The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
PerspectivesFull Access

Sensory Processing Dysfunction in the Personal Experience and Neuronal Machinery of Schizophrenia

Abstract

Sensory processing deficits, first investigated by Kraepelin and Bleuler as possible pathophysiological mechanisms in schizophrenia, are now being recharacterized in the context of our current understanding of the molecular and neurobiological brain mechanisms involved. The National Institute of Mental Health Research Domain Criteria position these deficits as intermediaries between molecular and cellular mechanisms and clinical symptoms of schizophrenia, such as hallucinations. The prepulse inhibition of startle responses by a weaker preceding tone, the inhibitory gating of response to paired sensory stimuli characterized using the auditory P50 evoked response, and the detection of slight deviations in patterns of sensory stimulation eliciting the cortical mismatch negativity potential demonstrate deficits in early sensory processing mechanisms, whose molecular and neurobiological bases are increasingly well understood. Deficits in sensory processing underlie more complex cognitive dysfunction and are in turn affected by higher-level cognitive difficulties. These deficits are now being used to identify genes involved in familial transmission of schizophrenia and to monitor potentially therapeutic drug effects for both treatment and prevention. This research also provides a clinical reminder that patients’ sensory perception of the surrounding world, even during treatment sessions, may differ considerably from others’ perceptions. A person’s ability to understand and interact effectively with the surrounding world ultimately depends on an underlying sensory experience of it.

When patients tell us that they are hearing voices or seeing visions, we often assume that these experiences are layered onto a perception of the world that is basically similar to our own. Yet patients report instead more globally distorted perceptions of their world, which underlie their psychotic symptoms. Elyn Saks thus describes the first day that she became psychotic (1):

One morning in class, I suddenly decided that I needed to get up, leave school, and walk home…. As I walked along, I began to notice that the colors and shapes of everything around me were becoming more intense. And, at some point, I began to realize that the houses I was passing were sending special messages to me: Look closely. You are special. You are especially bad. Look closely and ye shall find. There are many things you must see. See. See.

Her suddenly heightened perception of her familiar neighborhood quickly became delusional. We may easily overlook these basic perceptual disturbances in diagnostic interviews, yet they are a basis for the more bizarre distortions of reality that lead to clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia and other psychoses. Deficits in auditory and visual sensory processing may not fully explain patients’ psychotic experiences, but they are a paradigmatic example of research that has been guided by patients’ own descriptions of their experience of the world. Clinicians who have listened to their own patients describe similar experiences of sensory overload may find that their understanding of their patients’ experience of the world around them is sharpened (2).

Although both Kraepelin and Bleuler included descriptions of perceptual abnormalities in schizophrenia in their textbooks (3), the detailed methodology involved in their characterization often makes the research advances in their characterization obscure. The strategy of using simple stimuli—tones or light patterns—helps isolate specific mechanisms, but it can also leave the impression that only initial sensation is being tested. The processing of such simple stimuli is actually complex and involves both cortical and subcortical circuitry. Deficits in processing the simplest stimuli found in schizophrenia may underlie more complex hallucinations and delusions, as for Dr. Saks, in two ways: neuropsychologically, failure to register basic sensory information correctly makes poor decisions about it inevitable; and neurobiologically, the same neuronal mechanisms that register the information are utilized throughout the brain, so that deficits in neuronal mechanisms detected in sensory areas are likely to be present also in regions that have more complex executive functions. Accordingly, many of the neurotransmitter mechanisms most associated with schizophrenia, such as glutamate, GABA, dopamine, and acetylcholine, as well as their receptors, are widely distributed throughout brain, as are many of the associated risk genes, which suggests that their effects on brain function should be equally detectable across brain regions.

David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel similarly described their Nobel Prize research in vision (4):

To examine the workings of this visual pathway our strategy since the late 1950’s has been (in principle) simple. Beginning, say, with the fibers of the optic nerve, we record with microelectrodes from a single nerve fiber and try to find out how we can most effectively influence the firing by stimulating the retina with light….

There was a time, not so long ago, when one looked at the millions of neurons in the various layers of the cortex and wondered if anyone would ever have any idea of their function…. For the visual cortex the answer seems now to be known in broad outline: Particular stimuli turn neurons on or off; groups of neurons do indeed perform particular transformations. It seems reasonable to think that if the secrets of a few regions such as this one can be unlocked, other regions will also in time give up their secrets.

Elementary sensations, such as auditory detection thresholds or visual acuity, are generally considered to be normal in patients, reflecting integrity of the peripheral sensory apparatus. However, research like that of Hubel and Wiesel points to the many stages of processing that occur as sight and sound pass from the eyes and ears through successive brain regions before becoming recognizable as images and words. Much of this processing occurs outside the realm of conscious awareness, so unless it is specifically tested, patients will be unaware of their deficits. Moreover, because basic sensory systems have been extensively characterized not only in humans but also in animal models, patterns of deficit can be directly linked to underlying biology.

Over recent decades, both the nature of the deficits and their consequences for function have become increasingly clear. In this review, we highlight the important role played by sensory deficits in both the subjective and objective functioning of individuals with schizophrenia, as well as how an increasing focus on sensory dysfunction may improve functional outcomes for patients with schizophrenia and other severe neuropsychiatric disorders.

Deficits in sensory processing are most effectively demonstrated using neurophysiological measures, such as event-related potentials, which uniquely trace the flow of information from sense organs through brainstem and subcortical regions and into sensory and then higher cortical brain regions. Results of event-related-potential studies, however, are strongly corroborated by both behavioral and neuroimaging measures that provide additional information regarding the location and nature of underlying cortical impairments (Table 1).

TABLE 1. Sensory Processing Dysfunctions in Schizophreniaa

ModalityPatient ExperienceNeurocognitive DisturbanceNeurophysiological ParadigmsNeuroimaging ParadigmsNeurotransmitter MechanismsCandidate Genes
AuditoryNoises appear louder; misperception of sounds; hallucinations (6); reduced response to environmental change as a chronic adaptation (110)Decreased detection of target signals in digit-vigilance and digit-coding tasks; poor pitch perception; phonological reading deficits (74); amusia (121)Diminished PPI (7, 11) and P50/N100 sensory gating (10); reduced N100, MMN, and P300 amplitude (12, 71)Increased frontotemporal/thalamic activity during gating (14); reduced auditory activation to deviant auditory stimuli (68)Diminished nicotinic cholinergic and NMDAR glutamate activation of inhibitory interneurons; increased catecholaminergic sensitization of neuron responses (36, 37)CHRNA7, NRG1, COMT, DISC1 (122)
VisualObjects appear fragmented and distorted (102); decreased sensitivity to dim, rapidly presented, or moving objects (48)Reduced “closure” ability (98); impaired face emotion recognition (99); visual reading deficits (104, 105)Diminished visual P1 to low-spatial-frequency stimuli (89, 123, 94); diminished event-related desynchronization of ongoing rhythms (121)Reduced activation of low-spatial-frequency regions of visual cortex (91, 92)Nonlinear amplification failure in subcortical visual pathways; decreased glutamate NMDAR activation (90)DTNBP1 (124); NOS1 (125)

aPPI=prepulse inhibition; MMN=mismatch negativity; NMDAR=N-methyl-d-aspartate-type glutamate receptor.

TABLE 1. Sensory Processing Dysfunctions in Schizophreniaa

Enlarge table

Auditory Dysfunction in Schizophrenia

It is well known that humans are distinguished from other primates by our superior reasoning and problem-solving ability, both of which are related to a dramatic expansion in frontal brain regions as compared with other primates (Figure 1). It is less appreciated that an equally marked evolutionary expansion has occurred in the human auditory system (5). This expansion underlies not only our unique linguistic abilities but also our greatly expanded ability to appreciate pitch, rhythm, and other musical features relative to other primates. Like other recently evolving regions, the auditory cortex completes its maturation late in human development (5). Thus, like prefrontal regions, these systems may remain vulnerable to disruption even during late adolescence and early adulthood, when schizophrenia typically develops. In general, sensory systems play a dual role—first, to orient attention to critical regions and features of the environment, and second, to decode the information emanating from those regions to enable subsequent voluntary processing. In schizophrenia, both the attentional and informational roles of the sensory systems are impaired, contributing to the overall pattern of symptoms and neurocognitive deficits we frequently associate with the disorder.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. The Cortical Areas Responsible for Perception and Executive Function Have Both Experienced Especially Enhanced Expansion in the Evolution of the Human Braina

a Reprinted with permission, from Hill J, Inder T, Neil J, Dierker D, Harwell J, Van Essen D: Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107:13135–13140.

