, the deficit group had a significant association with birth in June to August, June-July, and July-August (Table 2). More deficit than nondeficit patients were born from May to August, but this difference was not significant (Table 2). No other variable was significantly associated with summer birth in any of the regression models. In contrast, the negative symptom that was used to define the deficit group (restricted affect) was significantly associated with just one definition of summer, June-July (data available on request from authors).

The deficit group also had a significantly higher percentage of births in June to August and June-July, compared to all births in Dumfries and Galloway (Table 3). In the median split analysis, an excess of summer births was apparent in the deficit group, compared to all births, in each of the two epochs (data not shown).

When subjects with a diagnosis of psychosis not otherwise specified were excluded, an excess of summer births was still apparent in the deficit group (data not shown).

Discussion

In an epidemiological study of incident cases of psychosis in southwestern Scotland, we replicated the association of deficit schizophrenia with summer birth. This association was present within schizophrenia, i.e., in a comparison between deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia patients, after accounting for clinical and demographic features. In contrast, within schizophrenia, the negative symptom measure (as distinguished from the deficit categorization) had a weaker association with summer birth. The deficit schizophrenia group also had an excess of summer births in comparison to all births in the same region.

The validity of our results depends on the validity of the deficit and nondeficit categorizations. Although our current approach to distinguishing deficit and nondeficit groups was somewhat different from the approach we used previously, the characteristics of the two groups were similar to those of groups identified with the Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome and the Proxy for the Deficit Syndrome (1732). The association with summer birth remained after accounting for the variance due to other potentially confounding clinical and demographic factors (including two variables for which the deficit group did not have the desired results in the validity testing). Any miscategorizations due to this method seem more likely to result in a failure to find an association than in a false positive association.

Because we restricted our analyses to patients in their first year of treatment, the subjects included a relatively high percentage of patients with psychosis not otherwise specified. We restricted the data analyses to the first year to approximate an incident case sample and to avoid bias due to survival in the sense of biased mortality and in the sense of survival as a case, i.e., chronic illness. In addition, when patients with a diagnosis of psychosis not otherwise specified were excluded, an excess of summer births was still apparent in the deficit group (data not shown). The lack of follow-up, with confirmation of diagnoses, is a limitation of our study, and it is possible that changes in diagnoses might have changed our findings. However, the multiple replications of the summer-birth effect (2023, 47) suggest that this limitation did not create the association reported here.

Another limitation is that we did not have an ideal nonpatient comparison group, as the earliest year for which data on all births were available was 1974. In our first report on summer birth, there was also a suggestion of a difference between births in the deficit group and all births in the same region (20). We have also found a significant difference between deficit births and all births in an epidemiological nonpatient comparison group in data from the Epidemiological Catchment Area study, which was conducted in five centers in the United States (22), and in a study of incident cases of psychosis from Cantabria in northern Spain (47). The consistency of the findings of the present study with those of other studies suggests that the shortcomings of the comparison groups have not resulted in a false positive association.

A measure of negative symptoms had a weaker association with summer birth than did the deficit versus nondeficit categorization. Negative symptoms were also not associated with summer births in previous studies (2023). Negative symptoms and the deficit criteria define groups that overlap substantially but are not synonymous, and these two measures also have different correlates (19, 27, 37). The ability to detect an association—or in the case of the positive psychotic symptoms, a failure to detect an association—is limited by the reliability of the variables of interest. However, the consistency of the studies of carefully validated deficit groups and summer birth, despite the use of a variety of scales, suggests that this issue did not distort our results.

Other studies have also suggested that season of birth is related to clinical characteristics. A review of the literature on season and schizophrenia (9) concluded that, on average, winter-born schizophrenia patients have a relatively benign disease compared to those born at other times of the year. In two subsequent studies, winter birth was found to be associated with the paranoid subtype in men (10, 11), which is a relatively benign form of illness. The deficit syndrome is associated with a relatively severe form of illness (18, 19, 37); thus, our present results are consistent with the previous studies.

The replicated association between summer birth and deficit schizophrenia is not consistent with the interpretation that deficit/nondeficit differences are found because the deficit group is at the extreme end of a severity spectrum, i.e., that the deficit group has a more severe form of the same pathophysiology found in nondeficit schizophrenia. Deficit subjects do not have a more severe form of the winter-birth excess, which is associated with schizophrenia as a whole, but a different risk factor, i.e., summer birth. It is likely that the winter-birth effect is the one that has been noticed in previous studies of schizophrenia because the deficit versus nondeficit categorization has not been made and the prevalence of nondeficit schizophrenia is greater than that of deficit schizophrenia. Not only is the widely replicated winter-birth phenomenon probably restricted to the nondeficit group, but the effect size may be greater than previously thought.

