Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2020

Open Access 15-10-2018 | Original Article

You ≠ me: individual differences in the structure of social cognition

Auteurs: D. J. Shaw, K. Czekóová, C. R. Pennington, A. W. Qureshi, B. Špiláková, M. Salazar, M. Brázdil, T. Urbánek

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2020

Abstract

This study investigated the structure of social cognition, and how it is influenced by personality; specifically, how various socio-cognitive capabilities, and the pattern of inter-relationships and co-dependencies among them differ between divergent personality styles. To measure social cognition, a large non-clinical sample (n = 290) undertook an extensive battery of self-report and performance-based measures of visual perspective taking, imitative tendencies, affective empathy, interoceptive accuracy, emotion regulation, and state affectivity. These same individuals then completed the Personality Styles and Disorders Inventory. Latent Profile Analysis revealed two dissociable personality profiles that exhibited contrasting cognitive and affective dispositions, and multivariate analyses indicated further that these profiles differed on measures of social cognition; individuals characterised by a flexible and adaptive personality profile expressed higher action orientation (emotion regulation) compared to those showing more inflexible tendencies, along with better visual perspective taking, superior interoceptive accuracy, less imitative tendencies, and lower personal distress and negativity. These characteristics point towards more efficient self-other distinction, and to higher cognitive control more generally. Moreover, low-level cognitive mechanisms served to mediate other higher level socio-emotional capabilities. Together, these findings elucidate the cognitive and affective underpinnings of individual differences in social behaviour, providing a data-driven model that should guide future research in this area.
Opmerkingen

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00426-018-1107-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Introduction