Normal Auditory Physiology

Most clinical tests of auditory function, such as audiological assessments and far-field brainstem potentials, are tests of the most peripheral portions of the system in the inner ear and the auditory nerve leading to the brainstem, which do not appear to be affected in schizophrenia (6). The auditory information from the inner ear is initially processed in brainstem relay neurons, primarily to separate tones and regulate sensitivity to high and low volumes and to orient the person to the location of sound (Figure 2). Auditory information then ascends through the medial geniculate nucleus to the auditory cerebral cortex, which plays a critical role in the decoding of simple auditory features such as pitch, intensity, and location, which are then elaborated in subsequent auditory regions into more complex auditory percepts such as language and tonality. Dysfunction within the cortical regions may thus contribute to the complex patterns of social and communicative disturbances seen in schizophrenia.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. P50 Sensory Gating and Prepulse Inhibition of Startle Share Common Circuits, for Which Most of the Neurons and Transmitters Have Been Identifieda

a Breaks in pathways indicate where some relay neurons in the brainstem and the many layers of processing in the neocortex have been omitted. Sound activates the auditory nerve in the ear, which is primarily processed by a pathway from the cochlear nucleus through the medial geniculate nucleus in the thalamus to the cerebral neocortex, from which the entorhinal cortex sends the information to hippocampus neurons via glutamate synapses. This pathway carries detailed auditory information that is eventually processed as words, music, and so on. (Figure 3 describes physiological deficits in the cerebral cortex itself in schizophrenia.) In the brainstem, a branch of the auditory pathway activates the nucleus of the lateral lemniscus, which then activates the reticular formation, which controls attentional activation in the brain. Reticular formation neurons activate the medial septal nucleus in the basal forebrain, which is the source of acetylcholine (Ach) to alpha-7 nicotinic receptors on inhibitory interneurons of the hippocampus (shown in black). These neurons are also activated by the entorhinal cortex via N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) type glutamate receptors. When paired sounds stimulate the brain, the first travels through the main auditory pathways to activate glutamate synapses on the hippocampal neurons, which process the information. The reticulo-septal-interneuron pathway is also activated, which initiates γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) presynaptic inhibition of the glutamate inputs to the hippocampus neurons, so that a second stimulus cannot excite them as strongly. This inhibitory sensory gating regulates the flow of information to the hippocampus to ensure that repetitive, less important information does not overwhelm the limited capacity of hippocampus neurons to process information into memory. Deficits in the inhibitory mechanism are common in schizophrenia. The top left inset demonstrates this physiology using the P50 auditory evoked response, partially generated by the hippocampal neurons and recorded from the scalp. Sounds are repeated at a 500-ms interval. The control subject inhibits most of the response to the second sound (dotted line), whereas the patient with schizophrenia does not. Startle occurs when loud sounds activate reticular formation neurons, which then activate motor neurons to produce startle reactions. A weaker tone preceding the loud sound activates a circuit from the hippocampus to nucleus accumbens neurons, which inhibit the startle response. Decreased inhibition of the hippocampal response and increased dopaminergic (DA) activation from the ventral tegmental area (VTA), converging on the nucleus accumbens, produces deficits in prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia. The nucleus accumbens neurons inhibit pedunculopontine neurons (not shown), which modulate the excitability of reticular formation neurons during startle. The bottom left inset shows the blink response recorded from the orbicularis oculi after a loud, startling sound. This startle response is partly inhibited in the control subject because of the weaker 20-ms prepulse tone preceding the startle sound by 60 ms. The schizophrenia startle response shows significantly less prepulse inhibition (PPI). (For further details on these circuits, see references 129131.)

Sensory and Sensorimotor Gating Dysfunction

Auditory gating relies on the interplay of auditory input and brainstem reticular formation function. Reticular neurons have rapidly habituating responses to repeated stimulation and thus form the brain’s initial sensory gating mechanism, which prevents other brain regions from being flooded with repeated and presumed less important sensory information, such as in a noisy environment. The habituation to repeated stimulation leaves the brain ready to respond to more novel and thus potentially more meaningful sensory stimuli.

The effect of the brainstem’s habituation is observed experimentally in two ways. First, a descending spinal pathway from the larger reticular neurons initiates the startle response. A weak tone initiates habituation of the reticular neurons’ response to a subsequent louder tone, leading to the phenomenon of prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle response (7, 8).

In parallel, projections from other reticular neurons ascend to the midbrain, where they contact cholinergic neurons in the medial septal nucleus, which in turn project to the hippocampus, thalamus, and neocortex. These cholinergic projections activate inhibitory interneurons in the hippocampus and other areas that modulate local excitatory glutamatergic neurotransmission (9). The hippocampus then inhibits response to less important information coming from the auditory cortex and other sensory regions, funneled to it through the superior temporal gyrus. In humans, the largest initial cerebral response to an auditory stimulus is a positive wave that occurs at 50 ms and is thus termed the P50 potential. The P50 response to the second of paired stimuli is decreased relative to the first in healthy persons, an example of inhibitory sensory gating (10).

Deficits in the integrity of both PPI and P50 gating in schizophrenia have been extensively replicated since they were first demonstrated, pointing to dysfunction even within these low-level circuits. The deficit in PPI is most prominent in schizophrenia when the weaker tone precedes the startling sound by 60 ms (7). The deficit is more pronounced in women, who normally show less inhibition than men. Patients with schizophrenia, either unmedicated or treated with most antipsychotic drugs, also have reduced inhibition of the P50 auditory evoked potential (see Figure 2). The inhibition of the second stimulus is maximal at 500 ms, later than the maximal PPI interval of 60 ms. In many patients, the two deficits do not occur together, and thus they represent distinct aspects of pathophysiology (11). The N100 also shows impaired gating in schizophrenia (12).

For both PPI and P50 gating, despite the origin of the effect in reticular brainstem neurons, the response engages an entire network of brain circuits—the dopaminergic circuits of the basal ganglia for PPI and the basal cholinergic nuclei and cortex for P50 gating (8, 9). Rodent models of P50 gating emphasize the response in the hippocampus, but sources for human P50 have been found in the superior temporal gyrus, and cortical rhythms are altered during the sensory gating paradigm, with a decrease in EEG beta waves (13). Deficits in the response to repeated sounds have also been localized to the hippocampus and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex by both dipole analysis and functional MRI (fMRI) (14, 15).

P50 inhibitory gating deficits in schizophrenia have been associated with the frequency and intensity of auditory hallucinations and with neurocognitive measures of decreased vigilance and attention, consistent with the role of inhibitory sensory gating in protecting the brain from being responsive to extraneous stimuli while it is trying to focus attention on a task (16, 17). The association between patients’ self-report of sensory phenomena and physiological measures is confounded by the patients’ psychopathology (18). Thus, several investigations in patients have not shown association between P50 inhibition and self-reports of sensory gating difficulties (19), but in schizotypal adults, deficiencies in P50 inhibition are associated with difficulties in “hearing everything at once” and with a sense of unreality, including perceptual anomalies and magical beliefs (20, 21). PPI deficits have been correlated with deficient social perception (22).

Genetic Endophenotypes

Inhibitory gating measures have also contributed significantly to the search for genes involved in schizophrenia. Deficits in both PPI and P50 gating are found in schizotypal individuals who have a family history of schizophrenia and in adolescents at high risk for schizophrenia (2325). As a complex genetic illness, schizophrenia has not been linked to a single major gene. However, these specific endophenotype measures, each thought to underlie one aspect of the genetic basis of schizophrenia, were hypothesized to have a closer relationship to particular elements of genetic risk (20).

In families affected with multiple cases of schizophrenia, the deficit in P50 gating segregates as an autosomal dominant trait and is linked to the CHRNA7 gene complex, which forms the α7-nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (26). Polymorphisms of CHRNA7 have been associated with PPI as well (27). Another related gene is NRG1, which forms neuregulin (28, 29). Neuregulin helps assemble both α-nicotinic and N-methyl-d-aspartate-type glutamate receptors (NMDARs) (30), and thus its association with both sensory processing deficits is not surprising. The α7-nicotinic receptor is found presynaptically on glutamate nerve terminals and postsynaptically on inhibitory interneurons (see Figure 2), particularly in the hippocampus and nucleus reticularis thalami (9). Thus, there is convergence between the genetic findings and the neurobiological model, corroborated by postmortem findings of decreased α7-nicotinic receptors in schizophrenia (31). As the scope of genomic analysis has increased, both deficits have been associated with entire networks of genetic variants in schizophrenia in the NIMH Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia study, which examines the relationships of these phenotypes to the cognitive and genetic aspects of schizophrenia (28).