The identification of a season-of-birth risk factor for separate subgroups of schizophrenia may have several implications. For instance, maternal exposure to certain viral infections, most often influenza, has been linked to the development of schizophrenia (4850). As infection appears to increase the risk of schizophrenia only if it occurs during the second trimester, and such infections are generally more prevalent during the winter, subjects with schizophrenia as a result of exposure to such infections may have an excess of spring or summer births. In future studies of viral infections and risk of schizophrenia, it would be desirable to examine deficit and nondeficit groups separately. Other proposed explanations of seasonal variation of births, besides seasonal variation in infections, include decreased exposure to sunshine, seasonal variations of exposure to environmental toxins, and seasonal variations in nutrition (51).

The apparent double dissociation between season of birth and deficit versus nondeficit categorization is consistent with the evidence that deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia have differences in pathophysiology (52). Use of the deficit versus nondeficit categorization in future studies may provide additional insight in the search for nongenetic risk factors for schizophrenia.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3

Received Oct. 3, 2000; revisions received June 4 and Nov. 20, 2001; accepted Feb. 28, 2002. From the Maryland Psychiatric Research Center and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland; the Academic Department of Psychological Medicine, Gartnavel Royal Hospital, Gartnavel, Scotland; and the Department of Clinical Research, Crichton Royal Hospital, Dumfries, Scotland. Address reprint requests to Dr. Kirkpatrick, Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, P.O. Box 21247, Baltimore, MD 21228; (e-mail). Supported in part by NIMH grants MH-40279 and MH-60487 and by the Stanley Foundation. The authors thank Robert McMahon, Ph.D., and Ann Summerfelt for statistical consultation.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. Month of Birth for Patients With Deficit and Nondeficit Schizophrenia Identified at First Contact With Psychiatric Services for Treatment of Psychosis and for All Births in Dumfries and Galloway, Scotland, 1979–1998a

aPatients with deficit schizophrenia had restricted and/or blunted affect but did not exhibit dysphoria, according to assessment with the Operational Checklist for Psychotic Disorders (34).

References

1. Brixey SN, Gallagher BJ III, McFalls JA Jr, Parmelee LF: Gestational and neonatal factors in the etiology of schizophrenia. J Clin Psychol 1993; 49:447-456Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Dalman C, Allebeck P, Cullberg J, Grunewald C, Koster M: Obstetric complications and the risk of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study of a national birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:234-240Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. McGrath J, Castle D: Does influenza cause schizophrenia? a five year review. Aust NZ J Psychiatry 1995; 29:23-31Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Mednick SA, Machon RA, Huttunen MO, Bonett D: Adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to an influenza epidemic. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:189-192Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Mednick SA, Huttunen MO, Machon RA: Prenatal influenza infections and adult schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1994; 20:263-267Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Myhrman A, Rantakallio P, Isohanni M, Jones P, Partanen U: Unwantedness of a pregnancy and schizophrenia in the child. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 169:637-640Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. O’Callaghan E, Sham PC, Takei N, Murray G, Glover G, Hare EH, Murray RM: The relationship of schizophrenic births to 16 infectious diseases. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165:353-356Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Torrey EF, Miller J, Rawlings R, Yolken RH: Seasonality of births in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a review of the literature. Schizophr Res 1997; 28:1-38Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Boyd JH, AE Pulver, Stewart W: Season of birth: schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Bull 1986; 12:173-186Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Hsieh HH, Khan MH, Atwal SS, Cheng SC: Seasons of birth and subtypes of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1987; 75:373-376Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Nasrallah HA, McCalley-Whitters M: Seasonality of birth in subtypes of chronic schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1984; 69:292-295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Zipursky RB, Schulz SC: Seasonality of birth and CT findings in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1987; 22:1288-1292Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Sacchetti E, Calzeroni A, Vita A, Terzi A, Pollastro F, Cazzullo CL: The brain damage hypothesis of the seasonality of births in schizophrenia and major affective disorders: evidence from computerised tomography. Br J Psychiatry 1992; 160:390-397Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Degreef G, Mukherjee S, Bilder R, Schnur D: Season of birth and CT scan findings in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 1988; 24:461-464Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Franzek E, Beckmann H: Season-of-birth effect reveals the existence of etiologically different groups of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1992; 32:375-378Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Ohlund LS, Ohman A, Ost LG, Lindstrom LH, Wieselgren IM: Electrodermal orienting response, maternal age, and season of birth in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1991; 36:223-232Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW, McKenney PD, Alphs LD, Carpenter WT Jr: The Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome: an instrument for research in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1989; 30:119-123Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Fenton WS, McGlashan TH: Antecedents, symptom progression, and long-term outcome of the deficit syndrome in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:351-356LinkGoogle Scholar

19. Tek C, Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW: A five-year followup study of deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2001; 49:253-260Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Kirkpatrick B, Ram R, Amador XF, Buchanan RW, McGlashan T, Tohen M, Bromet E: Summer birth and the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:1221-1226LinkGoogle Scholar

21. Kirkpatrick B, Castle D, Murray RM, Carpenter WT Jr: Risk factors for the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2000; 26:233-242Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Messias E, Kirkpatrick B: Summer birth and the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia in the Epidemiological Catchment Area study. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189:608-612Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Tek C, Kirkpatrick B, Kelly C, McCreadie RG: Summer birth and deficit schizophrenia in Nithsdale, Scotland. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189:613-617Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Buchanan RW, Kirkpatrick B, Heinrichs DW, Carpenter WT Jr: Clinical correlates of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:290-294LinkGoogle Scholar