We take for granted the sophisticated repertoire of skills required to navigate our way through the myriad of social contexts encountered on a daily basis. Even in simple face-to-face interactions, for example, achieving a desired outcome requires us to infer the mental and emotional states of our interaction partner(s) whilst adapting our own behaviour accordingly. This broad set of cognitive mechanisms is referred to collectively as social cognition, and an extensive literature has revealed its highly nuanced and multifaceted composition (see Frith & Frith, 2012; Happé, Cook, & Bird, 2017). Despite this developing understanding, however, an overarching model of social cognition has not yet been specified; it remains unknown if and how its various sub-components are inter-related or co-dependent on one another, or how individual differences in this structure impact on social behaviour.
Research has delineated a variety of socio-cognitive and -emotional mechanisms that enable individuals to interact within social situations (see Frith & Frith, 2012). The conventional perspective on social cognition places each facet within a hierarchical network, in which lower level mechanisms contribute partly towards, or serve as necessary prerequisites for higher level processes (Happé et al., 2017). One such elementary mechanism is self-other distinction (SOD), through which we are able to treat independently and distinguish flexibly between representations of the self and others (Lamm, Bukowski, & Silani, 2016; Steinbeis, 2016). Inefficient SOD results in ego-centric misattributions of our own cognitive and affective states onto others, leading us to respond inappropriately in social interactions. This is true, especially when inferring our interaction partners’ perspective or understanding of a situation; since the perspective of others often conflicts with our own, appreciating another’s point of view requires us to detach ourselves from our own viewpoint (Epley, Keysar, van Boven, & Gilovich, 2004). Experimental tasks designed to measure perspective-taking attempt to tap into this capacity to resolve self-other conflict, and demonstrate the cognitive cost involved in SOD (Keysar, Barr, Balin, & Brauner, 2000; Samson et al., 2010). Recently, however, conflict resolution has been shown to be just one of two dimensions along which perspective-taking ability varies; compared with individuals reporting an ego-centric attentional focus during social interactions, those reporting more altercentrism or a greater flexibility in attentional focus between self and other exhibit a better perspective-taking performance (Bukowski & Samson, 2016). In this light, variations in attentional focus might underlie the reports of individual differences in perspective taking (e.g., Peterson, Bellows, & Peterson, 2015).
Self-other distinction is also believed to serve a fundamental role in imitation. Humans exhibit an involuntary tendency to mimic one another during social interaction, which serves important social functions—it enhances rapport and affiliation among interactants (Chartrand & Lakin, 2013; Leighton, Bird, Orsini, & Hayes, 2010). Neuroscientific investigations have revealed this involuntary behaviour results from a common neural coding (“mirroring”) of self- and other-action (Catmur, Walsh, & Heyes, 2009; Iacoboni et al., 2009). Since our own and others’ actions share a common representational space in the brain, controlling imitative tendencies requires SOD to differentiate between them (Brass, Ruby, & Spengler, 2009; Guzman et al., 2016; Steinbeis, 2016). Consistent with the notion of SOD providing a low-level mechanism common to both perspective taking and imitation, several studies have demonstrated that the expression of involuntary imitation is related inversely to perspective-taking performance (Santiesteban et al., 2012a; b; Shaw, Czekóová, & Porubanová, 2017; Spengler, Bird, & Brass, 2010). Interestingly, attentional focus also appears to exert an influence on imitative tendencies, since individuals scoring high on narcissism show lower involuntary imitation (Hogeveen & Obhi, 2013; Obhi, Hogeveen, Giacomin, & Jordan, 2014).
Neural mirroring mechanisms give rise to brain coupling between actor and observer, thereby affording inter-subjectivity. As such, similar mechanisms might contribute towards empathy—that is, an affective state elicited by observing or imagining another person experiencing an emotion, which is both isomorphic to the other’s state yet accompanied by the knowledge that the other person is the source of one’s own affective response (de Vignemont & Singer, 2006). Through neural mirroring, observing another’s emotions might activate our own affective brain systems, thereby allowing us to share in their affective state (Bastiaansen, Thioux, & Keysers, 2009; Lamm, Decety, & Singer, 2011). Recent models distinguish between an affective and cognitive component of empathy (e.g., Shamay-Tsoory, 2011; Shamay-Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Perry, 2009). Given conflicting taxonomies surrounding this dichotomy, we take the former to capture the process of affective sharing (or emotional contagion), and the latter analogous to emotion recognition—a propositional knowledge of the other’s emotional state (see Coll et al., 2017). Since empathic expression is considered a primary trait that drives individual differences in social behaviour (Baron-Cohen, 2009), it is important to elucidate its interdependences on other socio-cognitive and/or -emotional mechanisms that might exert a mediating influence (Kanske, Boeckler, & Singer, 2015). For example, SOD should be necessary to disambiguate between self- and other-affect representations during affective empathy/sharing; inadequate SOD might lead to self-other merging, resulting in personal distress when sharing in others’ negative affective states (Lamm et al., 2016).
Accurate processing and interpretation of affective states elicited during empathy also requires a mechanism of interoception, the sensory system through which afferent physiological signals of bodily states are communicated (Tsakiris & Critchley, 2016). Our sensitivity to internal physiological signals influences our subjective experience of emotions and the processing of affective states observed in others (Critchley & Garfinkel, 2017; Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, & Dolan, 2004). Indeed, some studies indicate that greater interoceptive accuracy is related to heightened empathic expression (Critchley & Harrison, 2013; Grynberg & Pollatos, 2015). Other studies have found no such relationship, however, suggesting that any link between interoception and empathy is complex, possibly involving other mediating mechanisms (e.g., Ainley, Maister, & Tsakiris, 2015). Those exhibiting high interoceptive accuracy also demonstrate a proficiency in regulating their emotions (Füstös, Gramann, Herbert, & Pollatus, 2012). Conversely, interoceptive hypervigilance is associated with the symptoms of anxiety and depression (Dunn et al., 2010; for a review see Domschke, Stevens, Pfleiderer, & Gerlach, 2010). The direction of this association appears to be mediated by self-referential processing (e.g., self-image; for a review, see Paulus & Stein, 2010). Given its potential influence on other components of social cognition, elucidating individual differences in interoception and its relationship with other aspects of social cognition is central to our understanding of social behaviour (Garfinkel et al., 2015).
Emotional reactions comprise a set of physiological changes that influence our behavioural response to environmental demands; when scared, we might recoil automatically, but, when angered, we might engage aggressively with the provocateur. Although such automatic behavioural reactions can serve important adaptive functions (i.e., fight-or-flight responses), emotion-driven behaviours are often ill-suited to social contexts and must be down-regulated (Gross, 2002). Suppressing impulsive emotion-driven behavioural responses requires cognitive control in the context of emotionally evocative stimuli (Tottenham et al., 2011). Emotion regulation, then, is another core component of social behaviour (Schipper & Peterman, 2013). Indeed, emotional dysregulation is observed in clinical syndromes characterised largely by atypical social behaviour, such as autism (Konstantareas & Stewart, 2006), conduct disorder (Davidson, Putnam, & Lawson, 2000), and borderline personality disorder (Fertuck, Lenzenweger, Clarkin, Hoermann, & Stanley, 2006). A number of potential mechanisms appear to underlie this; for example, research demonstrates that the ability to self-regulate emotional states is associated inversely with an individual’s general negative affectivity (Eisenberg et al., 1994; Okun, Shepard, & Eisenberg, 2000).
Given the combined influence of attentional focus (alter- versus ego-centrism), interoceptive sensitivity and state affectivity, and the capacity for emotional self-regulation, it follows that any discussion of individual differences in social cognition demands a consideration of personality. Very few studies have explored the links between social cognition and personality in the healthy population, however, and those that have report inconsistent findings; while reduced imitative tendencies are reported in narcissism (Hogeveen & Obhi 2013; Obhi et al., 2014), and the self-reported cognitive and affective empathy have been associated with agreeableness and conscientiousness (Melchers et al., 2016), the other studies have found no such relationships (Butler, Ward, & Ramsay, 2015). This contrasts with the findings of investigations into atypical social cognition in clinical samples; social cognitive dysfunction is observed in a number of personality disorders (Herpertz & Bertsch, 2014), and disorder severity is often related to empathic expression (Hengartner et al., 2014). Individuals with narcissistic personality disorder, for instance, exhibit deficits in affective but not cognitive empathy (Herpertz & Bertsch, 2014). Furthermore, patients with borderline personality disorder display impaired emotion regulation and high personal distress when empathising (New et al., 2012), together with poor perspective taking (Semerari et al., 2014; 2015). This may be a consequence of their ego-centric attentional focus (Frick, Lang, & Kotchoubey, 2012). Antisocial personality disorder is also characterised by ego-centrism, coupled with emotional detachment (for a comprehensive review, see Thoma et al., 2013). For this reason, Moroni et al. (2016) have proposed that personality style, rather than diagnostic category, gives rise to these atypical patterns of social cognition.
The sparse research that has investigated relationships between personality and social cognition in the healthy population has employed a variable-centred approach; studies have focused on the degree of relatedness between pre-defined stable personality traits (e.g., neuroticism) and behavioural measures across entire samples (e.g., Butler et al., 2015). The findings from such investigations provide no information on the organisation or dynamics of the mechanisms underlying an individual’s personality, however (Block, 2010; Kuhl, Kazén, & Koole, 2006). An alternative functional approach is offered by Personality System Interaction theory (PSI; Kuhl, 2000a, b), which considers personality to emerge through dynamic interplays between affective dispositions (e.g., sensitivity to negative affect) and the preference for certain modes of cognitive processing (e.g., analytical versus intuitive). In short, there are four sub-systems that interact to structure personality: intention memory (reason), extension memory (self), intuitive behavioural control (habits), and an object recognition system (mistake focus). Interactions between these systems, regulated by positive and negative affectivity, determine an individual’s personality style and, in turn, their behavioural pattern in social contexts. As such, personalities will differ substantially between people who are able to regulate their affective state flexibly according to task demands (action orientation) and those who ruminate in their emotions (state orientation). The PSI framework is conceptualised dimensionally; each dimension is measured on a continuum ranging from personality style to disorder (e.g., charming to histrionic), with disorders proposed to emerge as a result of inflexible (persistent) and maladaptive preferences for a certain style across different situations. This theory may, therefore, be advantageous when investigating individual differences in social cognition. Importantly, PSI theory is informed by neurobiological data (Baumann, Kuhl, & Kazén, 2005; see Kuhl & Quirin, 2011) and supported by behavioural evidence (Jostmann, & Koole, 2007; Kazén, Kuhl, & Quirin, 2015; Koole & Fockenberg, 2011; Quirin, Bode, & Kuhl, 2011). Furthermore, since the functional basis of relationships between affective and cognitive systems is the same for personality styles and disorders, PSI theory permits parallels to be drawn between healthy and clinical populations; it allows us to build upon the recent findings of specific impairments to social cognition in different personality disorders (Moroni et al., 2016; Semerari et al., 2014, 2015) by identifying subgroups of non-clinical individuals with similar profiles of personality style.
To advance our understanding of individual differences in the structure of social cognition, this investigation employed an analytical technique capable of capturing sample heterogeneity by classifying individuals into dissociable subgroups of personality styles defined by PSI. Differences among personality profiles were then explored with a large battery of self-report and performance-based measures employed frequently to assess discrete components of social cognition; namely, perspective taking, imitative tendencies, affective empathy, interoception, and emotion regulation. This allowed us to gain insight into the tapestry of inter-relationships and co-dependencies among these core components of social cognition in the typical population, and the potentially differentiating influence of personality.

Methods

Participants

Three hundred and seven right-handed individuals (122 males) from Brno, Czech Republic, participated in this study, but data from four of these individuals were omitted, because they represented multivariate outliers. Data from the remaining 303 individuals (122 males; Mage = 23.1 years, SE = 0.18) were used for personality assessment, with all individuals reporting normal or correct-to-normal eyesight and no neurological or psychiatric diagnoses. After excluding from this initial personality sample any individuals from whom we had incomplete data across all measures of social cognition (n = 13; see “Statistical Analyses”), further analyses were conducted on a sub-set of 290 individuals (117 males; Mage = 23.03 years, SE = 0.19). Sensitivity power analyses indicated that this sample size was sufficient to detect the smallest effect size of interest (r > .10) with 80% power, consistent with guidelines on individual differences (Gignac & Szodari, 2016). Comprehensive participant demographics are provided in Supplementary Table S1 (see Supplementary Material). The study was approved by the Ethics Board of the Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences in the Czech Republic. All individuals provided informed consent prior to their participation in the study, and were recompensed with 200 Kč (approx. €7.5).

Measures and procedure

Participants completed a personality assessment prior to their arrival in the laboratory. Individual facets of social cognition were then measured using a variety of computerised measures administered in Cogent (v1.31; http://​www.​vislab.​ucl.​ac.​uk/​cogent), a toolbox for MATLAB (vR2015b; The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA).