Drug Development

Although schizophrenia cannot be reproduced in animals, mouse and rat analogues of PPI and P50 deficits can be produced. Increased dopaminergic neurotransmission from stimulants and psychotomimetic antagonists of NMDARs such as phencyclidine (PCP) induce the deficit pharmacologically (8). Deficits in CHRNA7 null mutant mice for P50 gating (32) and in NRG1 null mutants for PPI have also been reported (8). Intriguingly, the NRG1-induced deficits do not appear until adolescence (33), modeling the time course for onset of schizophrenia (34).

Animal models were first used to examine effects of nicotine and clozapine, with good corroboration in clinical observations (10). Cigarette smoking increases P50 inhibition and PPI in schizophrenia (35). Further animal and clinical investigation confirmed nicotine's activation of α7-nicotinic receptors as the mechanism of its effects on P50 inhibition (36, 37). Clozapine, unlike other antipsychotics, significantly normalizes P50 inhibition, through increased release of acetylcholine in the hippocampus and the resultant indirect activation of α7-nicotinic receptors on inhibitory interneurons (38).

Investigational therapeutics followed, to develop specific α7-nicotinic receptor agonists to enhance cognition. The initial proof of principle for several of the agents included the demonstration of normalization of P50 inhibition in schizophrenia (39). Results of human studies have been mixed, with some but not all showing enhancement of cognition and relief of negative symptoms (3941). Nevertheless, at least one compound is currently in late-stage clinical testing, with definitive results expected within the next several years (42).

Development

P50 deficits are already present in many newborns of parents with schizophrenia (Table 2) (43). In the perinatal period, α7-nicotinic receptors are critical to the development of cerebral inhibition. However, cholinergic inputs do not reach the hippocampus until shortly before birth. Therefore, they are not stimulated by synaptically released acetylcholine. Choline, in the millimolar concentrations found in amniotic fluid, activates α7-nicotinic receptors. Dietary supplementation of choline stimulates the development of cerebral inhibition as measured by P50 inhibition after birth (44). Deficits in development of P50 inhibition in newborns are correlated with the first appearance of attention deficit symptoms in later childhood (45).

TABLE 2. Development of Sensory Processing Dysfunctions

PeriodFetal or Newborn Offspring of Parent With SchizophreniaAdolescents at Risk for SchizophreniaSchizophrenia, Childhood and AdultUnaffected Adult RelativesSchizotypal Adults
Deficit in PPIIncreased incidence of poor startle habituationIncreased prevalenceIncreased prevalence, decreased with second-generation antipsychotics, smokingIncreased prevalenceIncreased prevalence
Deficit in P50 gatingIncreased incidenceIncreased prevalenceIncreased prevalence, decreased with clozapine and nicotinic agonistsIncreased prevalence, segregates as autosomal dominantIncreased prevalence, particularly with positive family history
Deficits in MMNIncreased prevalenceIncreased prevalence, unaffected by antipsychoticsVariable results (126, 127)Increased prevalence (127, 128)
Deficits in visual P1UnknownIncreased prevalence, unaffected by antipsychoticsIncreased prevalence

TABLE 2. Development of Sensory Processing Dysfunctions

Enlarge table

Deficits in Neocortical Discrimination of Auditory Stimuli

The brainstem-based gating mechanisms operate at a basic filtering level to enable people to operate effectively in noisy, stimulus-filled environments. Once information reaches the cerebral cortex, fine discrimination of differences is needed for the complex feature extraction necessary for uses like language and the more emotive features of sound, including voice tone and music. The primary auditory cortex contains different regions that support the extraction of these different features, including the localization of the source versus the content of sounds (46).

Sound Localization in Schizophrenia

One of the strongest clues to auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia is the presence of auditory hallucinations. Although neural substrates of hallucinations remain incompletely understood, it has been known since Penfield (47) that stimulation of auditory regions induces auditory hallucinations similar to those of schizophrenia. Consistent with this literature, auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia are increasingly tied to auditory cortex dysfunction. For example, hallucinations are accompanied by objective hyperactivity of left-sided auditory regions (48). Similarly, increased ability of the left hemisphere to localize sound in the right side of space is correlated with greater severity of hallucinations (49). Increased development of the superior longitudinal fasciculi white matter fiber tracts connecting the auditory areas in the temporal lobe with the prefrontal cortex is also correlated with greater severity of auditory hallucinations and provides a possible anatomical correlate (50).

Neither sound localization nor the cortical anatomical substrate is enhanced in schizophrenia, compared with healthy subjects, but their preservation in patients with more severe hallucinations compared with those without this symptom intensity may reflect the output of a relatively intact cerebral cortex that is attempting to analyze an unregulated stream of sensory information resulting from earlier gating dysfunctions and from specific synaptic dysfunction within the cortex itself. Consistent with this hypothesis, deficits in P50 gating and PPI are both associated with increased severity of auditory hallucinations (51, 52).

Tone Matching Ability

One of the most widely studied measures of cortical auditory dysfunction is the mismatch negativity (MMN), which is elicited whenever a stimulus differs from what is expected based on recent auditory experience. MMN is elicited most frequently in an auditory “oddball” task, in which a series of repetitive stimuli is interrupted infrequently by a physically deviant “oddball” stimulus. Deviant stimuli can differ on any of several physical dimensions, including pitch, duration, intensity, and location (53) (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Mismatch Negativity (MMN) Paradigm, Deficits in Schizophrenia, and Process in the NMDA Receptora

a Panel A is a schematic illustration of the MMN paradigm, showing repetitive standard stimuli along with stimuli that are deviant in frequency (pitch) or duration. Panel B shows the distribution of MMN deficits in schizophrenia patients compared with controls in response to duration, frequency, and intensity deviants; the inset shows the current flow when MMN is generated within the auditory cortex. Panel C is a schematic illustration of the MMN process in the NMDA receptor. Surface-recorded MMN activity reflects current flow through open, unblocked NMDA receptors on pyramidal neurons in the auditory cortex. In panel C1, NMDA receptors are normally blocked by Mg2+ at resting membrane potential. In panel C2, when a deviant stimulus is presented without prior standard stimuli, even though the channels open, the blockade prevents current flow. Brain responses therefore occur only through non-NMDA-type glutamate receptors. In panel C3, when repetitive standard stimuli are presented, they depolarize the membrane, leading to unblocking of the channel. In panel C4, once channels are unblocked, presentation of the deviant stimulus leads to NMDA-receptor-mediated current flow. (Panel B is reprinted from T. Friedman et al., “Differential relationships of mismatch negativity and visual P1 deficits to premorbid characteristics and functional outcome in schizophrenia,” Biological Psychiatry, volume 71, issue 6, pp. 521–529, Copyright 2012, with permission from Elsevier.)

MMN has been tied to impaired neurotransmission at NMDARs in both intracranial (54) and surface (55) recordings in nonhuman primates and in ketamine challenge studies in healthy human volunteers (3). Unlike PPI, MMN generation is little affected by either dopaminergic or serotonergic manipulations (56). However, as with P50 gating, ketamine-induced deficits in MMN generation are reversed by nicotinic agonists (57), possibly by presynaptic stimulation of nicotinic receptors on the glutamate nerve terminals (40, 58), with similar effects observed in schizophrenia (39). Other compounds that may reverse MMN deficits in schizophrenia include N-acetylcysteine, a precursor of the brain antioxidant glutathione (59), and d-serine, an endogenous NMDAR modulator (60).

Deficits in MMN generation were first reported in the early 1990s, and this finding has since been a widely replicated neurophysiological finding in schizophrenia (3, 6163) (Figure 3B). Deficits in MMN generation correlate highly with overall level of function (64) and age at which symptoms of the illness first appeared (65). Like P50 and PPI deficits, MMN deficits are heritable across the general population (66). In persons at high clinical risk for schizophrenia, MMN has emerged as a predictor of who will progress to schizophrenia and therefore may require the most intensive remediation (67). Although the majority of studies of auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia have used neurophysiological measures such as MMN, similar deficits may be detected using fMRI to evaluate response to stimulus change within auditory cortical regions (68).

In keeping with the continued maturation of the auditory cortex in adolescence, MMN impairments continue to develop during the initial stages of the illness (69) in parallel with progressive volume reduction in auditory brain regions (70). In addition to MMN deficits, patients with schizophrenia also show deficits in the generation of other cortical auditory responses, such as the auditory steady-state response to rapidly presented stimuli (71). Patients fail to entrain their neuronal response to the rhythm of repeatedly presented stimuli, and so make less efficient use of sensory processing resources (72).

