Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 3/2018

22-11-2017 | Original Paper

What Are You Doing With That Object? Comparing the Neural Responses of Action Understanding in Adolescents With and Without Autism

Auteurs: Jennifer J. Pokorny, Naomi V. Hatt, Sally J. Rogers, Susan M. Rivera

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 3/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Understanding another’s actions, including what they are doing and why they are doing it, can be difficult for individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This understanding is supported by the action observation (AON) and mentalizing (MZN) networks, as well as the superior temporal sulcus. We examined these areas in children with ASD and typically developing controls by having participants view eating and placing actions performed in conventional and unconventional ways while functional magnetic resonance images were collected. We found an effect of action-type, but not conventionality, in both groups, and a between groups difference only when viewing conventional eating actions. Findings suggest there are not global AON/MZN deficits in ASD, and observing unconventional actions may not spontaneously activate the MZN.
Literatuur
go back to reference Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & Bower, T. G. R. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3(3), 294–301. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00123.CrossRef Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & Bower, T. G. R. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3(3), 294–301. doi:10.​1111/​1467-7687.​00123.CrossRef
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Bastiaansen, J. A., Thioux, M., Nanetti, L., van der Gaag, C., Ketelaars, C., Minderaa, R., & Keysers, C. (2011). Age-related increase in inferior frontal gyrus activity and social functioning in autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 69(9), 832–838. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.007.CrossRefPubMed Bastiaansen, J. A., Thioux, M., Nanetti, L., van der Gaag, C., Ketelaars, C., Minderaa, R., & Keysers, C. (2011). Age-related increase in inferior frontal gyrus activity and social functioning in autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 69(9), 832–838. doi:10.​1016/​j.​biopsych.​2010.​11.​007.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Kadosh, C. K., & Blakemore, S. -J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(8), 1654–1664. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.011.CrossRef Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Kadosh, C. K., & Blakemore, S. -J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(8), 1654–1664. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neubiorev.​2010.​10.​011.CrossRef
go back to reference Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125(Pt 8), 1839–1849. doi:10.1093/brain/awf189.CrossRef Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125(Pt 8), 1839–1849. doi:10.​1093/​brain/​awf189.CrossRef
go back to reference Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611.CrossRefPubMed Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. doi:10.​1038/​nn1611.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Filimon, F., Rieth, C. A., Sereno, M. I., & Cottrell, G. W. (2015). Observed, executed, and imagined action representations can be decoded from ventral and dorsal areas. Cerebral Cortex, 25(9), 3144–3158. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu110.CrossRefPubMed Filimon, F., Rieth, C. A., Sereno, M. I., & Cottrell, G. W. (2015). Observed, executed, and imagined action representations can be decoded from ventral and dorsal areas. Cerebral Cortex, 25(9), 3144–3158. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhu110.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A., & Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage, 15, 870–878.CrossRefPubMed Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A., & Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage, 15, 870–878.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gunther Moor, B., Op de macks, Z. A., Güroǧlu, B., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Van der molen, M. W., & Crone, E. A. (2012). Neurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 44–52. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr020.CrossRef Gunther Moor, B., Op de macks, Z. A., Güroǧlu, B., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Van der molen, M. W., & Crone, E. A. (2012). Neurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 44–52. doi:10.​1093/​scan/​nsr020.CrossRef
go back to reference Jastorff, J., Clavagnier, S., Gergely, G., & Orban, G. A. (2011). Neural mechanisms of understanding rational actions: Middle temporal gyrus activation by contextual violation. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 318–329. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq098.CrossRefPubMed Jastorff, J., Clavagnier, S., Gergely, G., & Orban, G. A. (2011). Neural mechanisms of understanding rational actions: Middle temporal gyrus activation by contextual violation. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 318–329. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhq098.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Libero, L. E., Maximo, J. O., Deshpande, H. D., Klinger, L. G., Klinger, M. R., & Kana, R. K. (2014). The role of mirroring and mentalizing networks in mediating action intentions in autism. Molecular Autism, 5(50). doi:10.1186/2040-2392-5-50. Libero, L. E., Maximo, J. O., Deshpande, H. D., Klinger, L. G., Klinger, M. R., & Kana, R. K. (2014). The role of mirroring and mentalizing networks in mediating action intentions in autism. Molecular Autism, 5(50). doi:10.​1186/​2040-2392-5-50.