Personality

Personality was assessed with the Personality Styles and Disorders Inventory (PSDI; Kuhl & Kazén, 2002), a self-report instrument borne out of PSI theory. The PSDI comprises 140 items, each rated on a four-point Likert scale (0 = “Certainly no”, 3 = “Certainly yes”). Together, these items measure 14 personality dimensions, which are presented in Supplementary Table S2 along with reliability measures (all a > 0.64). Importantly, personality styles identified with this instrument differentiate on a variety of cognitive tasks (e.g., Urbánek, & Marček, 2016).

Imitative tendencies

Imitation was assessed with a stimulus–response compatibility task (Brass, Bekkering, & Prinz, 2001), whereby participants are required to execute finger-lifting actions in response to a coloured dot (imperative stimulus) while observing task-irrelevant finger actions performed by a model’s hand (stimulus hand). The degree to which participants are faster and more accurate at executing finger movements signalled by the imperative stimulus when they observe simultaneous compatible (matching) compared with incompatible (opposing) movements is referred to as automatic imitation, and is considered an experimental measure of spontaneous mimicry. Genschow et al. (2017) reported a high split-half reliability of 0.86 for this compatibility effect.
To avoid the confounding influences of anatomical, spatial- and orthogonal-compatibility effects (Shaw et al., 2017), the observed finger actions were performed by a left or right stimulus hand rotated, respectively, clockwise (+ 90°) or counter-clockwise (− 90°) from the participants’ perspective (see Supplementary Figure S1). Each trial began with the stimulus hand resting on a flat surface, signalling that participants should depress both the left and right directional arrows on a standard keyboard with the index and middle finger of their right hand, respectively. After 800, 1600, or 2400 ms, selected randomly, the warning stimulus was replaced with an end-point image of the same stimulus hand performing either an index or middle finger extension. In this end-point image, a dot was presented between the index and middle finger, the colour of which served as the imperative stimulus—it signalled whether the participant should extend their own index or middle finger (the colour-finger pairing was counterbalanced across participants). In response to the imperative stimulus, participants lifted the corresponding finger as quickly as possible, thereby releasing a key. The trial then ended with a blank screen presented for 1000 ms. The combination of imperative stimulus and stimulus hand resulted in four trial types: compatible (the same finger action was both signalled and observed), incompatible (opposite finger actions were signalled and observed), baseline (a movement was signalled, but the stimulus hand did not move a finger), and catch (no movement was signalled, but the stimulus hand moved a finger). The procedure comprised two blocks of 90 trials—30 compatible, 30 incompatible, 20 baseline, and 10 catch—with accuracy and response time (RT) measured on each trial. Each block presented either the + 90° or − 90° rotation of the stimulus hand, with the order of blocks counterbalanced across participants. Five practice trials were completed before the task began. To be consistent with the vast majority of stimulus–response paradigms employed to investigate automatic imitation and its relationship with other socio-cognitive variables (Cracco et al., 2018), we subtracted mean RT on the incompatible from the compatible condition. We refer to this herein as automatic imitation, with higher scores representing greater imitative tendencies.

Visual perspective taking

The director task (e.g., Keysar et al., 2000) was employed to measure low-level (level-one) visual perspective-taking (VPT). This task requires participants to move objects around a grid of shelves according to verbal instructions given by a ‘Director’. Sometimes, the grid display affords two competing perspectives; the participants’ viewpoint from the front of the display differs from the Director’s vantage point from the rear. Under these conditions, the participants must ignore their own self-perspective and act according to their inference of the Director’s perspective.
An example trial from each of the three conditions is illustrated in Supplementary Figure S1. On each trial, the stimulus consisted of an 8 × 8 grid of shelves forming 16 boxes, five of which had opaque backs. Various objects were placed within eight of these boxes, and the participant was presented with a recorded verbal instruction from the Director to move one of the objects to a different box. The Director was located behind the shelf display in all the conditions, which meant that she was unable to see the contents of the five opaque boxes—their contents were visible only from the participant’s (front) perspective. On experimental trials, the Director’s instruction referred to an object in one of the opaque boxes; in the illustrated example, the Director would instruct the participant to “move the smallest apple”. This created a discrepancy between the Director and participants’ perspectives, and to follow the instructions correctly on these trials, the participant had to discount any “distractor” objects not visible to the Director (e.g., they were to move the medium-sized apple rather than the smallest). In the first- and second-control conditions, there was no distractor object to discount; it was replaced in the first, and in the second, the Director’s instruction referred to an object in a box that they could see. Each condition comprised 20 trials presented pseudo-randomly. The recordings of verbal instructions were equivalent across all 60 trials [mean duration = 3.26 (SD = 0.22) s]. Participants responded by indicating with the mouse into which box the object should be moved, and accuracy and RT were measured on each trial.
In line with our previous research (Shaw et al., 2017), in which we revealed a relationship between VPT and automatic imitation using the exact same experimental procedures, we used a residualised measure of difference between the conditions: First, at the participant level, to account for any speed-accuracy trade-off, we calculated inverted efficiency scores in each condition by dividing the mean RT by the proportion of correct responses. This was justified, since RT and accuracy were correlated in all conditions (r > 0.12, p < 0.043; see Bruyer & Brysbaert, 2011). We then averaged inverted efficiency scores over the two control conditions. Finally, across the entire sample, we regressed these averaged scores across the control conditions from those in the experimental condition. This produced standardised residuals for each participant, with higher values representing poorer performance on trials, whereby their own perspective differed from the director’s relative to those in which no such discrepancy existed. To our knowledge, there has been no formal assessment of reliability for the director task, so we assessed the split-half reliability of our data from this performance measure. Both RTs and accuracy demonstrated excellent split-half reliability in each condition (> 0.96).

Interoception

We assessed interoception implicitly by measuring heartbeat estimation with the mental tracking method (Schandry, 1981)—a well-validated technique with the reports of test–retest reliability up to 0.81 (Knoll & Hodapp, 1992; Werner et al., 2013). This task measures the accuracy with which individuals count their own heartbeat, a measure that correlates highly with awareness of other bodily cues (e.g., gastric events; Herbert et al., 2012).
To ensure accurate measurements of heart rate, participants were instructed not to drink caffeinated drinks or smoke cigarettes 1 h prior to the experimental procedure. Prior to the task, participants were instructed to rest for 5 min, allowing their heart rate to return to resting levels. Participants then completed a heartbeat estimation task, which comprised a four block sequence of different time intervals (25, 35, 45, and 55 s) presented in a pseudo-random fashion, with inter-block intervals ranging between 10 and 30 s. Throughout all the blocks, participants wore noise-cancelling headphones through which white noise was played and were asked to close their eyes. Participants received the following the standardised instruction: ‘Without checking, can you count silently each heartbeat you feel in your body from the first to the second tone. Any kind of physical manipulation, such as holding your breath or evaluating your pulse, is not allowed’. After each block, the participant was asked to state verbally the number of heartbeats that they had counted in that interval.
Actual heartbeat was measured during each interval via the electrocardiogram (ECG), recorded with a PsychLab EEG8 amplifier unit (http://​www.​psychlab.​com/​EEG_​8_​amplifier.​html) sampling at 1000 Hz and Ag-AgCl surface electrodes positioned on the participant’s left and right wrist. The ECG data acquired during the task were analysed offline using a processing pipeline in MATLAB (http://​www.​librow.​com/​cases/​case-2), which involves a Fourier transformation to remove low-frequency drifts from the recording and the subsequent identification of local maxima considered to be R-peaks. Our measure of interoceptive accuracy was calculated according to the formula given by Garfinkel et al. (2015):
$${\text{1 }} - {\text{ }}\left( {|{\text{nbeat}}{{\text{s}}_{{\text{real}}}} - {\text{ nbeat}}{{\text{s}}_{{\text{reported}}}}|} \right)/\left( {\left( {{\text{nbeat}}{{\text{s}}_{{\text{real}}}}+{\text{nbeat}}{{\text{s}}_{{\text{reported}}}}} \right)/{\text{2}}} \right).$$