On the behavioral level, deficits in MMN generation are associated with impairments of basic auditory discrimination, such as tone matching (3) and auditory spatial discrimination (49), both underappreciated aspects of the clinical features of schizophrenia. Important social information, including emotion and attitude, is conveyed by variation in vocal intonation (“prosody”). Consequently, impaired ability to detect vocal intonation may contribute significantly to impaired social function (73).

Deficits in tone matching contribute significantly to the ability to detect emotional prosody (74, 75), as well as to communications such as sarcasm that require the individual to appreciate what another person may be thinking (76, 77), a trait called “theory of mind,” a form of empathy. On standardized tests of musical ability, such as the Montreal Battery for Evaluation of Amusia (78), nearly 50% of schizophrenia patients show deficits, compared with only 10% of the general population (79).

Deficits in MMN generation, particularly to duration deviants, are also prominent in developmental dyslexia, reflecting impaired ability to perform phonological operations (i.e., “sounding out” words) required for successful reading (80). Recently, similar reductions in phonological reading ability have been observed in patients with established, but not prodromal, schizophrenia, which suggests that progressive reductions in auditory function during the peri-onset period may result in a significant regression in reading ability in schizophrenia from premorbid levels (81). Such findings may identify pathways through which deficits in sensory function lead to impaired functional outcome when schizophrenia itself develops later on.

Although neural circuitry underlying MMN generation has not been fully determined, specific properties of the NMDAR appear to play a key role. NMDARs are unique in that they are controlled by both the resting membrane potential of the postsynaptic cell and glutamate release from the presynaptic cell. Specifically, at resting membrane potential (approximately −65 mV), NMDARs are blocked by Mg, which binds to a site within the NMDA channel (Figure 3C). Thus, even if glutamate is released from presynaptic terminals in response to a stimulus, no current flows through NMDAR channels even though they are open.

A proposed model of MMN generation (Figure 3C) is that repetitive standard stimuli induce subthreshold depolarization of the resting membrane potential in a population of neurons sensitive to stimulus properties that differ from those of the standard stimuli, leading to unblocking of the channels even though the channels are still closed. Once receptors are unblocked, presentation of the deviant stimulus leads to opening of the channel. Current flow through the open, unblocked NMDAR channel on neurons in the auditory cortex leads to the generation of the surface MMN potential. Although this phenomenon can be studied best in humans by use of sensory stimulation and auditory evoked potentials, similar phenomena occur elsewhere in the brain and lead to the types of cognitive disturbances observed in schizophrenia.

Visual Sensory Systems

Although sensory studies in schizophrenia to date have focused mostly on auditory dysfunction, visual function is increasingly investigated as well. Like the auditory system, visual function in the brain is subserved by two separate pathways: a dorsal-stream “where” system, which is also termed “perception for action,” and a ventral-stream “what” system, which is also termed “perception for identification” (see Figure 1).

Basic Visual Function

The visual system begins in the retina and projects via the visual lateral geniculate nucleus to the cortex. Subcortical visual systems are divided into two main pathways (82). The magnocellular pathway consists of large neurons that rapidly conduct low-resolution visual information to the dorsal visual stream, and thus is most analogous to the brainstem pathways involved in rapid response to auditory stimuli, subserving “where.” By contrast, the parvocellular system consists of more numerous smaller neurons that conduct slower but higher-resolution information primarily to the ventral visual stream, subserving “what.”

Consistent with their different functions, magnocellular and parvocellular neurons have divergent physiological properties. For example, magnocellular neurons respond strongly even to dim and low-contrast stimuli, giving rise to our familiar ability to detect extremely dim objects in the near dark even if we cannot identify what they are. The rapid, nonlinear increase in the response of magnocellular neurons to increasing stimulus contrast (Figure 4A) has been tied to the physical properties of NMDARs, particularly their need for cooperative binding of two glutamate molecules for activation (see Figure 3C). Magnocellular neurons are also highly sensitive to moving or flickering objects, which accounts for the strong influence of such objects on our attentional systems. These properties depend heavily on function of NMDARs within both subcortical and cortical visual regions (83).

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4. Response Properties of Magnocellular and Parvocellular Neurons in Schizophreniaa

a Panel A is a schematic illustration of response properties of visual magnocellular and parvocellular neurons. Magnocellular neurons show a nonlinear saturation response pattern (see reference 132), whereas parvocellular neurons show a more linear response pattern. The nonlinear response pattern is thought to depend on properties of the NMDA receptor (see reference 83). Panels B and C show visual event-related-potential responses to low- and high-spatial-frequency stimuli in schizophrenia. Magnocellular and parvocellular pathways are differentially sensitive to low- versus high-spatial-frequency stimuli. Patients show preferential activation impairments in P1 response to low spatial frequencies (arrow), suggesting preferential magnocellular pathway dysfunction (see reference 90). Panel D is an illustration of the response deficit in schizophrenia. In response to low-spatial-frequency (LSF) stimuli, both patients and controls show a nonlinear response pattern with increasing stimulus contrast. In patients, however, the maximum amplitude of the response is lower, consistent with underlying NMDA receptor dysfunction (see reference 3). (Panels B and C are reprinted from P.D. Butler et al., “Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments,” Brain 2007, volume 130, issue 2, pp. 417–430, by permission of Oxford University Press.)

By contrast, parvocellular neurons are smaller and more numerous, and hence are far more able than magnocellular neurons to transmit fine details of the visual scene. The parvocellular system responds in a more graded fashion to shades of gray than the magnocellular system (Figure 4A) and is sensitive to color, permitting fine object recognition (3). Parvocellular neurons show more linear responses than magnocellular neurons, suggesting less engagement of NMDARs. Relative dysfunction of the systems, therefore, will depend on both the pathways involved and the stimulus paradigms used.

As with the auditory system, routine clinical visual testing, such as use of an eye chart, does not adequately assess potential disturbances in schizophrenia. Magnocellular function can be assessed only through use of specially designed stimuli or tasks, such as measures of contrast sensitivity to low-spatial-frequency stimuli or related neurophysiological measures. Furthermore, although we are consciously aware of events occurring in the ventral visual system, we are typically unaware of processing taking place within our dorsal stream. For example, we may turn our heads in response to a flickering object before we are consciously aware of it. A consequence of such unconscious processing is that patients experiencing dorsal stream dysfunction may be unaware of what they are missing and may not know that their subjective experience differs from that of other people.

Visual Deficits in Schizophrenia

Using the newly invented photographic shutter, Kraepelin and colleagues observed that dementia praecox patients showed severe deficits in detecting stimuli presented for brief intervals (20 ms), confirming earlier findings with a rotating striped drum apparatus (84). Nevertheless, modern studies of visual processing in schizophrenia were not conducted until the 1960s, when Holzman observed that both patients and their unaffected family members showed more variability of eye movement during eye tracking than did healthy volunteers (85, 86). Subsequent studies of visual backward masking (87) demonstrated that patients not only required more time to detect a target—a parameter termed “critical stimulus duration” (as in the earlier Kraepelin studies)—but also showed more sensitivity to masking of the stimulus by a later, more intense stimulus, suggesting impaired interplay between different visual pathways (88).

Since then, more specific paradigms for demonstrating visual dysfunction have been developed on the basis of improved understanding of magnocellular and parvocellular system function (63). Neurophysiological studies have demonstrated impaired generation of the visual P1 potential to stimuli biased toward the magnocellular visual system (65, 8991) (Figure 4B,C). Other potentially useful paradigms include steady-state visual evoked potentials (Figure 4D), contour integration, and coherent motion tasks (63). The fMRI literature on early visual deficits is more limited. Nevertheless, impaired activation has been observed in response to low-spatial-frequency stimulation (91, 92), along with visual behavioral deficits. Visual regions are often not assessed in studies of higher cognitive dysfunction; nevertheless, impaired activation of visual regions has been observed in meta-analyses of visual working memory tasks (93), supporting recent suggestions that low-level visual deficits cascade up the system to undermine higher-order cognitive impairments, as in the auditory system (3).

Overall, deficits in sensory response are observed even in the absence of the attentional impairments (94), which suggests that visual deficits are more likely a cause than a consequence of attentional dysfunction. As in the early Holzman studies, deficits in visual processing are consistently observed not only in schizophrenia patients but also in unaffected family members, which suggests that they represent a possible heritable vulnerability factor for the disorder (95).

Functional Implications

As with auditory deficits, visual dysfunction in schizophrenia may go undetected unless specifically evaluated. Nevertheless, as in the auditory system, low-level deficits may cascade up the system to produce higher-order cognitive impairments.