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. J., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic schedule—generic: A standard measures of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947.CrossRefPubMed Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. J., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic schedule—generic: A standard measures of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. doi:10.​1023/​A:​1005592401947.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage, 19, 1233–1239.CrossRefPubMed Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage, 19, 1233–1239.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 310–327. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.310.CrossRefPubMed Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 310–327. doi:10.​1037/​0033-2909.​133.​2.​310.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., & McCarthy, G. (2004). Grasping the intentions of others: The perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(10), 1706–1716. doi:10.1162/0898929042947900.CrossRefPubMed Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., & McCarthy, G. (2004). Grasping the intentions of others: The perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(10), 1706–1716. doi:10.​1162/​0898929042947900​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67(5), 2060–2073. doi:10.2307/1131609.CrossRefPubMed Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67(5), 2060–2073. doi:10.​2307/​1131609.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). SCQ: Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). SCQ: Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Shaw, D. J., Grosbras, M. -H., Leonard, G., Pike, G. B., & Paus, T. (2012). Development of the action observation network during early adolescence: A longitudinal study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 64–80. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq105.CrossRefPubMed Shaw, D. J., Grosbras, M. -H., Leonard, G., Pike, G. B., & Paus, T. (2012). Development of the action observation network during early adolescence: A longitudinal study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 64–80. doi:10.​1093/​scan/​nsq105.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sodian, B., Schuwerk, T., & Kristen, S. (2015). Implicit and spontaneous theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders. In M. Fitzgerald (Ed.), Autism spectrum disorder—Recent advances (pp. 113–135). InTech. doi:10.5772/59393. Sodian, B., Schuwerk, T., & Kristen, S. (2015). Implicit and spontaneous theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders. In M. Fitzgerald (Ed.), Autism spectrum disorder—Recent advances (pp. 113–135). InTech. doi:10.​5772/​59393.
go back to reference Spunt, R. P., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Identifying the what, why, and how of an observed action: An fMRI study of mentalizing and mechanizing during action observation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 63–74. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21446.CrossRefPubMed Spunt, R. P., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Identifying the what, why, and how of an observed action: An fMRI study of mentalizing and mechanizing during action observation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 63–74. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2010.​21446.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wiggins, L. D., Bakeman, R., Adamson, L. B., & Robins, D. L. (2007). The utility of the social communication questionnaire in screening for autism in children referred for early intervention. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 22(1), 33–38. doi:10.1177/10883576070220010401.CrossRef Wiggins, L. D., Bakeman, R., Adamson, L. B., & Robins, D. L. (2007). The utility of the social communication questionnaire in screening for autism in children referred for early intervention. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 22(1), 33–38. doi:10.​1177/​1088357607022001​0401.CrossRef
go back to reference Wilson, B. N., Crawford, S. G., Green, D., Roberts, G., Aylott, A., & Kaplan, B. J. (2009). Psychometric properties of the revised developmental coordination disorder questionnaire. Physical & Occupational Therapy In Pediatrics, 29(2), 182–202. doi:10.1080/01942630902784761.CrossRef Wilson, B. N., Crawford, S. G., Green, D., Roberts, G., Aylott, A., & Kaplan, B. J. (2009). Psychometric properties of the revised developmental coordination disorder questionnaire. Physical & Occupational Therapy In Pediatrics, 29(2), 182–202. doi:10.​1080/​0194263090278476​1.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
What Are You Doing With That Object? Comparing the Neural Responses of Action Understanding in Adolescents With and Without Autism
Auteurs
Jennifer J. Pokorny
Naomi V. Hatt
Sally J. Rogers
Susan M. Rivera
Publicatiedatum
22-11-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 3/2018
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3338-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2018

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 3/2018 Naar de uitgave