Emotion regulation

Implicit measure

Emotion regulation was measured implicitly with an emotional Go/No-Go task, which assesses individuals’ ability to maintain cognitive control over impulsive responses to emotionally evocative stimuli (e.g., Tottenham et al., 2011). The previous research using the Go/No-Go task reports good test–retest reliability (0.65; Weafer, Baggott, & de Wit, 2013).
This task comprised six blocks of 40 trials. In each block, a sequence of face stimuli was presented in rapid succession, each face showing one of two expressions. At the beginning of each block, participants were instructed to press the space bar as quickly as possible whenever a particular expression was presented. These “Go” trials occurred frequently (70%; 28) in a given block to instil a pre-potent tendency for the participant to respond. The remaining “No-Go” trials (30%; 12) presented participants with a different emotional expression. Participants were instructed to withhold the response for any facial expression other than the “Go” expression. The task comprised three emotional (angry, fearful, and happy expressions) and three non-emotional blocks (neutral expressions). In any given block, an emotional expression was always paired with a neutral expression; the emotional expression served as either the “Go” stimulus (with the neutral expression as the “No-Go” stimulus) or the “No-Go” stimulus (with the neutral expression serving as the “Go” stimulus). The blocks comprising these pairings were performed in counterbalanced order: Angry–neutral and neutral–angry; fearful–neutral and neutral–fearful; happy–neutral and neutral–happy. Each trial started with a fixation cross presented for 1000–2000 ms (M = 1500), followed by the face stimulus presented for 500 ms (see Supplementary Figure S1). The order of Go and No-Go trials was pseudo-randomised to ensure that no two No-Go trials occurred in succession. Prior to the task, participants performed a short practice block with a different stimulus set to that used in the experimental blocks. The face stimuli were selected from the Radboud Faces Database (14 males; Langner et al., 2010); each one grey scaled and cropped to remove any hair.
As a measure of emotion regulation, we extracted the proportion of trials in which the participant incorrectly responded to an emotional No-Go stimulus; that is, the false-alarm rate on emotional blocks (Tottenham et al., 2011). Following the approach taken by Tottenham et al. (2011), these values were z-scored by normalising to the sample standard deviation. Importantly, higher values on this measure represent poorer emotion regulation ability.

Explicit measure

To assess emotion regulation via self-report, participants completed the Action Control Scale (Kuhl, 1994), an instrument that measures an individual’s ability to regulate affective states quickly and flexibly in response to environmental demands (action orientation) rather than fixating on them in a change-preventing volitional mode that allows them to impact upon behaviour (state orientation).
This 36-item questionnaire consists of three sub-scales, each measured by 12 items: action orientation after failure versus preoccupation (AOF); demand-related action orientation versus hesitation (AOD); and action orientation during activity performance versus volatility (AOP). Each item presents an everyday situation (e.g., “When I am told that my work has been completely unsatisfactory”), and participants select one of two possibilities—one indicative of action orientation (“I don’t let it bother me for too long”) and the other of state orientation (“I feel paralyzed”). Since AOP is considered less relevant than AOF and AOD to the personality theory (see below; Kuhl, 1994), we focus only on the latter two dimensions herein. These two sub-scales achieved acceptable reliability (a > 0.74; see Supplementary Table S2).

Empathy

Implicit measure

To measure empathy implicitly, we developed a task that followed the same principles as the Multifaceted Empathy Test (Dziobek et al., 2008); a performance measure proven effective in dissociating between the cognitive and affective components of empathy at the behavioural and neurophysiological level, and in healthy and clinical samples (Mazza et al., 2014; Moore et al., 2015).
In our adaptation (see Czekóová et al., 2016), participants were presented with 30 colour photographs of individuals experiencing different emotions in various contexts. The task involves two blocks, with each photograph presented randomly in each block. In the first block, participants were required to select one of four options that best describes how they believe the person in the image is feeling (cognitive empathy). In the second block, they rated their own arousal in response to observing the emotion expressed by the person in the image (affective empathy) on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “None”, 7 = “Very strong”; affective empathy). Both accuracy and arousal ratings were recorded. In both blocks, each image was presented for a maximum of 10 s. Internal consistency was excellent for measures of affective empathy (Cronbach’s α = 0.84). Unlike our initial piloting, however, in which we achieved good levels of reliability for the measure of cognitive empathy (n = 112, α = 0.67), this measure achieved unacceptable reliability in the present sample (α = 0.47). For this reason, we focus only on the former measurement herein, referred to henceforth as affective empathy.

Explicit measure

We also obtained an explicit, self-report measure of empathy with the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1983), a multidimensional measure of individual differences in trait empathy shown to be valid and highly reliable across a range of populations pertinent to the present sample (mid- to late-adolescence [Hawk et al., 2013], various European countries [De Corte et al., 2007; Gilet et al., 2013], and experimental hypotheses [autistic individuals; Rogers et al., 2007]).
This 28-item instrument consists of four seven-item sub-scales that measure discrete empathic tendencies: Perspective taking (adopting spontaneously the psychological perspective of others), Fantasy (transposing oneself imaginatively into the feelings and actions of fictitious characters), Empathic concern (adopting “other-oriented” feelings of sympathy and concern), and Personal distress (having “self-oriented” feelings of personal anxiety and unease in tense interpersonal settings). The Perspective taking and Fantasy sub-scales’ index cognitive empathy, whilst Empathic concern and Personal distress reflect affective empathy. Participants indicated their answer for each item on a five-point Likert scale (1 = “Does not describe me well”, 5 = “Describes me very well”). Given that we were interested primarily in empathy as it unfolds during real-world social interactions, we focused only on perspective taking, empathic concern, and personal distress (for related discussions see De Corte et al., 2007; Su, Lee, Ding, & Comer, 2005) [see Supplementary Table S2 for the internal consistency of all three sub-scales (α > 0.73)].

State affectivity

To measure participants’ state affectivity at the time of testing, we employed the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (Quirin, Kazén, & Kuhl, 2009)—a task designed to measure implicitly an individual’s positive and negative affective state. This measure has been shown to have good test–retest reliability (> 0.72) and construct validity (Quirin et al., 2009; van der Ploeg et al., 2016), and to predict physiological indices of affectivity (e.g., cortisol levels) much better than direct self-report instruments (Quirin & Bode, 2014).
The test consisted of six artificial words (e.g., “SAFME” and “TALEP”) that participants were asked to rate in terms of the extent to which they conveyed six different mood states (happy, cheerful, energetic, helpless, tense, or inhibited), using a four-point Likert scale (1 = “Does not fit at all”, 4 = “Fits very well”). In a pseudo-random order, each non-word was presented six times alongside one of the six mood-state adjectives, resulting in 36 trials. Ratings across both positive and negative adjectives showed acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.71 and 0.66, respectively), and positive and negative affectivity scores were uncorrelated (r(288) = 0.08). Given its known relationship with the other measures of social cognition (Eisenberg et al., 1994; Okun et al., 2000), we focused our analyses only on scores of negative affectivity (referred to herein as negativity).