One critical function tied to the early visual system is the ability to “close” or complete fragmented images. This process of perceptual closure, often referred to as the “cat behind the Venetian blind effect,” depends on rapid transmission of low-resolution information through the magnocellular/dorsal stream visual system to the prefrontal cortex in advance of more detailed information reaching the ventral stream visual cortex via the parvocellular visual system (96, 97). In schizophrenia, this framing function is lost because of impaired transmission within the magnocellular visual system (98), leading not only to impaired ability to recognize fragmented objects but also to impaired ability to discriminate facial emotion (99) or to engage in tasks that require sustained attention to visual information (91, 100). Patients in the early stages of schizophrenia often experience visual illusions and feelings that images are fragmentary (101, 102). Loss of this framing function may serve as a physiological substrate for this common perceptual experience.

Similarly, the magnocellular visual system plays a critical role in organizing visual space during reading of connected passages of text, such as paragraphs (103, 104). Although single-word reading has been extensively studied in schizophrenia, studies of paragraph reading have only recently been initiated based on emerging “whole brain” theories of schizophrenia. As predicted, schizophrenia patients show severe deficits in passage reading but not single-word reading (105), and these deficits correlate with both behavioral (106, 107) and fMRI-based (108) assessment of visual magnocellular function. Consistent with other visual findings, visual reading deficits extend to individuals at clinical high risk for schizophrenia (81) as well as to unaffected family members (106).

Auditory-Visual Interaction

Given the parallel deficits in auditory and visual function, processes that rely on the synergistic interplay between these systems should be those most affected in schizophrenia. To date, these interactions have been studied mostly in regard to social cognition and, more recently, reading, where expected synergistic consequences have been observed.

In social cognition, subjects infer emotion from both auditory cues and facial expression. Like schizophrenia patients, individuals with congenital amusia show impaired auditory emotion recognition (79) but may compensate by using visual cues and higher-order cognitive reasoning. In schizophrenia, the auditory and visual emotion-processing deficits are correlated but independent (74), which suggests that the confluence of deficits contributes in parallel to the social disconnection of schizophrenia patients.

Similarly, with regard to reading, individuals with isolated visual (as opposed to phonological) deficits may compensate by becoming “auditory” rather than “visual” readers. In schizophrenia, the parallel deficits in auditory and visual processing combine to produce a “double deficit” pattern of dyslexia that leads to significant impairments and poor socioeconomic outcomes (81). Such relationships are observed even in individuals who do not suffer from schizophrenia. However, whereas developmental dyslexia occurs during school years, permitting early detection and remediation, schizophrenia-related dyslexia represents a regression from a higher level of function and thus may go undetected unless specifically assessed.

Deficits in Nonschizophrenia Disorders

Sensory dysfunction has been studied most extensively in the context of schizophrenia, but additional studies suggest that sensory deficits may also be present dimensionally across other neuropsychiatric disorders. For example, deficits in PPI appear in bipolar disorder (109) and Huntington’s disease (110), consistent with the hyperarousal often seen in these conditions. Similarly, P50 deficits may be associated with psychotic mania and childhood autism spectrum disorder (ASD), consistent with impairments in excitatory/inhibitory balance (111, 112). By contrast, MMN appears to be differentially impaired in schizophrenia relative to either bipolar disorder or depression (113). In higher-functioning ASD, alterations are found in the consistency, although not in the overall amplitude, of unimodal sensory response (114). Similarly, patients may have superior, rather than impaired, performance in basic unimodal functions such as pitch processing (115) and visual search capabilities (116) that are impaired in schizophrenia. ASD patients show particular deficits in multisensory facilitation, perhaps leading to a fragmented experience of the environment (117, 118). The National Institute of Mental Health has recently proposed the Research Domain Criteria initiative (119), which views neurocognitive deficits as being present dimensionally across neuropsychiatric disorders. Sensory processing measures are included in this initiative and may make appropriate targets for intervention across, as well as within, specific nosological entities.

Discussion

The cognitive dysfunction of schizophrenia encompasses a profound disturbance in sensory processing. As Hubel and Wiesel found in their investigations, the understanding of how sensory information is processed has proven to be a valuable strategy for identifying specific neuronal dysfunctions. Assessments of sensory dysfunction, including PPI, P50 inhibitory gating, auditory MMN, and visual P1, steady-state, and magnocellular responses reveal heritable neurobiological abnormalities in schizophrenia that include deficits in nicotinic cholinergic, NMDA glutamatergic, dopaminergic, and GABA-ergic synaptic mechanisms. Many of these abnormalities are manifest before the onset of clinical signs of psychosis. The synaptic mechanisms and associated genes are now targets for new drug development to improve cognitive function in schizophrenia, one of the disorder’s most limiting clinical features.

While research tools for assessing sensory dysfunction are not used in clinical practice, clinical observations of patients’ sensory function have a role in treatment. The first clinical report of chlorpromazine’s effects in schizophrenia included its effects on patients’ sensory perception of their environment, described by Delay and Deniker (120) as “apparent indifference or the slowing of responses to external stimuli,” in contrast to their baseline state of hyperexcitability. Clinicians treating acutely ill patients look for the subsidence of the hypervigilant state as an early therapeutic milestone. Both isolation in a quiet environment and antipsychotic drugs are used to achieve this initial therapeutic effect. Dr. Saks’s description of her hyperawareness of her neighborhood is an example of a personal experience of this phase of psychosis.

Patients are often disturbed by their sensory distortions, but they may not volunteer this information to clinicians, particularly if they are already developing paranoia. When the clinician explains that many patients find that colors may seem more intense, visual perception may be more fragmented, and sounds may seem too loud or too intrusive, patients are often relieved to learn that their situation is understood. These topics may be simpler to explore with patients than their more cognitively elaborated hallucinations and paranoid delusions. Clinicians must be aware as well that patients may be unable to detect the variations in tonality that are used to communicate information, such as emotion or even attitude (e.g., humor and sarcasm), or to process the visual information needed to correctly interpret facial expression, contributing to their difficulty in interpersonal and social interactions.

Because available first- and second-generation antidopaminergic antipsychotics do not affect all the mechanisms that are associated with sensory disturbance in schizophrenia, many aspects of patients’ sensory dysfunction will persist chronically. Persistent abnormalities in P50 gating are associated with impaired ability to focus attention on specific tasks, a cognitive deficit associated with poor psychosocial rehabilitation (16). MMN deficits are tied to impairments in both orientation to critical environmental events and detection of the sensory modulations that are used to communicate social information such as emotion or attitude by tone of voice (3). Deficits in visual function are tied to impaired facial discrimination and reading. The result is a loss of connection to people and the environment, especially during the chronic phases of illness. Clinicians and families, as well as patients themselves, who are aware of these impairments may be able to compensate for them by more explicit explanations of communications, with the realization that patients may easily misperceive even the most helpful intent.

From the Division of Experimental Therapeutics, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Medical Center, and the Program in Cognitive Neuroscience and Schizophrenia, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, New York; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Denver School of Medicine, Aurora.
Address correspondence to Dr. Javitt ().

Supported by NIH grants R01 MH49334 and P50 MH086385 to Dr. Javitt and a VA Medical Research Service award and grant P50 MH-086383 to Dr. Freedman.

Dr. Javitt has received research support or travel support from or served as a consultant for or adviser to Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, FORUM, Lundbeck, Omeros, Otsuka, Pfizer, Promentis, Roche, SK Biopharmaceuticals, Sunovion, and Takeda; he has intellectual property rights for use of glycine, d-serine, and glycine transport inhibitors in schizophrenia. Dr. Freedman has a patent through the Department of Veterans Affairs on CHRNA7 sequences; he derives no income from it.

