Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2008

01-11-2008 | Original Article

Visual word recognition is accompanied by covert articulation: evidence for a speech-like phonological representation

Auteurs: Brianna M. Eiter, Albrecht W. Inhoff

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Two lexical decision task (LDT) experiments examined whether visual word recognition involves the use of a speech-like phonological code that may be generated via covert articulation. In Experiment 1, each visual item was presented with an irrelevant spoken word (ISW) that was either phonologically identical, similar, or dissimilar to it. An ISW delayed classification of a visual word when the two were phonologically similar, and it delayed the classification of a pseudoword when it was identical to the base word from which the pseudoword was derived. In Experiment 2, a LDT was performed with and without articulatory suppression, and pseudowords consisted of regular pseudowords and pseudohomophones. Articulatory suppression decreased sound-specific ISW effects for words and regular pseudowords but not for pseudohomophones. These findings indicate that the processing of an orthographically legal letter sequence generally involves the specification of more than one sound code, one of which involves covert articulation.
Literatuur
go back to reference Abramson, M., & Goldinger, S. D. (1997). What the reader’s eye tells the mind’s ear: Silent reading activates inner speech. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 1059–1068.PubMed Abramson, M., & Goldinger, S. D. (1997). What the reader’s eye tells the mind’s ear: Silent reading activates inner speech. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 1059–1068.PubMed
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory: The interface between memory and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 281–288.CrossRef Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory: The interface between memory and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 281–288.CrossRef
go back to reference Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340–357.PubMedCrossRef Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340–357.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berent, I. (1997). Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: Regularity effects are not necessary evidence for assembly. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1727–1742.PubMedCrossRef Berent, I. (1997). Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: Regularity effects are not necessary evidence for assembly. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1727–1742.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Birch, S., Pollatsek, A., & Kingston, J. (1998). The nature of the sound codes accessed by visual language. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 70–93.CrossRef Birch, S., Pollatsek, A., & Kingston, J. (1998). The nature of the sound codes accessed by visual language. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 70–93.CrossRef
go back to reference Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., & Rizzollati, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 399–402.PubMedCrossRef Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., & Rizzollati, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 399–402.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1996). List context effects on masked phonological priming in the lexical decision task. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 515–519. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1996). List context effects on masked phonological priming in the lexical decision task. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 515–519.
go back to reference Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99.PubMedCrossRef Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Inhoff, A. W., Connine, C., Eiter, B. M., Radach, R., & Heller, D. (2004). The phonological representation of words after their fixation during sentence reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 320–325.PubMed Inhoff, A. W., Connine, C., Eiter, B. M., Radach, R., & Heller, D. (2004). The phonological representation of words after their fixation during sentence reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 320–325.PubMed
go back to reference Kosslyn, S. M., & Matt, A. M. (1977). If you speak slowly, do people read your prose slowly? Person-particular speech recoding during reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 250–252. Kosslyn, S. M., & Matt, A. M. (1977). If you speak slowly, do people read your prose slowly? Person-particular speech recoding during reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 250–252.
go back to reference Lee, H., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1999a). The time course of phonological, semantic, and orthographic coding in reading: Evidence from the fast-priming technique. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 624–634.PubMed Lee, H., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1999a). The time course of phonological, semantic, and orthographic coding in reading: Evidence from the fast-priming technique. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 624–634.PubMed
go back to reference Lee, Y., Binder, K. S., Kim, J., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1999b). Activation of phonological codes during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 948–964.PubMedCrossRef Lee, Y., Binder, K. S., Kim, J., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1999b). Activation of phonological codes during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 948–964.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Levin, H., Grossman, J., Kaplan, E., & Yang, R. (1972). Constraints and the eye-voice span in right and left embedded sentences. Language and Speech, 15, 30–39.PubMed Levin, H., Grossman, J., Kaplan, E., & Yang, R. (1972). Constraints and the eye-voice span in right and left embedded sentences. Language and Speech, 15, 30–39.PubMed
go back to reference Liu, W., Inhoff, I., Ye, Y., & Wu, C. (2003). Use of parafoveally visible characters during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1213–1227.CrossRef Liu, W., Inhoff, I., Ye, Y., & Wu, C. (2003). Use of parafoveally visible characters during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1213–1227.CrossRef
go back to reference Lukatela, G., Eaton, T., Sabadini, L., & Turvey, M. T. (2001). Vowel duration affects visual word identification: Evidence that the mediating phonology is phonetically informed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 151–162.CrossRef Lukatela, G., Eaton, T., Sabadini, L., & Turvey, M. T. (2001). Vowel duration affects visual word identification: Evidence that the mediating phonology is phonetically informed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 151–162.CrossRef
go back to reference McCutchen, D., Bell, L. C., France, I. M., & Perfetti, C. A. (1991). Phoneme specific interference in reading: The tongue-twister effect revisited. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 87–103.CrossRef McCutchen, D., Bell, L. C., France, I. M., & Perfetti, C. A. (1991). Phoneme specific interference in reading: The tongue-twister effect revisited. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 87–103.CrossRef
go back to reference McCutchen, D., & Perfetti, C. A. (1982). The visual tongue-twister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 672–687.CrossRef McCutchen, D., & Perfetti, C. A. (1982). The visual tongue-twister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 672–687.CrossRef
go back to reference Perfetti, C. A., & Bell, L. (1991). Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 473–485.CrossRef Perfetti, C. A., & Bell, L. (1991). Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 473–485.CrossRef
go back to reference Pexman, P. M., Lupker, S. J., & Jared, D. (2001). Homophone effects in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 27, 139–156.CrossRef Pexman, P. M., Lupker, S. J., & Jared, D. (2001). Homophone effects in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 27, 139–156.CrossRef
go back to reference Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R., & Rayner, K. (1992). Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 148–162.PubMedCrossRef Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R., & Rayner, K. (1992). Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 148–162.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pollatsek, A., Tan, L. H., & Rayner, K. (2000). The role of phonological codes in integrating information across saccadic eye movements in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 607–633.PubMedCrossRef Pollatsek, A., Tan, L. H., & Rayner, K. (2000). The role of phonological codes in integrating information across saccadic eye movements in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 607–633.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Saito, S., & Baddeley, A. (2004). Irrelevant sound disrupts speech production: Exploring the relationship between short-term memory and experimentally induced slips of the tongue. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57, 1309–1340.PubMed Saito, S., & Baddeley, A. (2004). Irrelevant sound disrupts speech production: Exploring the relationship between short-term memory and experimentally induced slips of the tongue. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57, 1309–1340.PubMed
go back to reference Stone, G. O., & Van Orden, G. C. (1993). Stratetic control of processing in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 744–774.PubMedCrossRef Stone, G. O., & Van Orden, G. C. (1993). Stratetic control of processing in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 744–774.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watkins, K. E., Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T. (2003). Modulation of motor excitability during speech perception: The role of broca’s area. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 978–987.CrossRef Watkins, K. E., Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T. (2003). Modulation of motor excitability during speech perception: The role of broca’s area. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 978–987.CrossRef
go back to reference Wilson, M. (2001). The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 44–57.PubMed Wilson, M. (2001). The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 44–57.PubMed
go back to reference Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Sereno, M. I., & Iacoboni, M. (2004). Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 701–702.PubMedCrossRef Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Sereno, M. I., & Iacoboni, M. (2004). Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 701–702.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Visual word recognition is accompanied by covert articulation: evidence for a speech-like phonological representation
Auteurs
Brianna M. Eiter
Albrecht W. Inhoff
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0166-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2008

Psychological Research 6/2008 Naar de uitgave