Statistical analyses

After the removal of four multivariate outliers across tasks, Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) was performed on the personality data from the initial sample of 303 individuals to identify distinct profiles of personality styles. Due to technical or performance failures, however, some of these behavioural measures were missing or incomplete for 13 participants (5 males). Since we were interested in data from individuals who performed all measures, Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) and Structural Equation Modelling (SEM) were performed on a final sample of 290 participants to compare personality profiles across each measure of social cognition, and the inter-relationships among them. Where measures violated the assumption of normality, non-parametric analyses were conducted. Values below present means (± SE).

Latent Profile Analysis

Latent Profile Analysis (Lazarsfeld & Henry, 1968) was conducted on PSDI data to classify participants into homogeneous sub-groups on the basis of preferred personality styles across all personality dimensions. LPA is a data-driven, model-based analytical technique that uncovers hidden groups within a sample of individuals, while taking into account uncertainty of group membership for each participant. Models with one to five latent profiles were calculated to identify the best-fitting model. The optimal model solution was determined on the basis of several fit indices, including Akaike Information Criteria (AIC; Akaike, 1974), Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), sample-size adjusted BIC, the Lo-Mendell-Rubin Adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test (LMRT; Lo, Mendell, & Rubin, 2001), the Parametric Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT; McLachlan & Peel, 2000), and entropy, as well as with respect to the previous research. The LMRT and BLRT reflect a significant improvement in model fit after comparing a given profile solution with a model that contains one less profile. Entropy summarises the degree of accuracy in classifying participants into discrete profiles on the basis of posterior probabilities, with higher values signifying a better profile separation (Ramaswamy, DeSarbo, Reibstein, & Robinson, 1993). While there is no general consensus on which fit indices should be preferred in case of their disagreement, the past research suggests that some indicators tend to perform better than others—BIC and sample-size adjusted BIC, for instance, have been shown to be more accurate in identification of correct solution compared to AIC, which tends to overestimate the number of profiles. Furthermore, the BLRT has been found to outperform LMRT as well as the rest of above-listed indices across various models and sample sizes (Nylund, Asparouhov & Muthén, 2007; Morgan, 2015). For this reason, we used maximal convergence among all fit indices to determine the optimal number of profiles.
The analyses were conducted in Mplus 7.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012), with 100 bootstrap samples drawn from all the models for BLRT.

Performance measure checks

The stimulus–response compatibility task successfully elicited automatic imitation; participants were faster to respond correctly to the imperative stimulus on compatible compared with incompatible trials [480.12 (± 3.71) vs. 487.96 (3.82) ms; Z(290) = 3.27, p = 0.001]. Accuracy was equivalent between the conditions; however, [94.79 (± 0.33) vs. 94.81 (± 0.34) ms; Z(290) = 0.48, p = 0.630].
When measuring VPT, participants were expectedly slower and less accurate on the experimental condition [4.98 (± 0.07) s, 81.28 (± 1.04) %] compared with control conditions one [4.94 (± 0.06) s, Z = 2.29, p = 0.022; 87.93 (± 0.95) %, Z = 9.57, p < 0.001] and two of the Director Task [4.88 (± 0.05) s, Z = 4.52, p < 0.001; 86.53 (± 0.92) %, Z = 7.76, p < 0.001].

Structural equation modelling

We conducted SEM to investigate directional relationships between our various measures of socio-cognitive and -emotional abilities, and the influence of personality. First, all pairwise plots were examined for nonlinearity and heteroscedasticity, and the distributions of the dependent variables were checked for multivariate outliers (c.f. Tabachnik & Fidell, 2013) before the variance of each variable was examined and modified as per Kline (2015). We then defined a default model—a reference against which any subsequent modifications suggested by the SEM analysis could be evaluated (e.g., addition or removal of directional paths and covariances). We employed a two-step process to arrive at a default structure objectively: first, we performed an MANOVA to identify variables that did and did not differ directly between the two personality classes. Having identified variables that did not differ, we then entered each into a mediation analysis to examine whether or not they differed between personality profiles indirectly via the influence of other measures. This approach follows contemporary recommendations that argue against the first condition of the causal steps approach (Baron & Kenny, 1986); specifically, the causal and outcome variable do not need to be correlated for the identification of important mediating influences (Hayes, 2013; Kenny & Judd, 2014; Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010). Only variables that differed significantly between the personality profiles and correlated with those that did not differ were considered as mediators, however, thereby satisfying the second and third conditions of the causal steps approach. Mediation analyses were conducted using ordinary least-squares path analysis (Hayes, 2013). Indirect effects were assessed with 1000 bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals to ensure that, for significant paths, these did not overlap zero.
Having defined a default model by combining all fully or partially mediated models, in a step-by-step manner, we implemented structural changes suggested by modification indices that decreased disparity in fit between the observed and modelled data; specifically, changes that resulted in significant decreases of χ2, AIC, and the BIC.

Results

All fit indices from the LPA suggested that a two-profile solution was optimal (see Supplementary Table S3). Figure 1 illustrates these two profiles of personality styles, with negative scores, indicating that a style is used infrequently and positive scores suggestive of a tendency (persistence) for that style. Latent profile 1 (P#1) comprised 187 participants (61.7%; 78 males), and is characterised by low scores on the Self-critical-Avoidant, Spontaneous-Borderline, and Passive-Depressive styles. Instead, individuals in this profile demonstrate a slight tendency for the Optimistic–Rhapsodic and Charming–Histrionic styles. Importantly, however, these style preferences are less strong than those shown by profile 2 (P#2), indicating less rigidity (greater flexibility). The 116 participants (38.3%; 44 males) comprising P#2 scored low on Optimistic–Rhapsodic and Charming–Histrionic personality styles but especially high on the Self-critical–Avoidant, Spontaneous–Borderline, and Passive–Depressive styles. Such strong scores on these personality styles indicate relative inflexibility. No significant differences between the profiles were found with respect to demographics such as sex, age, marital status, or completed education (see Supplementary Table S1). To check the reliability of the participant classification and profile characterisation, we performed two split-half validations; the sample was divided randomly into halves and a two-profile LPA was performed on each of the four sub-samples. Aside from a single instance of misclassification in one sub-sample, profile classifications were equivalent across all four iterations, as was the characterisation of the two profiles (see Supplementary Figure S2).
As shown in Table 1, an MANOVA revealed that mean scores on seven of our 11 measures of social cognition differed significantly between the two personality profiles. In line with the results of the LPA, individuals showing greater flexibility between styles (P#1) scored higher on both dimensions of action orientation (AOF and AOD), and lower on Personal distress and negativity relative to P#2. These individuals also reported higher Perspective taking, which showed superior interoceptive accuracy and exhibited considerably lower automatic imitation compared with P#2.
Table 1
Direct group comparisons
Measure
Profile means
F
p value
η p 2
 