References

1 Saks ER: The Center Cannot Hold. New York, Hyperion, 2007Google Scholar

2 Gottschalk LA, Haer JL, Bates DE: Effect of sensory overload on psychological state: changes in social alienation-personal disorganization and cognitive-intellectual impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972; 27:451–457Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Javitt DC: When doors of perception close: bottom-up models of disrupted cognition in schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2009; 5:249–275Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Hubel DH, Wiesel TN: Brain mechanisms of vision. Sci Am 1979; 241:150–162Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Hill J, Inder T, Neil J, Dierker D, Harwell J, Van Essen D: Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107:13135–13140Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 McKay CM, Headlam DM, Copolov DL: Central auditory processing in patients with auditory hallucinations. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:759–766LinkGoogle Scholar

7 Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Sprock J, Hsieh MH, Braff DL: Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:1325–1335Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Geyer MA, Krebs-Thomson K, Braff DL, Swerdlow NR: Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: a decade in review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 156:117–154Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Frazier CJ, Rollins YD, Breese CR, Leonard S, Freedman R, Dunwiddie TV: Acetylcholine activates an alpha-bungarotoxin-sensitive nicotinic current in rat hippocampal interneurons, but not pyramidal cells. J Neurosci 1998; 18:1187–1195Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Freedman R, Adler LE, Waldo MC, Pachtman E, Franks RD: Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in inhibitory pathways in schizophrenia: comparison of medicated and drug-free patients. Biol Psychiatry 1983; 18:537–551MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Braff DL, Light GA, Swerdlow NR: Prepulse inhibition and P50 suppression are both deficient but not correlated in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:1204–1207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Turetsky BI, Greenwood TA, Olincy A, Radant AD, Braff DL, Cadenhead KS, Dobie DJ, Freedman R, Green MF, Gur RE, Gur RC, Light GA, Mintz J, Nuechterlein KH, Schork NJ, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Calkins ME: Abnormal auditory N100 amplitude: a heritable endophenotype in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia probands. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64:1051–1059Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Smucny J, Wylie K, Rojas D, Stevens K, Olincy A, Kronberg E, Zheng L, Tregellas J: Evidence for gamma and beta sensory gating deficits as translational endophenotypes for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2013; 214:169–174Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Tregellas JR, Davalos DB, Rojas DC, Waldo MC, Gibson L, Wylie K, Du YP, Freedman R: Increased hemodynamic response in the hippocampus, thalamus, and prefrontal cortex during abnormal sensory gating in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2007; 92:262–272Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Mayer AR, Ruhl D, Merideth F, Ling J, Hanlon FM, Bustillo J, Cañive J: Functional imaging of the hemodynamic sensory gating response in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp 2013; 34:2302–2312Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Cullum CM, Harris JG, Waldo MC, Smernoff E, Madison A, Nagamoto HT, Griffith J, Adler LE, Freedman R: Neurophysiological and neuropsychological evidence for attentional dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1993; 10:131–141Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Smith AK, Edgar JC, Huang M, Lu BY, Thoma RJ, Hanlon FM, McHaffie G, Jones AP, Paz RD, Miller GA, Cañive JM: Cognitive abilities and 50- and 100-msec paired-click processes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:1264–1275LinkGoogle Scholar

18 Light GA, Braff DL: Do self-reports of perceptual anomalies reflect gating deficits in schizophrenia patients? Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:463–467Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Jin Y, Potkin SG, Patterson JV, Sandman CA, Hetrick WP, Bunney WEJr: Effects of P50 temporal variability on sensory gating in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1997; 70:71–81Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Greenwood TA, Light GA, Swerdlow NR, Radant AD, Braff DL: Association analysis of 94 candidate genes and schizophrenia-related endophenotypes. PLoS ONE 2012; 7:e29630Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Croft RJ, Lee A, Bertolot J, Gruzelier JH: Associations of P50 suppression and desensitization with perceptual and cognitive features of “unreality” in schizotypy. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50:441–446Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Wynn JK, Sergi MJ, Dawson ME, Schell AM, Green MF: Sensorimotor gating, orienting, and social perception in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005; 73:319–325Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Cadenhead KS, Light GA, Geyer MA, Braff DL: Sensory gating deficits assessed by the P50 event-related potential in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:55–59LinkGoogle Scholar

24 Myles-Worsley M, Ord L, Blailes F, Ngiralmau H, Freedman R: P50 sensory gating in adolescents from a Pacific island isolate with elevated risk for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55:663–667Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Brockhaus-Dumke A, Schultze-Lutter F, Mueller R, Tendolkar I, Bechdolf A, Pukrop R, Klosterkoetter J, Ruhrmann S: Sensory gating in schizophrenia: P50 and N100 gating in antipsychotic-free subjects at risk, first-episode, and chronic patients. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64:376–384Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Freedman R, Coon H, Myles-Worsley M, Orr-Urtreger A, Olincy A, Davis A, Polymeropoulos M, Holik J, Hopkins J, Hoff M, Rosenthal J, Waldo MC, Reimherr F, Wender P, Yaw J, Young DA, Breese CR, Adams C, Patterson D, Adler LE, Kruglyak L, Leonard S, Byerley W: Linkage of a neurophysiological deficit in schizophrenia to a chromosome 15 locus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997; 94:587–592Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Liu X, Hong X, Chan RC, Kong F, Peng Z, Wan X, Wang C, Cheng L: Association study of polymorphisms in the alpha 7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit and catechol-O-methyl transferase genes with sensory gating in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2013; 209:431–438Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Greenwood TA, Lazzeroni LC, Murray SS, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Green MF, Gur RE, Gur RC, Hardiman G, Kelsoe JR, Leonard S, Light GA, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A, Radant AD, Schork NJ, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Freedman R, Braff DL: Analysis of 94 candidate genes and 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168:930–946LinkGoogle Scholar

29 Kéri S, Beniczky S, Kelemen O: Suppression of the P50 evoked response and neuregulin 1-induced AKT phosphorylation in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:444–450LinkGoogle Scholar

30 Li B, Woo RS, Mei L, Malinow R: The neuregulin-1 receptor erbB4 controls glutamatergic synapse maturation and plasticity. Neuron 2007; 54:583–597Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Freedman R, Hall M, Adler LE, Leonard S: Evidence in postmortem brain tissue for decreased numbers of hippocampal nicotinic receptors in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1995; 38:22–33Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Adams CE, Yonchek JC, Schulz KM, Graw SL, Stitzel J, Teschke PU, Stevens KE: Reduced Chrna7 expression in mice is associated with decreases in hippocampal markers of inhibitory function: implications for neuropsychiatric diseases. Neuroscience 2012; 207:274–282Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Law AJ, Wang Y, Sei Y, O’Donnell P, Piantadosi P, Papaleo F, Straub RE, Huang W, Thomas CJ, Vakkalanka R, Besterman AD, Lipska BK, Hyde TM, Harrison PJ, Kleinman JE, Weinberger DR: Neuregulin 1-ErbB4-PI3K signaling in schizophrenia and phosphoinositide 3-kinase-p110δ inhibition as a potential therapeutic strategy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109:12165–12170Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Deakin IH, Law AJ, Oliver PL, Schwab MH, Nave KA, Harrison PJ, Bannerman DM: Behavioural characterization of neuregulin 1 type I overexpressing transgenic mice. Neuroreport 2009; 20:1523–1528Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Adler LE, Hoffer LD, Wiser A, Freedman R: Normalization of auditory physiology by cigarette smoking in schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1856–1861LinkGoogle Scholar

36 Olincy A, Freedman R: Nicotinic mechanisms in the treatment of psychotic disorders: a focus on the α7 nicotinic receptor. Handb Exp Pharmacol 2012; 213:211–232Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Miwa JM, Freedman R, Lester HA: Neural systems governed by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors: emerging hypotheses. Neuron 2011; 70:20–33Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Nagamoto HT, Adler LE, McRae KA, Huettl P, Cawthra E, Gerhardt G, Hea R, Griffith J: Auditory P50 in schizophrenics on clozapine: improved gating parallels clinical improvement and changes in plasma 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol. Neuropsychobiology 1999; 39:10–17Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Preskorn SH, Gawryl M, Dgetluck N, Palfreyman M, Bauer LO, Hilt DC: Normalizing effects of EVP-6124, an alpha-7 nicotinic partial agonist, on event-related potentials and cognition: a proof of concept, randomized trial in patients with schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Pract 2014; 20:12–24Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Freedman R, Olincy A, Buchanan RW, Harris JG, Gold JM, Johnson L, Allensworth D, Guzman-Bonilla A, Clement B, Ball MP, Kutnick J, Pender V, Martin LF, Stevens KE, Wagner BD, Zerbe GO, Soti F, Kem WR: Initial phase 2 trial of a nicotinic agonist in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:1040–1047LinkGoogle Scholar

41 Lieberman JA, Javitch JA, Moore H: Cholinergic agonists as novel treatments for schizophrenia: the promise of rational drug development for psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:931–936LinkGoogle Scholar

42 Preskorn SH: The role of proof of concept (POC) studies in drug development using the EVP-6124 POC study as an example. J Psychiatr Pract 2014; 20:59–60Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Hunter SK, Kisley MA, McCarthy L, Freedman R, Ross RG: Diminished cerebral inhibition in neonates associated with risk factors for schizophrenia: parental psychosis, maternal depression, and nicotine use. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37:1200–1208Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Ross RG, Hunter SK, McCarthy L, Beuler J, Hutchison AK, Wagner BD, Leonard S, Stevens KE, Freedman R: Perinatal choline effects on neonatal pathophysiology related to later schizophrenia risk. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170:290–298LinkGoogle Scholar

45 Hutchison AK, Hunter SK, Wagner BD, Calvin EA, Zerbe GO, Ross RG: Diminished INFANT P50 sensory gating predicts increased 40-month-old attention, anxiety/depression, and externalizing symptoms. J Atten Disord (Epub ahead of print, June 11, 2013)Google Scholar

46 Kaas JH, Hackett TA: “What” and “where” processing in auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:1045–1047Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Penfield W, Perot P: The brain’s record of auditory and visual experience: a final summary and discussion. Brain 1963; 86:595–696Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Kompus K, Westerhausen R, Hugdahl K: The “paradoxical” engagement of the primary auditory cortex in patients with auditory verbal hallucinations: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:3361–3369Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Perrin MA, Butler PD, DiCostanzo J, Forchelli G, Silipo G, Javitt DC: Spatial localization deficits and auditory cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2010; 124:161–168Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 Shergill SS, Kanaan RA, Chitnis XA, O’Daly O, Jones DK, Frangou S, Williams SC, Howard RJ, Barker GJ, Murray RM, McGuire P: A diffusion tensor imaging study of fasciculi in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:467–473LinkGoogle Scholar

51 Smith DM, Grant B, Fisher DJ, Borracci G, Labelle A, Knott VJ: Auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia correlate with P50 gating. Clin Neurophysiol 2013; 124:1329–1335Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Kumari V, Peters ER, Fannon D, Premkumar P, Aasen I, Cooke MA, Anilkumar AP, Kuipers E: Uncontrollable voices and their relationship to gating deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2008; 101:185–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 Näätänen R, Escera C: Mismatch negativity: clinical and other applications. Audiol Neurootol 2000; 5:105–110Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54 Javitt DC, Steinschneider M, Schroeder CE, Arezzo JC: Role of cortical N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity generation: implications for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93:11962–11967Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55 Gil-da-Costa R, Stoner GR, Fung R, Albright TD: Nonhuman primate model of schizophrenia using a noninvasive EEG method. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110:15425–15430Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 Leung S, Croft RJ, Guille V, Scholes K, O’Neill BV, Phan KL, Nathan PJ: Acute dopamine and/or serotonin depletion does not modulate mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy human participants. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010; 208:233–244Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Knott V, Shah D, Millar A, McIntosh J, Fisher D, Blais C, Ilivitsky V: Nicotine, auditory sensory memory, and sustained attention in a human ketamine model of schizophrenia: moderating influence of a hallucinatory trait. Front Pharmacol 2012; 3:172Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

58 Lin H, Hsu FC, Baumann BH, Coulter DA, Lynch DR: Cortical synaptic NMDA receptor deficits in α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor gene deletion models: implications for neuropsychiatric diseases. Neurobiol Dis 2014; 63:129–140Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

59 Lavoie S, Murray MM, Deppen P, Knyazeva MG, Berk M, Boulat O, Bovet P, Bush AI, Conus P, Copolov D, Fornari E, Meuli R, Solida A, Vianin P, Cuénod M, Buclin T, Do KQ: Glutathione precursor, N-acetyl-cysteine, improves mismatch negativity in schizophrenia patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33:2187–2199Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

60 Kantrowitz JT, Javitt DC: Amelioration of neurophysiological deficits in schizophrenia by high-dose d-serine: Relevance for clinical treatment development. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75(suppl 9):11SCrossrefGoogle Scholar

61 Umbricht D, Krljes S: Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2005; 76:1–23Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

62 Light GA, Swerdlow NR, Rissling AJ, Radant A, Sugar CA, Sprock J, Pela M, Geyer MA, Braff DL: Characterization of neurophysiologic and neurocognitive biomarkers for use in genomic and clinical outcome studies of schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 2012; 7:e39434Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

63 Green MF, Butler PD, Chen Y, Geyer MA, Silverstein S, Wynn JK, Yoon JH, Zemon V: Perception measurement in clinical trials of schizophrenia: promising paradigms from CNTRICS. Schizophr Bull 2009; 35:163–181Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

64 Light GA, Braff DL: Mismatch negativity deficits are associated with poor functioning in schizophrenia patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:127–136Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

65 Friedman T, Sehatpour P, Dias E, Perrin M, Javitt DC: Differential relationships of mismatch negativity and visual P1 deficits to premorbid characteristics and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 71:521–529Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66 Hall MH, Schulze K, Rijsdijk F, Kalidindi S, McDonald C, Bramon E, Murray RM, Sham P: Are auditory P300 and duration MMN heritable and putative endophenotypes of psychotic bipolar disorder? A Maudsley Bipolar Twin and Family Study. Psychol Med 2009; 39:1277–1287Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

67 Perez VB, Woods SW, Roach BJ, Ford JM, McGlashan TH, Srihari VH, Mathalon DH: Automatic auditory processing deficits in schizophrenia and clinical high-risk patients: forecasting psychosis risk with mismatch negativity. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75:459–469Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

68 Wible CG, Kubicki M, Yoo SS, Kacher DF, Salisbury DF, Anderson MC, Shenton ME, Hirayasu Y, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of auditory mismatch in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:938–943LinkGoogle Scholar

69 Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, Griggs CB, Bonner-Jackson A, McCarley RW: Mismatch negativity in chronic schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:686–694Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

70 Salisbury DF, Kuroki N, Kasai K, Shenton ME, McCarley RW: Progressive and interrelated functional and structural evidence of post-onset brain reduction in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:521–529Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

71 Javitt DC, Spencer KM, Thaker GK, Winterer G, Hajós M: Neurophysiological biomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Drug Discov 2008; 7:68–83Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

72 Lakatos P, Schroeder CE, Leitman DI, Javitt DC: Predictive suppression of cortical excitability and its deficit in schizophrenia. J Neurosci 2013; 33:11692–11702Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73 Brekke J, Kay DD, Lee KS, Green MF: Biosocial pathways to functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005; 80:213–225Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

74 Gold R, Butler P, Revheim N, Leitman DI, Hansen JA, Gur RC, Kantrowitz JT, Laukka P, Juslin PN, Silipo GS, Javitt DC: Auditory emotion recognition impairments in schizophrenia: relationship to acoustic features and cognition. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:424–432LinkGoogle Scholar

75 Leitman DI, Foxe JJ, Butler PD, Saperstein A, Revheim N, Javitt DC: Sensory contributions to impaired prosodic processing in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:56–61Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

76 Leitman DI, Ziwich R, Pasternak R, Javitt DC: Theory of mind (ToM) and counterfactuality deficits in schizophrenia: misperception or misinterpretation? Psychol Med 2006; 36:1075–1083Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

77 Kantrowitz JT, Hoptman MJ, Leitman DI, Silipo G, Javitt DC: The 5% difference: early sensory processing predicts sarcasm perception in schizophrenia and schizo-affective disorder. Psychol Med 2014; 44:25–36Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

78 Sloboda JA, Wise KJ, Peretz I: Quantifying tone deafness in the general population. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2005; 1060:255–261Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

79 Kantrowitz JT, Scaramello N, Jakubovitz A, Lehrfeld JM, Laukka P, Elfenbein HA, Silipo G, Javitt DC: Amusia and protolanguage impairments in schizophrenia. Psychol Med (Epub ahead of print, March 31, 2014)Google Scholar

80 Kujala T, Näätänen R: The mismatch negativity in evaluating central auditory dysfunction in dyslexia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2001; 25:535–543Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

81 Revheim N, Corcoran CM, Dias E, Hellmann E, Martinez A, Butler PD, Lehrfeld JM, DiCostanzo J, Albert J, Javitt DC: Reading deficits in schizophrenia and individuals at high clinical risk: relation to sensory function, course of illness, and psychosocial outcome. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:949–959LinkGoogle Scholar

82 Milner AD, Goodale MA: Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46:774–785Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

83 Nelson SB, Sur M: NMDA receptors in sensory information processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1992; 2:484–488Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

84 Busch A: Auffassungs- und Merkfähigkeit bei Dementia praecox [The power of comprehension and observation in dementia praecox], in Psychologische Arbeiten, vol 5, part 3. Edited by Kraepelin E. Leipzig, Wilhelm Engelmann, 1908, pp 293–337Google Scholar

85 Holzman PS: Assessment of perceptual functioning in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1972; 24:29–41CrossrefGoogle Scholar

86 Levy DL, Lajonchere CM, Dorogusker B, Min D, Lee S, Tartaglini A, Lieberman JA, Mendell NR: Quantitative characterization of eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2000; 42:171–185Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

87 Saccuzzo DP, Braff DL: Early information processing deficit in schizophrenia: new findings using schizophrenic subgroups and manic control subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981; 38:175–179Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