25. Buchanan RW, Breier A, Kirkpatrick B, Elkashef A, Munson RC, Gellad F, Carpenter WT Jr: Structural abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:59-65LinkGoogle Scholar

26. Buchanan RW, Strauss ME, Kirkpatrick B, Holstein C, Breier A, Carpenter WT: Neuropsychological impairments in deficit vs nondeficit forms of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:804-811Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Fenton WS, McGlashan TH: Testing systems for assessment of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:179-184Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Heckers S, Goff D, Schacter DL, Savage CR, Fischman AJ, Alpert NM, Rauch SL: Functional imaging of memory retrieval in deficit vs nondeficit schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:1117-1123Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Arango C, Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW: Neurological signs and the heterogeneity of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:560-565LinkGoogle Scholar

30. Tamminga CA, Thaker GK, Buchanan R, Kirkpatrick B, Alphs LD, Chase TN, Carpenter WT: Limbic system abnormalities identified in schizophrenia using positron emission tomography with fluorodeoxyglucose and neocortical alterations with deficit syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:522-530Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Kopelowicz A, Zarate R, Tripodis K, Gonzalez V, Mintz J: Differential efficacy of olanzapine for deficit and nondeficit negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:987-993LinkGoogle Scholar

32. Kopelowicz A, Liberman RP, Mintz J, Zarate R: Comparison of efficacy of social skills training for deficit and nondeficit negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:424-425LinkGoogle Scholar

33. Allardyce J, Morrison G, Vanos J, Kelly J, Murray RM, McCreadie RG: Schizophrenia is not disappearing in south-west Scotland. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177:38-41Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. McGuffin P, Farmer A, Harvey I: A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in psychotic illness: development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:764-770Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research Diagnostic Criteria: rationale and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35:773-782Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Schwartz JE, Fennig S, Tanenberg-Karant M, Carlson G, Craig T, Galambos N, Lavelle J, Bromet EJ: Congruence of diagnoses 2 years after a first-admission diagnosis of psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:593-600Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW, Breier A, Carpenter WT Jr: Case identification and stability of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1993; 47:47-56Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Kirkpatrick B, Ram R, Bromet E: The deficit syndrome in the Suffolk County Mental Health Project. Schizophr Res 1996; 22:119-126Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Kirkpatrick B, Amador XF, Flaum M, Yale SA, Gorman JM, Carpenter WT Jr, Tohen M, McGlashan T: The deficit syndrome in the DSM-IV Field Trial, I: alcohol and other drug abuse. Schizophr Res 1996; 20:67-78Google Scholar

40. Kirkpatrick B, Amador XF, Yale SA, Bustillo JR, Buchanan RW, Tohen M: The deficit syndrome in the DSM-IV Field Trial, part II: depressive episodes and persecutory beliefs. Schizophr Res 1996; 20:79-90Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW, Breier A, Carpenter WT Jr: Depressive symptoms and the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994; 182:452-455Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Mayerhoff DI, Loebel AD, Alvir JM, Szymanski SR, Geisler SH, Borenstein M, Lieberman JA: The deficit state in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1417-1422LinkGoogle Scholar

43. Kirkpatrick B, Ross DE, Walsh D, Karkowski L, Kendler KS: Family characteristics of deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia in the Roscommon Family Study. Schizophr Res 2000; 45:57-64Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44. Liddle PF, Barnes TR, Morris D, Haque S: Three syndromes in chronic schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1989; Nov (7):119-122Google Scholar

45. Buchanan RW, Carpenter WT: Domains of psychopathology: an approach to the reduction of heterogeneity in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994; 182:193-204Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46. Andreasen NC, Arndt S, Alliger R, Miller D, Flaum M: Symptoms of schizophrenia: methods, meanings, and mechanisms. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:341-351Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47. Kirkpatrick B, Herrera Castanedo S, Varquez-Barquero JL: Summer birth and deficit schizophrenia: Cantabria, Spain. J Nerv Ment Dis (in press)Google Scholar

48. Brown AS: Prenatal infection and adult schizophrenia. Int J Ment Health 2001; 29:22-37Google Scholar

49. Bagalkote H, Pang D, Jones PB: Maternal influenza and schizophrenia in the offspring. Int J Ment Health 2000/2001:29:3-21Google Scholar

50. Brown AS, Cohen P, Harkavy-Friedman J, Babulas V, Malaspina D, Gorman JM, Susser ES: Prenatal rubella, premorbid abnormalities, and adult schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 49:473-486Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51. Welham J, Davies G, Auliciems A, McGrath J: Climate, geography and the search for candidate, nongenetic, risk factors for schizophrenia. Int J Mental Health 2000; 29:79-100CrossrefGoogle Scholar

52. Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW, Ross DE, Carpenter WT Jr: A separate disease within the syndrome of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:165-171Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar