Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2015

01-07-2015 | Original Paper

What we think we learn from watching others: the moderating role of ability on perceptions of learning from observation

Auteurs: Nicola J. Hodges, Thomas Coppola

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Despite increased interest in the processes guiding action observation and observational learning, we know little about what people think they learn from watching, how well perceptions of learning marry with actual ability and how ability perceptions develop across multiple observation trials. Based on common coding ideas, we would think that ability and perceptions of ability from watching should be well matched. We conducted two studies to answer these questions that involved repeated observation of a 2-ball juggling task. After each video observation, observers judged if they could perform the skill and gave a confidence score (0–100 %). In Experiment 1, an Observe-only group was compared to an Observe + Physical practice and No-practice group. Both observer groups showed a better physical approximation of the juggling action after practice and in retention and their confidence increased in a linear fashion. Confidence showed a small, yet significant relationship to actual success. In Experiment 2, we limited physical practice to 5 attempts (across 50 observation trials). In general, people who had high perceptions of ability following a demonstration were overconfident, whereas those with lower perceptions of ability were accurate in their assessments. Confidence generally increased across practice, particularly for trials following observation rather than physical practice. We conclude that while perceptions of ability and actual ability show congruence across trials and individuals, observational practice increases people’s confidence in their ability to perform a skill, even despite physical experiences to the contrary.
Literatuur
go back to reference Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3, 111–150.PubMedCrossRef Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3, 111–150.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Adams, J. A. (1986). Use of the model’s knowledge of results to increase the observer’s performance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 12, 89–98. Adams, J. A. (1986). Use of the model’s knowledge of results to increase the observer’s performance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 12, 89–98.
go back to reference Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1016–1109.CrossRef Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1016–1109.CrossRef
go back to reference Ashford, D., Bennett, S. J., & Davids, K. (2006). Observational modeling effects for movement dynamics and movement outcome measures across differing task constraints: a meta-analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 185–205.PubMedCrossRef Ashford, D., Bennett, S. J., & Davids, K. (2006). Observational modeling effects for movement dynamics and movement outcome measures across differing task constraints: a meta-analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 185–205.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.PubMedCrossRef Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (4th ed., pp. 71–81). New York: Academic Press. Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (4th ed., pp. 71–81). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Beek, P. J. (1989). Juggling dynamics. Amsterdam: Free University Press. Beek, P. J. (1989). Juggling dynamics. Amsterdam: Free University Press.
go back to reference Beilock, S. L., Lyons, I. M., Mattarella-Micke, A., Nusbaum, H. C., & Small, S. L. (2008). Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 13269–13273.CrossRef Beilock, S. L., Lyons, I. M., Mattarella-Micke, A., Nusbaum, H. C., & Small, S. L. (2008). Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 13269–13273.CrossRef
go back to reference Burke, C. J., Tobler, P. N., Baddeley, M., & Schultz, W. (2010). Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 14431–14436.CrossRef Burke, C. J., Tobler, P. N., Baddeley, M., & Schultz, W. (2010). Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 14431–14436.CrossRef
go back to reference Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.PubMedCrossRef Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16, 1905–1910.PubMedCrossRef Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16, 1905–1910.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1982). The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: making the unobservable observable. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 153–167.PubMedCrossRef Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1982). The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: making the unobservable observable. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 153–167.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1985). Role of timing of visual monitoring and motor rehearsal in observational learning of action patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17, 269–281.PubMedCrossRef Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1985). Role of timing of visual monitoring and motor rehearsal in observational learning of action patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17, 269–281.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1990). Representational guidance of action production in observational learning: a causal analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 85–97.PubMedCrossRef Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1990). Representational guidance of action production in observational learning: a causal analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 85–97.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clark, S. E., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2002). Peer mastery versus peer coping models: model type has differential effects on psychological and physical performance measures. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 43, 179–196. Clark, S. E., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2002). Peer mastery versus peer coping models: model type has differential effects on psychological and physical performance measures. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 43, 179–196.
go back to reference Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. M. J., Hamilton, A. F. C., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. M. J., Hamilton, A. F. C., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Crossman, E. (1959). A theory of the acquisition of speed-skill. Ergonomics, 2, 153–166.CrossRef Crossman, E. (1959). A theory of the acquisition of speed-skill. Ergonomics, 2, 153–166.CrossRef
go back to reference Dickinson, J., Weeks, D., Randall, B., & Goodman, D. (2004). One-trial motor learning. In A. M. Williams & N. J. Hodges (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: research, theory, and practice (pp. 63–83). London: Routledge. Dickinson, J., Weeks, D., Randall, B., & Goodman, D. (2004). One-trial motor learning. In A. M. Williams & N. J. Hodges (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: research, theory, and practice (pp. 63–83). London: Routledge.
go back to reference Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: the new psychology of success. New York: Random House. Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: the new psychology of success. New York: Random House.
go back to reference Feltz, D. L., Landers, D. M., & Raeder, U. (1979). Enhancing self-efficacy in high-avoidance motor tasks: a comparison of modeling techniques. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 112–122. Feltz, D. L., Landers, D. M., & Raeder, U. (1979). Enhancing self-efficacy in high-avoidance motor tasks: a comparison of modeling techniques. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 112–122.
go back to reference Gruetzmacher, N., Panzer, S., Blandin, Y., & Shea, C. H. (2011). Observation and physical practice: coding of simple motor sequences. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1111–1123.PubMedCrossRef Gruetzmacher, N., Panzer, S., Blandin, Y., & Shea, C. H. (2011). Observation and physical practice: coding of simple motor sequences. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1111–1123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heathcote, A., Brown, S., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2000). The power law repealed: the case for an exponential law of practice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 185–207.CrossRef Heathcote, A., Brown, S., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2000). The power law repealed: the case for an exponential law of practice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 185–207.CrossRef
go back to reference Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Hayes, S. J., & Breslin, G. (2007). What is modeled during observational learning? Journal of Sports Sciences, 25, 531–545.PubMedCrossRef Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Hayes, S. J., & Breslin, G. (2007). What is modeled during observational learning? Journal of Sports Sciences, 25, 531–545.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral Brain Science, 24, 849–878.CrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral Brain Science, 24, 849–878.CrossRef
go back to reference Iacoboni, M. (2005). Neural mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 623–637.CrossRef Iacoboni, M. (2005). Neural mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 623–637.CrossRef
go back to reference Kitsantas, A., Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. (2000). The role of observation and emulation in the development of athletic self-regulation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 811–817.CrossRef Kitsantas, A., Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. (2000). The role of observation and emulation in the development of athletic self-regulation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 811–817.CrossRef
go back to reference Law, B., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2005). Effects of self-modeling on figure skating jump performance and psychological variables. European Journal of Sport Science, 5, 143–152.CrossRef Law, B., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2005). Effects of self-modeling on figure skating jump performance and psychological variables. European Journal of Sport Science, 5, 143–152.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewthwaite, R., & Wulf, G. (2012). Motor learning through a motivational lens. In N. J. Hodges & A. M. Williams (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: research, theory and practice (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). London: Routledge. Lewthwaite, R., & Wulf, G. (2012). Motor learning through a motivational lens. In N. J. Hodges & A. M. Williams (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: research, theory and practice (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). London: Routledge.
go back to reference Maslovat, D., Hayes, S. J., Horn, R., & Hodges, N. J. (2010). Motor learning through observation. In D. Elliot & M. Khan (Eds.), Vision and action: the control of goal-directed movement (pp. 315–339). Champaign: Human Kinetics. Maslovat, D., Hayes, S. J., Horn, R., & Hodges, N. J. (2010). Motor learning through observation. In D. Elliot & M. Khan (Eds.), Vision and action: the control of goal-directed movement (pp. 315–339). Champaign: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Maslovat, D., Hodges, N.J., Krigolson, O., & Handy, T. (2010). Observational practice benefits are limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 119–130. Maslovat, D., Hodges, N.J., Krigolson, O., & Handy, T. (2010). Observational practice benefits are limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 119–130.
go back to reference McCullagh, P., Law, B., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2012). Modeling and performance. In S. Murphy (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of sport and performance psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. McCullagh, P., Law, B., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2012). Modeling and performance. In S. Murphy (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of sport and performance psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W., & Hommel, B. (2002). Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance (XIX ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Prinz, W., & Hommel, B. (2002). Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance (XIX ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Ram, N., & McCullagh, P. (2003). Self-Modeling: influence on psychological response and physical performance. Sport Psychologist, 17, 220–222. Ram, N., & McCullagh, P. (2003). Self-Modeling: influence on psychological response and physical performance. Sport Psychologist, 17, 220–222.
go back to reference Randall, W. E., Dickinson, J., & Goodman, D. (1995). Studies in one-trial motor learning. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 29, 229–249. Randall, W. E., Dickinson, J., & Goodman, D. (1995). Studies in one-trial motor learning. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 29, 229–249.
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2006). Mirrors in the mind. Scientific American, 295, 30–37.CrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2006). Mirrors in the mind. Scientific American, 295, 30–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Scully, D. M., & Newell, K. M. (1985). The acquisition of motor skills: toward a visual perception perspective. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 12, 169–187. Scully, D. M., & Newell, K. M. (1985). The acquisition of motor skills: toward a visual perception perspective. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 12, 169–187.
go back to reference Shea, C. H., Wright, D. L., Wulf, G., & Whitacre, C. (2000). Physical and observational practice afford unique learning opportunities. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 27–36.PubMedCrossRef Shea, C. H., Wright, D. L., Wulf, G., & Whitacre, C. (2000). Physical and observational practice afford unique learning opportunities. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 27–36.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weiss, M. R., McCullagh, P., Smith, A. L., & Berlant, A. R. (1998). Observational learning and the fearful child: influence of peer models on swimming skill performance and psychological responses. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 69, 380–394.PubMedCrossRef Weiss, M. R., McCullagh, P., Smith, A. L., & Berlant, A. R. (1998). Observational learning and the fearful child: influence of peer models on swimming skill performance and psychological responses. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 69, 380–394.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2009). Conceptions of ability affect motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, 461–467.PubMedCrossRef Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2009). Conceptions of ability affect motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, 461–467.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zentgraf, K., & Munzert, J. (2009). Motor imagery and its implications for understanding the motor system. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 219–229.PubMed Zentgraf, K., & Munzert, J. (2009). Motor imagery and its implications for understanding the motor system. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 219–229.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
What we think we learn from watching others: the moderating role of ability on perceptions of learning from observation
Auteurs
Nicola J. Hodges
Thomas Coppola
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0588-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2015

Psychological Research 4/2015 Naar de uitgave