Introduction
Routine outcome monitoring (ROM) and feedback to therapists and patients during treatment can be an effective and cost-efficient method to improve patient outcomes in psychotherapy [
1‐
4]. In the past two decades, many institutions all over the world have therefore started implementing ROM in their services systems [
5‐
8]. Feedback from outcome measures can support clinicians to detect patients who deteriorate early in the treatment process and enable clinicians to adapt their treatment strategy as needed. Implementing progress feedback seems particularly important considering that studies have shown that statistical methods outperform clinicians in predicting treatment failure and that the accuracy of predictions can be improved by approximately 13% when statistical algorithms are applied [
9,
10].
Since feedback on outcomes does not necessarily provide any information on how to adjust the treatment strategy, other sources of information need to be consulted. Adding so-called clinical support tools (CST) to feedback can help to provide such information [
11]. CSTs can be defined as problem-solving tools, which alert the clinician to potential obstacles to a positive treatment outcome and provide suggestions for possible interventions. The domains upon which CSTs are based should be relevant to change processes and optimally be applicable to many patients with different psychopathological diagnoses. Asay and Lambert [
12] named four general factors particularly related to change. Based on empirical findings, the authors argue that 40% of recovery can be attributed to (1) client variables and extratherapeutic factors. Further, 30% of improvement can be ascribed to (2) therapeutic relationship factors, while (3) hope and expectancy factors are as important as (4) models and technique factors, each accounting for 15% of recovery.
Lambert et al. [
13] developed the Assessment for Signal Clients (ASC), a self-report questionnaire that assesses three of these four factors. Extratherapeutic factors are measured by two subscales, namely social support and life events. Relationship factors are assessed by a therapeutic alliance scale and hope and expectancy factors are operationalized by a motivation scale. Clinical cut-off scores are provided for each scale. Further, item cut-offs help the clinician to determine which of the scale’s items are most critical. Therapists are also provided with a decision tree [
13], guiding them through the four scales (1) therapeutic alliance, (2) motivation, (3) social support, and (4) stressful life events hierarchically. Further, therapists are guided to reevaluate the diagnosis, need for medication, and the treatment method.
Research has shown that feedback on patient progress is especially effective (i.e., improves treatment outcome) for patients whose symptom distress develops negatively over the course of treatment (negative change trajectory), so-called not-on-track (NOT) patients [
14]. A recent meta-analysis by Lambert et al. [
3] reported a weighted effect size of feedback versus treatment as usual (TAU) of .33 for NOT patients (small effect). To further enhance these effects, CSTs are added to feedback [
3,
14]. In a meta-analysis by Shimokawa et al. [
14], the mean effect size for the combination of feedback and CSTs versus TAU reached
g = .70 (medium to large effect) for NOT patients, while Lambert et al. [
3] found a lower but still considerable mean effect size of
g = .49 (small to medium effect).
Research that particularly focuses on CST domains (the categories or sections that structure the different tools, for instance, therapy motivation or social support) is relatively scarce. Two studies have investigated ASC data to find out more about potential obstacles to a positive outcome. White et al. [
15] examined ASC data from 107 NOT patients from a hospital-based outpatient clinic. About 58% of patients presented with enough problems to exceed a clinical cut-off on at least one of the four ASC scales. In other words, for more than 40% of NOT patients, it was not possible to identify a potential obstacle to positive treatment outcome. This could indicate that more domains should be examined to be able to identify underlying obstacles to successful treatment for more patients. Probst et al. [
16] evaluated the importance of the ASC scales in a sample of patients showing extreme deviations from their statistically generated expected recovery curves. The life events and social support domains were associated with extreme negative deviations. The authors concluded that prioritizing extratherapeutic factors in the decision tree might help to prevent treatment failure.
Building on findings by White et al. [
15], further domains beyond those assessed by the ASC may be relevant to patient deterioration and important to consider when implementing CSTs. Emotion regulation as well as risk behavior and suicidality could be worthy candidates when implementing CSTs. Emotion regulation is a process that has previously been associated with the development and maintenance of clinical disorders [
17], but it has not been implemented in CSTs so far. It comprises different affective styles that influence the quality, intensity, timing, and duration of emotions [
18]. Three emotion regulation strategies that have been consistently found in the literature are tolerating, adjusting, and concealing emotions [
19‐
21]. An instrument that assesses individual differences in emotion regulation is the Affective Style Questionnaire (ASQ), developed by Hofman and Kashdan [
21]. In contrast to emotion regulation, risk behavior like drinking or substance abuse as well as suicidality is assessed in other systems, but these factors are not usually implemented as an individual domain. However, they can have a major impact on the course of therapy and clinicians may profit from more information on these topics [
22]. This gap could be closed by implementing an extra domain in feedback systems that covers risk and suicidality.
Although many studies have focused on the question of whether feedback is effective [
3,
14,
23], questions regarding the implementation and explanatory power of domains selected as the basis of CSTs remain unanswered. To date, few studies have been conducted that compare OT and NOT patients regarding the domains and individual items upon which CSTs are based. A comprehensive picture on the factors that lead to treatment failure, however, is necessary in order to prevent deterioration in therapy by means of feedback and CSTs. The current study therefore aims to evaluate CST domains that are associated with treatment failure. More specifically, we strive to find out more about the difference between OT and NOT patients regarding these domains and the individual items of the underlying scales. This is important in order to not only be able to provide feedback that the treatment strategy should be adjusted, but more specifically to indicate which strategies or interventions can be used to optimize treatment outcomes. This knowledge can be used to support the continued development of ROM systems.
The current study aims to investigate the following research questions:
(1)
Which domains have predictive value for NOT trajectories?
(2)
Do OT and NOT patients differ regarding how often they surpass the domain cut-offs?
(3)
Do OT and NOT patients score differently on the individual items assessing the potential obstacles to a positive treatment outcome?
Discussion
This study aimed to extend knowledge on routine outcome monitoring by comparing patients at risk of treatment failure to patients, whose treatment progress is as expected. We examined whether OT and NOT patients differ with regard to certain factors that have been related to change in the literature and can be regarded as obstacles to a positive treatment outcome. In particular, this study sought to examine whether the domains risk/suicidality, therapeutic alliance, therapy motivation, social support, life events, and emotion regulation have predictive value for NOT trajectories (1st research question). Further, we investigated whether OT and NOT patients differ regarding the frequency of surpassing the domains’ cut-offs (2nd research question) and we also examined the item level to find out whether OT and NOT patients score differently on the individual items assessing these domains (3rd research question).
Overall, the results provide support for the validity of the selected domains’ application. Looking at the predictive value of the individual domains (1st research question), we found that session number, suicidality, therapy motivation, and the occurrence of life events seemed to be predictive of deteriorating in the following sessions. Neither social support, therapeutic alliance, nor emotion regulation predicted going off track in the present study. Thus, in contrast to previous studies [
15,
16], social support did not stand out as one of the most important factors of change.
The finding that a higher session number was associated with later deterioration is in line with research investigating sudden losses in psychotherapy (sudden, substantial increases in symptom distress between two consecutive sessions, i.e., sudden deterioration). While sudden gains (sudden, substantial decreases in symptom distress between two consecutive sessions, i.e., sudden improvement) occur rather early in therapy, study results have shown that sudden losses tend to occur later in therapy [
38,
42].
The finding that suicidality and risk behavior, which are associated with hopelessness and a lack of adaptive regulation strategies, are predictive of symptom worsening makes theoretical sense. Risk behavior such as drinking or substance abuse should be approached in therapy by identifying triggers in the patient’s daily life, for example. Acute suicidality requires the consideration of alternative treatment approaches or settings and should be discussed with the patient and possibly the supervisor in detail. Implementing this domain into the feedback system can help the clinician to identify and evaluate the risk and may provide information on this topic that would otherwise be lacking [
22].
In addition, the findings corroborate the relatively old idea that patients’ therapy motivation and expectations are linked to the initiation and maintenance of change in therapy [
43,
44]. More recent studies also support this idea [
45]) and especially in addiction treatments, resolving ambivalence has become crucial to prevent drop-out and improve outcome [
46]. The findings regarding the domain cut-off crossings and the individual items (results regarding the 2nd and 3rd research questions) show that OT and NOT patients do not differ regarding therapy motivation per se, but that a drop in motivation can promote a negative change trajectory. As motivational problems can have varying causes (e.g., lack of goals, lack of distress, primary or secondary gain [
46‐
49]), therapists need to determine the origin of the motivational problem before implementing interventions.
Further, the association between the occurrence of critical life events and later deterioration seems intuitive and fits with past research findings [
16]. Patients seem to be confronted with a problem and have difficulties coping (e.g., because they lack resources), resulting in symptom worsening. Receiving a signal alert, the therapist’s job is to consider the circumstances and think about the impact such an event has on the patient, his or her goals, and therapy and whether the treatment plan should be adjusted. Therapists might, however, feel that for some patients it makes more sense to continue according to the treatment plan.
Investigating the second research question, we were able to identify potential obstacles (i.e., at least one of the domain cut-offs was crossed) for most of the NOT patients (approximately 74%), which was significantly different than the OT patients. This is promising as therapists can use this as a guide to adjust their treatment strategy. However, the number of domain cut-off crossings was also high for OT patients (approximately 53%). For the RCT, this is not problematic, as therapists of OT patients do not receive feedback on these domains anyway. However, this finding may call for the adjustment of the domain cut-offs after data assessment in this study is complete. However, this finding could also indicate that making use of these domains can be helpful in the treatment of OT patients. It has to be noted that this finding refers to session six, in which most NOT patients were still on track. The results show that NOT patients have a higher burden regarding these domains even before going off track. NOT patients showed more deficits regarding risk/suicidality, life events, and social support. Although our results do not suggest that social support predicts changes in symptomatology, NOT patients tended to show more problems regarding their social network than OT patients at session six. Even though social support is an extratherapeutic domain, many different techniques can help patients to improve the quantity and quality of their social network [
50,
51]. In order to best help the patient, therapists first need to determine the source of the problem (e.g., role overload, difficult circumstances like moving to a different city or relationship break-up, social skills deficits) before deciding which techniques to implement [
51‐
54]. Exploring the critical items underlying the domain may be helpful. Similar to social support, emotion regulation did not seem to have a very high impact on symptomatic change. However, we did find that both OT and NOT patients showed substantial deficits in emotion regulation, as the domain’s cut-off was most commonly crossed irrespective of group membership.
As described above, the item cut-off alert therapists with NOT patients to the items that are particularly critical for that patient. Thus, the information can help the therapist to get a more in-depth and differentiated picture of the problem within the domain. While exploring the third research question, it became apparent that NOT and OT patients especially differed on the items assessing negative life events and suicidality. This suggests that these items are particularly good indicators of NOT patients. Other items that were significantly different between OT and NOT belonged to the emotion regulation and social support domains. Here again, it must be noted that this finding refers to session six in which most NOT patients were still on track. This indicates that NOT patients tend to have more deficits regarding these items even before going off track. None of the significantly differing items belonged to the motivation domain and only one item belonged to the alliance domain. Further, the percentage of cut-off crossings indicated that item cut-offs within these domains are rarely crossed. This could point to ceiling effects within these domains. Although few patients exceeded the domain and item cut-offs in these two areas, the information therapists can gather from feeding back the individual items can be highly relevant for treatment (e.g., the feeling that the therapist disapproves of oneself). This gives therapists the chance to identify specific problems (although rare) quickly and apply suitable interventions.
Discrepancies between these results and past studies [
15,
16] might be explained by the fact that the TTN uses a different and dynamic algorithm to determine OT and NOT patients in comparison to other systems. Further, this study is only one of few studies that actually compared OT and NOT patients regarding such domains. This is a result of institutions having varying routines, for instance, only handing out the ASC when a patient has gone off track instead of administering the questionnaire continuously over the course of treatment. While handing out the questionnaire when patients go off track has the advantage of immediately assessing potential obstacles, handing out the questionnaire in regular intervals to all patients allows for comparative analyses.
In summary, our analyses indicate that particularly focusing on the three domains risk/suicidality, motivation, and life events may prove to be an effective way to prevent treatment failure, as these seem to be directly linked to symptom change. However, the three other scales that do not directly differentiate between OT and NOT patients (social support, alliance, and emotion regulation) can also be helpful to direct clinicians’ attention to problems in NOT cases. Much knowledge is still lacking about the factors that influence change and impact implementation. Future research should build on such findings in order to support therapists to recognize patients at risk and provide effective problem resolution strategies. Further, the findings indicate that several individual items might be more important than others. Thus, questionnaires could be shortened in order to be more efficient in clinical practice.
This study is subject to several limitations. Not all feedback systems make use of the ASC and ASQ in order to determine potential obstacles to a positive treatment outcome (for instance, alliance problems; cf [
55].). Therefore, findings are less generalizable to these feedback systems. Further, in the study, not all questionnaires were assessed in the same way: the outcome measurement HSCL-11 was assessed via touch screen, while others were assessed via paper/pencil. In both cases, however, therapists received detailed progress feedback via the TTN system. Also, although OT and NOT patients were very similar regarding most demographic variables, it should be noted that they differed regarding treatment length. However, the finding that negatively developing cases have longer treatments has already been reported in other studies [
56]. Further, one of the suicidality items is also used to determine whether patients are considered off track or not. This, of course, increases the chance that NOT patients receive more warning signals regarding risk/suicidality and therefore weakens the validity of the corresponding findings in this study. Further, we decided to compare NOT patients’ sixth session with OT patients’ sixth session, which is somewhat arbitrary. It would be interesting to compare a NOT session with an OT session. However, as OT patients do not have a “key session” like NOT patients, because they do not go off track per definition, we opted to compare the sixth session of both groups. As NOT sessions tend to occur more frequently later in therapy, there might also be good arguments for making a different selection, which could be applied in future studies.
Despite these limitations, the current study provides important insights regarding domains that can play a role for NOT trajectories and can help to inform further improvements of decision-support systems in outpatient psychotherapy.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.