#1
#2
   
AOF
6.22 (± 0.20)
3.51 (± 0.26)
70.44
< 0.001
0.20
AOD
6.69 (± 0.20)
4.15 (± 0.30)
62.16
< 0.001
0.18
PT
18.82 (± 0.33)
17.60 (± 0.43)
5.05
0.025
0.02
EC
18.85 (± 0.37)
19.60 (± 0.48)
1.53
0.217
0.01
PD
12.25 (± 0.30)
16.95 (± 0.38)
94.92
< 0.001
0.25
Negativity
2.25 (± 0.03)
2.36 (± 0.04)
5.51
0.020
0.02
VPT
− 0.05 (± 0.07)
0.08 (± 0.10)
1.01
0.316
< 0.01
ERFAR
− 0.11 (± 0.07)
0.05 (± 0.09)
2.10
0.148
< 0.01
EmpathyAff
21.88 (± 0.22)
21.26 (± 0.29)
3.01
0.084
0.01
INTAcc
0.50 (± 0.02)
0.422 (± 0.03)
3.96
0.048
0.01
IMIAuto
3.70 (± 3.00)
14.70 (± 3.87)
5.06
0.025
0.02
Values present means (± SE)
AOF Failure-related action orientation, AOD Demand-related action orientation, PT Perspective-taking, EC Empathic concern; PD Personal distress, VPT visual perspective taking, ERFAR emotion regulation, EmpathyAff. affective empathy, INTAcc. interoceptive accuracy, IMIAuto. automatic imitation
Mediation analyses were then conducted on the four measures that did not differ significantly between the personality profiles: Empathic concern, visual perspective taking (VPT), emotion regulation, and affective empathy. Supplementary Table S4 presents the correlations between these four measures and all other dependent variables used to define potential mediators. Four models emerged that revealed variables serving as significant mediators in the influence of personality on measures of social cognition: (1) interoceptive accuracy fully mediated the effect on emotion regulation; (2) negativity and automatic imitation fully mediated the influence on affective empathy; (3) self-reported Perspective taking fully mediated the effect on VPT; (4) self-reported Personal distress, Perspective taking, and AOF partially mediated the influence on self-reported Empathic concern. The direction of these mediating influences is presented below, along with a description of the SEM results. It is important to stress that the term “full mediation” is a statistical concept—it does not imply that only the modelled variables underpin an observed relationship (see Hayes, 2018).
When these separate mediation models were combined in an SEM analysis, modification indices suggested only three changes: the removal of a direct pathway from personality profile to VPT and two covariances—between AOF and self-reported Perspective taking, and AOF and Personal distress. The final model is presented in Fig. 2, which achieved excellent levels of fit with the observed data across discrepancy functions (χ2[37] = 37.32, p = 0.45; root-mean-square error of approximation = 0.008) and comparative indices (normalised fit index = 0.896; comparative fit index = 0.999; Tucker Lewis index = 0.998). Moving clockwise through this structure from top left, the first branch presents an indirect pathway through which interoceptive accuracy fully mediates the influence of personality on emotion regulation. The ab path (indirect effect) reveals how the two personality profiles are estimated to differ on emotion regulation (the outcome) as a result of the effect of personality on interoceptive accuracy (the mediator), which, in turn, affects emotion regulation (the b path; cf. Hayes, 2013, pp. 92). In other words, while emotion regulation did not significantly differ directly between the two profiles, P#2 did have significantly poorer interoceptive accuracy and, through the positive relationship between interoception and emotion regulation, this was associated with a better emotion regulation (lower false-alarm rate).
The second branch encompasses a multimediator model, whereby there is no direct effect of personality on affective empathy but two indirect effects involving negativity and automatic imitation. First, individuals in P#2 exhibit stronger negativity than those in P#1, and this is associated inversely with affective empathy. As such, those in P#2 express a more negative affective state and, consequently, are less aroused by the emotions of others. P#2 also exhibit stronger automatic imitation relative to P#1, and automatic imitation is related positively with affective empathy. Due to the positive influence of automatic imitation on affective empathy, when taking into account the difference between profiles, people in P#2 show higher affective empathy than those in P#1. This inconsistent mediation reveals the opposing influences of negative affectivity and imitative tendencies on affective empathy, which has the potential to mask any difference between personality profiles.
The bottom right-hand branch comprises a single indirect pathway; those in P#2 report lower self-reported Perspective taking, and this is associated inversely with VPT. Keeping in mind that higher scores on VPT reflect poorer performance (inverted efficiency), the indirect path reveals that P#2 perform worse on the Director Task as a result of their self-reported lower Perspective taking.
The final branch presents another multimediator model, but this time comprising sub-scales from the self-report instruments. Relative to P#1, P#2 express lower Perspective taking, and through the positive relationship between Perspective taking and Empathic concern, in turn, demonstrate lower Empathic concern. Conversely, P#2 also exhibit higher Personal distress and, in turn, this is positively related to Empathic concern compared with P#1. Finally, people categorised as P#2 report lower AOF, with this associated negatively with scores of Empathic concern. Due to the negative influence of AOF on Empathic concern, P#2 express increased Empathic concern relative to P#1.

Discussion

This study sought to elucidate the structure of inter-relationships and co-dependencies among discrete components of social cognition, and the personality mechanisms driving individual differences in this structure. From a large non-clinical sample, we acquired both performance-based and self-report measures of various socio-cognitive and -emotional capacities, and personality data with an instrument capable of delineating between personality style configurations. After profiling participants according to their personality style tendencies, we compared them on discrete facets of social cognition. Our results revealed three primary findings: (1) two dissociable personality profiles emerged that exhibited contrasting cognitive and affective dispositions; one showing flexibility among personality styles and adaptive action orientation, the other expressing persistence for certain styles and state orientation; (2) these profiles differed on a number of performance and self-report measures, revealing that mechanisms driving individual differences in personality also shape social cognition; (3) other facets of social cognition appear to mediate differences between these profiles in selected components, uncovering a hierarchical structure.
The two emerging profiles differed primarily in their preference for styles of Self-critical–Avoidant, Passive–Depressive, Spontaneous–Borderline, Optimistic–Rhapsodic, and Charming–Histrionic. According to the PSI theory, the lack of a strong preference across personality styles in Profile 1 (P#1) is indicative of greater flexibility and adaptation; individuals employing a larger repertoire of styles should be able to adjust to different situational demands more efficiently (Block, 2002; Kuhl, 2000a). In contrast, the strong tendency for Self-critical–Avoidant and Passive–Depressive styles shown by individuals in Profile 2 (P#2) suggests more inflexibility or rigidity. The styles preferred by P#2 indicate that they have a heightened sensitivity for negative affect together with a tendency to down-regulate positive affect. Interestingly, this replicates two previous findings: our group has identified a similar profile in an independent sample (n = 431; Czekóová, Shaw, & Urbánek, 2016), and a large-scale review of personality styles reports an “Overcontrolled prototype” that exhibits many of the same characteristics (Donnellan & Robbins, 2010). In line with these contrasting personality characteristics, individuals in P#2 expressed significantly stronger negativity than those in P#1. The PSI framework predicts that (a) chronic down-regulation of positive affect may induce state orientation, and (b) state-oriented individuals discriminate poorly between their own and others’ thoughts, wishes, and expectations (Koole et al., 2006; Kuhl, 1992). Consistent with the former premise, P#2 scored significantly lower on both Failure- and Demand-related action orientation (AOF and AOD, respectively), indicating a poorer ability to regulate their emotions when faced with negative events. This profile also scored lower on self-reported Perspective taking during social interactions, reported higher Personal distress when empathising with others, and exhibited much stronger imitative tendencies—all indices of inefficient self-other distinction (SOD).
The characteristics of these two personality profiles appear to resemble divergent patterns of cognitive control (e.g., Hommel & Colzato, 2017)—a fundamental capacity to override, restrain, or inhibit unwanted yet dominant action tendencies. It is proposed that individuals with high cognitive control can adapt more flexibly to highly demanding situations (action orientation) compared with individuals exhibiting inefficient cognitive control, who are less capable of activating appropriate adjustment processes (state orientation; Jostman & Koole, 2007). Consistent with this, prior research has shown that those with efficient cognitive control have a better emotion regulation and lower negative affect (Inzlicht et al., 2015). In this light, it could be argued that individuals in P#1 (higher flexibility) have, in general, a better cognitive control than those in P#2, enabling them to regulate their emotions more effectively and switch flexibly between cognitive self- and other-action/-affect representations. This would result in efficient SOD, leading to the reduced Personal distress when empathising and better control over imitative tendencies shown by P#1. In contrast, poorer cognitive control will lead to a cognitive system that is too inflexible to switch between alternative cognitive representations. Further research is needed to ascertain whether cognitive control does, indeed, underlie the two divergent personality profiles which we have observed. Furthermore, since considerable progress has been made in elucidating the various factors contributing to cognitive control (e.g., genetic and neurophysiological mechanisms; see Hommel, 2015), future research should investigate whether these same factors drive these two differential personality profiles.
Participants in P#2 reported significantly lower Perspective taking, and this was negatively associated with their VPT performance—with a lower tendency to take the perspective of others during social interaction, individuals are worse when resolving conflicts between their own and another’s perspective. This is, perhaps, unsurprising given the inefficient SOD of P#2, as indexed by their increased automatic imitation (Brass et al., 2009) and Personal distress (Lamm et al., 2016), and their apparent inefficiency in meta-control; avoiding ego-centric errors in perspective taking demands an ability to distinguish flexibly between competing self and other representations (Keysar et al., 2000; for a critical discussion, see Heyes, 2014; Santiesteban et al., 2012a). The state orientation of these individuals also suggests a bias in attentional focus towards the self, an additional factor shown to impact negatively on the ability to adopt another person’s viewpoint (Bukowski & Samson, 2016). Interestingly, individuals in P#2 showed a strong preference for Self-critical-Avoidant personality style, and patients diagnosed with Avoidant personality disorder present difficulties with metacognitive abilities of monitoring and decentration—the mislabelling of self-states and poor inference of others’ states, respectively (Moroni et al., 2016). This will result in both low ability and motivation to take the perspective of others, thereby reinforcing social withdrawal and hindering practice in perspective taking (for a related discussion, see Bird & Viding, 2014). In line with this, an association was reported recently between higher social anxiety and lower VPT (Pile et al., 2017). Studies have also shown that training individuals to withhold involuntary imitation enhances perspective-taking performance (Santiesteban et al., 2012b). It would be interesting, then, to see how individuals comprising P#2 respond to such SOD training; if effective, this simple intervention might prove beneficial for samples characterised by ego-centrism (Pile et al., 2017).
Given the significant difference in self-reported action orientation between profiles, it is surprising that personality did not influence emotion regulation directly; differences in false-alarm rate for affective stimuli on the emotional Go/No-Go task emerged only after interoceptive accuracy was included in the model. Moreover, the positive association between interoceptive accuracy and emotion regulation seems counter-intuitive; why should the greater interoceptive accuracy shown by individuals in P#1 lead to poorer emotion regulation? We suggest that this indicates a complex interaction between affective dispositions and the processing of physiological signals. While action-orientated individuals are more decisive and show less rumination under stress, when combined with enhanced processing of emotional stimuli facilitated by heightened interoception (see Critchley & Garfinkel, 2017), this might manifest as impulsivity on the emotional Go/No-Go task—that is, it may interfere with the withholding of responses to emotional expressions in others. At this point, it is important to stress that action orientation per se should not be considered superior to state orientation; individuals will benefit from a state-orientated approach in certain social situations, such as dangerous and unpredictable contexts (see Kuhl, 1992). Rather, effective emotion regulation requires the ability to switch flexibly between regulatory strategies in different situations. Likewise, it has been proposed that a balance between flexibility and persistence is needed for optimal cognitive control (Hommel, 2015).
Converging with the findings of Ainley et al. (2015), no association was observed between interoceptive accuracy and affective empathy, whether the latter is measured via the ratings of arousal in response to the emotions of others or by self-report instrument. Instead, our data reveal two mechanisms that together mediate the influence of personality mechanisms on empathic expression in opposing directions: First, negativity is related inversely with affective empathy, and through this pathway, individuals in P#2 exhibiting stronger negativity than P#1 also show less affective empathy. Negative affectivity has been shown to be associated positively with Personal distress (Eisenberg et al., 1994), and our data show that P#2 exhibited both stronger negativity and greater Personal distress than P#1. As such, this mediating effect of negativity might drive individuals in P#2 to withdraw, either implicitly or explicitly, from empathic engagement. Second, the strength of involuntary imitative tendencies is associated positively with affective empathy, and so greater imitative tendencies shown by P#2 may lead them to experience more arousal when empathising. Involuntary imitation is attributed to neural perception–action matching mechanisms (see Heyes, 2011); observing an action activates corresponding cortical motor representations, which serves to prime its execution. Through this mirroring process, we can simulate others’ actions implicitly (e.g., Gallese, 2013). In the same way, these mirroring mechanisms might permit the simulation of other emotions during empathy; the experience, observation, and imagination of an emotion engages overlapping brain responses (Jabbi, Bastiaansen, & Keysers, 2008; Wicker et al., 2003). Taking this into consideration, our data suggest that the ability to empathize affectively with others depends upon an interaction between our own current affective state and our tendency to simulate the emotional state of others. The difference which we observed in imitative tendencies between personality profiles stands in contrast to the findings of Butler et al. (2015) who observed no such influence of stable personality traits. This discrepancy could be due to methodological factors: first, imitation elicited by the action stimuli employed by Butler et al. (2015) is influenced heavily by spatial confounds (see Bertenthal et al., 2006; Shaw et al., 2017), but the present study used stimuli that avoided these effects and might have unmasked this relationship. Second, our person-centred approach to personality assessment might be more sensitive than the variable-centred approach adopted by Butler et al. (2015) in uncovering associations between this aspect of behaviour and personality.
The majority of sub-scales measured by the self-report instruments were clustered together in a single multimediator model. Lower Perspective taking, higher Personal distress, and reduced AOF reported by P#2 served as mediators in the influence of personality on Empathic concern. First, P#2 report higher Personal distress relative to P#1 and, in turn, this is associated positively with Empathic concern. Since the Empathic concern sub-scale is considered an explicit measure of affective empathy, this is entirely consistent with this group’s increased affective empathy. This may be indicative of greater self-other merging. Indeed, these same individuals exhibit greater automatic imitation, which suggests lower SOD. As discussed above, imitative tendencies are believed to reflect neural mirroring mechanisms through which the actions and emotions of others are understood through a process of implicit simulation. Inefficient distinction between self- and other-affect representations will lead to a misattribution of increased arousal levels experienced during this simulation process, leading to Personal distress when empathising (Lamm et al., 2016).
Conversely, P#2 report lower Perspective taking and AOF compared with P#1, and these two sub-scales are related to Empathic concern in an opposing manner; while reduced Perspective taking is associated with lower Empathic concern, decreased AOF is related to greater Empathic concern. Focusing on the first of these indirect pathways, the ego-centric attentional focus of this group seems to reduce the concern that they feel for others when empathising. The second pathway might be explained by the other inter-relationships and co-dependencies revealed in our structural model; P#2 report more state orientation during negative experiences and a greater tendency to imitate others, the latter of which leads to greater arousal during empathy. In the same way that imitation of others’ emotional states will lead to personal distress without compensatory SOD mechanisms, the state-orientated style of emotion regulation characterising this profile seems to result in greater empathic concern for the agent whose affect that they are simulating.
It is noteworthy that, aside from Perspective taking, none of the other sub-scales from self-report instruments served to mediate differences between personality profiles on the corresponding performance measures. This lack of association between explicit and implicit measures (see also Ainley et al., 2015; Böckler, Tusche, & Singer, 2016; Melchers et al., 2016) may suggest that they capture different facets of social cognition, or that our subjective experiences differ from our actual abilities. Our results indicate that many of the explicit measures are inter-related, and covariance between self-report measures suggest that individuals’ subjective view of themselves is consistent. As such, further research is needed to investigate whether self-report measures of social cognition differ from actual ability, and how personality might influence this.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this is the first investigation into social cognition that has collected data from a large sample on such an extensive battery of self-report and performance-based measures, and considered the influence of personality on patterns of inter-relationships and mediating effects among them. Our results reveal a hierarchical pattern of relationships among various components of social cognition, informing us about manner in which lower level cognitive mechanisms (e.g., visual perspective taking and self-other distinction) may influence higher level socio-emotional processes (e.g., affective empathy and emotion regulation). By moving beyond the modular approach that has dominated social cognition research to date, such findings provide a more detailed characterisation of individual differences in social behaviour. Using a person-centred approach to personality assessment, we have also shown that different configurations of personality systems exert an influence over discrete aspects of social cognition. At the most general level, these two profiles appear to reflect opposing capacities for cognitive control–flexibility versus rigidity/persistence—thereby providing a model for future research.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The author(s) declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Ethical approval

“All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.”
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.CrossRefPubMed Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Block, J. (2002). Personality as an affect-processing system: Toward an integrative theory. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRef Block, J. (2002). Personality as an affect-processing system: Toward an integrative theory. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51, 5–13. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb-51-1-5.CrossRef Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51, 5–13. https://​doi.​org/​10.​5334/​pb-51-1-5.CrossRef
go back to reference Czekóová, K., Pokorná, Z., & Špiláková, B. (2016). Vývoj metody pro posouzení sociální kognice. Sociální procesy a osobnost 2015—Otázky a výzvy, 67–72. Sborník příspěvků. Masarykova Univerzita Brno. Czekóová, K., Pokorná, Z., & Špiláková, B. (2016). Vývoj metody pro posouzení sociální kognice. Sociální procesy a osobnost 2015—Otázky a výzvy, 67–72. Sborník příspěvků. Masarykova Univerzita Brno.
go back to reference Gilet, A. L., Mella, N., Studer, J., Grühn, D., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (2013). Assessing dispositional empathy in adults: A French validation of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI). Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 45, 42. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030425.CrossRef Gilet, A. L., Mella, N., Studer, J., Grühn, D., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (2013). Assessing dispositional empathy in adults: A French validation of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI). Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 45, 42. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0030425.CrossRef
go back to reference Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guildford Press. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guildford Press.
go back to reference Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York: Guildford Press. Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York: Guildford Press.
go back to reference Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, London: Guilford publications. Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, London: Guilford publications.
go back to reference Koole, S. L., Kuhl, J., Jostmann, N. B., & Finkenauer, C. (2006). Self-regulation in interpersonal relationships: the case of action versus state orientation. In K. D. Vohs & E. J. Finkel (Eds.), Self and relationships: Connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes (pp. 360–383). New York: Guilford Press. Koole, S. L., Kuhl, J., Jostmann, N. B., & Finkenauer, C. (2006). Self-regulation in interpersonal relationships: the case of action versus state orientation. In K. D. Vohs & E. J. Finkel (Eds.), Self and relationships: Connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes (pp. 360–383). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Kuhl, J. (1994). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the Action Control Scale (ACS-90). In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 47–56). Seattle: Hogrefe Huber. Kuhl, J. (1994). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the Action Control Scale (ACS-90). In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 47–56). Seattle: Hogrefe Huber.
go back to reference Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (2002). PSSI—Inventář stylů osobnosti a poruch osobnosti. Praha: Testcentrum. Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (2002). PSSI—Inventář stylů osobnosti a poruch osobnosti. Praha: Testcentrum.
go back to reference Lazarsfeld, P. F., & Henry, N. W. (1968). Latent structure analysis. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Lazarsfeld, P. F., & Henry, N. W. (1968). Latent structure analysis. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.
go back to reference New, A. S., Rot, M. A. H., Ripoll, L. H., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Lazarus, S., Zipursky, E., Weinstein, S. R., Koenigsberg, H. W., Hazlett, E. A., Goodman, M., & Siever, L. J. (2012). Empathy and alexithymia in borderline personality disorder: Clinical and laboratory measures. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26, 660–675. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2012.26.5.660.CrossRefPubMed New, A. S., Rot, M. A. H., Ripoll, L. H., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Lazarus, S., Zipursky, E., Weinstein, S. R., Koenigsberg, H. W., Hazlett, E. A., Goodman, M., & Siever, L. J. (2012). Empathy and alexithymia in borderline personality disorder: Clinical and laboratory measures. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26, 660–675. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1521/​pedi.​2012.​26.​5.​660.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1255. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018729.CrossRefPubMed Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1255. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0018729.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Semerari, A., Colle, L., Pellecchia, G., Carcione, A., Conti, L., Fiore, D., Moroni, F., Nicolò, G., Procacci, M., & Pedone, R. (2015). Personality disorders and mindreading: Specific impairments in patients with borderline personality disorder compared to other PDs. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 203, 626–631. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000339.CrossRefPubMed Semerari, A., Colle, L., Pellecchia, G., Carcione, A., Conti, L., Fiore, D., Moroni, F., Nicolò, G., Procacci, M., & Pedone, R. (2015). Personality disorders and mindreading: Specific impairments in patients with borderline personality disorder compared to other PDs. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 203, 626–631. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1097/​NMD.​0000000000000339​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, S. L. (2013). Multicollinearity and singularity. Using multivariate statistics (pp. 88–91). Boston: Pearson Education Inc. Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, S. L. (2013). Multicollinearity and singularity. Using multivariate statistics (pp. 88–91). Boston: Pearson Education Inc.
Metagegevens
Titel
You ≠ me: individual differences in the structure of social cognition
Auteurs
D. J. Shaw
K. Czekóová
C. R. Pennington
A. W. Qureshi
B. Špiláková
M. Salazar
M. Brázdil
T. Urbánek
Publicatiedatum
15-10-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1107-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2020

Psychological Research 4/2020 Naar de uitgave