88 Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Mintz J: Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania, I: specifying a mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:939–944Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

89 Schechter I, Butler PD, Zemon VM, Revheim N, Saperstein AM, Jalbrzikowski M, Pasternak R, Silipo G, Javitt DC: Impairments in generation of early-stage transient visual evoked potentials to magno- and parvocellular-selective stimuli in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 2005; 116:2204–2215Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

90 Butler PD, Martinez A, Foxe JJ, Kim D, Zemon V, Silipo G, Mahoney J, Shpaner M, Jalbrzikowski M, Javitt DC: Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain 2007; 130:417–430Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

91 Martínez A, Hillyard SA, Bickel S, Dias EC, Butler PD, Javitt DC: Consequences of magnocellular dysfunction on processing attended information in schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex 2012; 22:1282–1293Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

92 Martínez A, Hillyard SA, Dias EC, Hagler DJJr, Butler PD, Guilfoyle DN, Jalbrzikowski M, Silipo G, Javitt DC: Magnocellular pathway impairment in schizophrenia: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 2008; 28:7492–7500Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

93 Minzenberg MJ, Laird AR, Thelen S, Carter CS, Glahn DC: Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:811–822Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

94 Luck SJ, Fuller RL, Braun EL, Robinson B, Summerfelt A, Gold JM: The speed of visual attention in schizophrenia: electrophysiological and behavioral evidence. Schizophr Res 2006; 85:174–195Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

95 Holzman PS: Eye movements and the search for the essence of schizophrenia. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2000; 31:350–356Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

96 Sehatpour P, Molholm S, Javitt DC, Foxe JJ: Spatiotemporal dynamics of human object recognition processing: an integrated high-density electrical mapping and functional imaging study of “closure” processes. Neuroimage 2006; 29:605–618Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

97 Sehatpour P, Molholm S, Schwartz TH, Mahoney JR, Mehta AD, Javitt DC, Stanton PK, Foxe JJ: A human intracranial study of long-range oscillatory coherence across a frontal-occipital-hippocampal brain network during visual object processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; 105:4399–4404Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

98 Sehatpour P, Dias EC, Butler PD, Revheim N, Guilfoyle DN, Foxe JJ, Javitt DC: Impaired visual object processing across an occipital-frontal-hippocampal brain network in schizophrenia: an integrated neuroimaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:772–782Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

99 Schneider F, Gur RC, Koch K, Backes V, Amunts K, Shah NJ, Bilker W, Gur RE, Habel U: Impairment in the specificity of emotion processing in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:442–447LinkGoogle Scholar

100 Dias EC, Butler PD, Hoptman MJ, Javitt DC: Early sensory contributions to contextual encoding deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68:654–664Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

101 McGhie A, Chapman J: Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. Br J Med Psychol 1961; 34:103–116Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

102 Klosterkötter J, Schultze-Lutter F, Ruhrmann S: Kraepelin and psychotic prodromal conditions. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2008; 258(suppl 2):74–84Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

103 Stein J: The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 2001; 7:12–36Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

104 Laycock R, Crewther SG: Towards an understanding of the role of the “magnocellular advantage” in fluent reading. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008; 32:1494–1506Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

105 Revheim N, Butler PD, Schechter I, Jalbrzikowski M, Silipo G, Javitt DC: Reading impairment and visual processing deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2006; 87:238–245Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

106 Roberts EO, Proudlock FA, Martin K, Reveley MA, Al-Uzri M, Gottlob I: Reading in schizophrenic subjects and their nonsymptomatic first-degree relatives. Schizophr Bull 2013; 39:896–907Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

107 Whitford V, O’Driscoll GA, Pack CC, Joober R, Malla A, Titone D: Reading impairments in schizophrenia relate to individual differences in phonological processing and oculomotor control: evidence from a gaze-contingent moving window paradigm. J Exp Psychol Gen 2013; 142:57–75Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

108 Martínez A, Revheim N, Butler PD, Guilfoyle DN, Dias EC, Javitt DC: Impaired magnocellular/dorsal stream activation predicts impaired reading ability in schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin 2012; 2:8–16Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

109 Giakoumaki SG, Roussos P, Rogdaki M, Karli C, Bitsios P, Frangou S: Evidence of disrupted prepulse inhibition in unaffected siblings of bipolar disorder patients. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:1418–1422Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

110 Swerdlow NR, Paulsen J, Braff DL, Butters N, Geyer MA, Swenson MR: Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1995; 58:192–200Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

111 Franks RD, Adler LE, Waldo MC, Alpert J, Freedman R: Neurophysiological studies of sensory gating in mania: comparison with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1983; 18:989–1005MedlineGoogle Scholar

112 Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA, Prokofyev AO, Nygren G, Gillberg C, Elam M: Sensory gating in young children with autism: relation to age, IQ, and EEG gamma oscillations. Neurosci Lett 2008; 434:218–223Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

113 Umbricht D, Koller R, Schmid L, Skrabo A, Grübel C, Huber T, Stassen H: How specific are deficits in mismatch negativity generation to schizophrenia? Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53:1120–1131Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

114 Dinstein I, Heeger DJ, Lorenzi L, Minshew NJ, Malach R, Behrmann M: Unreliable evoked responses in autism. Neuron 2012; 75:981–991Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

115 Ouimet T, Foster NE, Tryfon A, Hyde KL: Auditory-musical processing in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and brain imaging studies. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2012; 1252:325–331Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

116 Joseph RM, Keehn B, Connolly C, Wolfe JM, Horowitz TS: Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Dev Sci 2009; 12:1083–1096Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

117 Collignon O, Charbonneau G, Peters F, Nassim M, Lassonde M, Lepore F, Mottron L, Bertone A: Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism. Cortex 2013; 49:1704–1710Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

118 Brandwein AB, Foxe JJ, Butler JS, Russo NN, Altschuler TS, Gomes H, Molholm S: The development of multisensory integration in high-functioning autism: high-density electrical mapping and psychophysical measures reveal impairments in the processing of audiovisual inputs. Cereb Cortex 2013; 23:1329–1341Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

119 Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, Sanislow C, Wang P: Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:748–751LinkGoogle Scholar

120 Delay J, Deniker P: Neuroleptic effects of chlorpromazine in therapeutics of neuropsychiatry. J Clin Exp Psychopathol 1955; 16:104–112MedlineGoogle Scholar

121 Dias EC, Bickel S, Epstein ML, Sehatpour P, Javitt DC: Abnormal task modulation of oscillatory neural activity in schizophrenia. Front Psychol 2013; 4:540Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

122 Greenwood TA, Swerdlow NR, Gur RE, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Freedman R, Green MF, Gur RC, Lazzeroni LC, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A, Radant AD, Ray A, Schork NJ, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Sugar CA, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Light GA, Braff DL: Genome-wide linkage analyses of 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170:521–532LinkGoogle Scholar

123 Doniger GM, Foxe JJ, Murray MM, Higgins BA, Javitt DC: Impaired visual object recognition and dorsal/ventral stream interaction in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:1011–1020Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

124 Donohoe G, Morris DW, De Sanctis P, Magno E, Montesi JL, Garavan HP, Robertson IH, Javitt DC, Gill M, Corvin AP, Foxe JJ: Early visual processing deficits in dysbindin-associated schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 63:484–489Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

125 O’Donoghue T, Morris DW, Fahey C, Da Costa A, Foxe JJ, Hoerold D, Tropea D, Gill M, Corvin A, Donohoe G: A NOS1 variant implicated in cognitive performance influences evoked neural responses during a high density EEG study of early visual perception. Hum Brain Mapp 2012; 33:1202–1211Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

126 Michie PT, Innes-Brown H, Todd J, Jablensky AV: Duration mismatch negativity in biological relatives of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52:749–758Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

127 Elliot Hong L, Moran LV, Du X, O’Donnell P, Summerfelt A: Mismatch negativity and low frequency oscillations in schizophrenia families. Clin Neurophysiol 2012; 123:1980–1988Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

128 Niznikiewicz MA, Spencer KM, Dickey C, Voglmaier M, Seidman LJ, Shenton ME, McCarley RW: Abnormal pitch mismatch negativity in individuals with schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res 2009; 110:188–193Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

129 Bickford PC, Luntz-Leybman V, Freedman R: Auditory sensory gating in the rat hippocampus: modulation by brainstem activity. Brain Res 1993; 607:33–38Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

130 Miller CL, Freedman R: Medial septal neuron activity in relation to an auditory sensory gating paradigm. Neurosci 1993; 55:373–380Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

131 Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL: Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology 2001; 156:194–215Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

132 Purpura K, Kaplan E, Shapley RM: Background light and the contrast gain of primate P and M retinal ganglion cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1988; 85:4534–4537Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar