Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2009

01-03-2009 | Original Article

Visual exploration pattern in hemineglect

Auteurs: René M. Müri, D. Cazzoli, T. Nyffeler, T. Pflugshaupt

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2009

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The analysis of eye movement parameters in visual neglect such as cumulative fixation duration, saccade amplitude, or the numbers of saccades has been used to probe attention deficits in neglect patients, since the pattern of exploratory eye movements has been taken as a strong index of attention distribution. The current overview of the literature of visual neglect has its emphasis on studies dealing with eye movement and exploration analysis. We present our own results in 15 neglect patients. The free exploration behavior was analyzed in these patients presenting 32 naturalistic color photographs of everyday scenes. Cumulative fixation duration, spatial distribution of fixations in the horizontal and vertical plane, the number and amplitude of exploratory saccades was analyzed and compared with the results of an age-matched control group. A main result of our study was that in neglect patients, fixation distribution of free exploration of natural scenes is not only influenced by the left–right bias in the horizontal direction but also by the vertical direction.
Literatuur
go back to reference Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J., Jacobs, D. H., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Neglect of radial and vertical space: Importance of the retinotopic reference frame. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 58, 724–728.PubMedCrossRef Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J., Jacobs, D. H., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Neglect of radial and vertical space: Importance of the retinotopic reference frame. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 58, 724–728.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Azouvi, P., Samuel, C., Louis-Dreyfus, A., Bernati, T., Bartolomeo, P., Beis, J. M., et al. (2002). Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 73, 160–166.PubMedCrossRef Azouvi, P., Samuel, C., Louis-Dreyfus, A., Bernati, T., Bartolomeo, P., Beis, J. M., et al. (2002). Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 73, 160–166.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barrett, A. M., Buxbaum, L. J., Coslett, H. B., Edwards, E., Heilman, K. M., Hillis, A. E., et al. (2006). Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for neglect and related disorders: Moving from bench to bedside in stroke patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1223–1236.PubMedCrossRef Barrett, A. M., Buxbaum, L. J., Coslett, H. B., Edwards, E., Heilman, K. M., Hillis, A. E., et al. (2006). Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for neglect and related disorders: Moving from bench to bedside in stroke patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1223–1236.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bartolomeo, P., & Chokron, S. (1999a). Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention? Neurology, 53, 2023–2027.PubMed Bartolomeo, P., & Chokron, S. (1999a). Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention? Neurology, 53, 2023–2027.PubMed
go back to reference Bartolomeo, P., & Chokron, S. (1999b). Egocentric frame of reference: Its role in spatial bias after right hemisphere lesions. Neuropsychologia, 37, 881–894.PubMedCrossRef Bartolomeo, P., & Chokron, S. (1999b). Egocentric frame of reference: Its role in spatial bias after right hemisphere lesions. Neuropsychologia, 37, 881–894.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Behrmann, M., Ghiselli-Crippa, T., & Dimatteo, I. (2001). Impaired initiation but not execution of contralesional saccades in hemispatial neglect. Behavioral Neurology, 13, 39–60.PubMed Behrmann, M., Ghiselli-Crippa, T., & Dimatteo, I. (2001). Impaired initiation but not execution of contralesional saccades in hemispatial neglect. Behavioral Neurology, 13, 39–60.PubMed
go back to reference Behrmann, M., Watt, S., Black, S. E., & Barton, J. J. (1997). Impaired visual search in patients with unilateral neglect: An oculographic analysis. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1445–1458.PubMedCrossRef Behrmann, M., Watt, S., Black, S. E., & Barton, J. J. (1997). Impaired visual search in patients with unilateral neglect: An oculographic analysis. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1445–1458.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bisiach, E., Pizzamiglio, L., Nico, D., & Antonucci, G. (1996). Beyond unilateral neglect. Brain, 119, 851–857.PubMedCrossRef Bisiach, E., Pizzamiglio, L., Nico, D., & Antonucci, G. (1996). Beyond unilateral neglect. Brain, 119, 851–857.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buxbaum, L. J., Ferraro, M. K., Veramonti, T., Farne, A., Whyte, J., Ladavas, E., et al. (2004). Hemispatial neglect: Subtypes, neuroanatomy and disability. Neurology, 62, 749–756.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J., Ferraro, M. K., Veramonti, T., Farne, A., Whyte, J., Ladavas, E., et al. (2004). Hemispatial neglect: Subtypes, neuroanatomy and disability. Neurology, 62, 749–756.PubMed
go back to reference Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: Bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2989–3000.PubMedCrossRef Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: Bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2989–3000.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chatterjee, A. (1998). Motor minds and mental models in neglect. Brain and Cognition, 37, 339–349.PubMedCrossRef Chatterjee, A. (1998). Motor minds and mental models in neglect. Brain and Cognition, 37, 339–349.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chokron, S., & Bartolomeo, P. (1997). Patterns of dissociation between left hemineglect and deviation of the egocentric reference. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1503–1508.PubMedCrossRef Chokron, S., & Bartolomeo, P. (1997). Patterns of dissociation between left hemineglect and deviation of the egocentric reference. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1503–1508.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., et al. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron, 21, 761–773.PubMedCrossRef Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., et al. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron, 21, 761–773.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Danckert, J., & Ferber, S. (2006). Revisiting unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44, 987–1006.PubMedCrossRef Danckert, J., & Ferber, S. (2006). Revisiting unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44, 987–1006.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gainotti, G., D'Erme, P., & Bartolomeo, P. (1991). Early orientation of attention toward the half space ipsilateral to the lesion in patients with unilateral brain damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 54, 1082–1089.PubMedCrossRef Gainotti, G., D'Erme, P., & Bartolomeo, P. (1991). Early orientation of attention toward the half space ipsilateral to the lesion in patients with unilateral brain damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 54, 1082–1089.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1989). Is neglect (only) lateral? A quadrant analysis of line cancellation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 793–798.PubMedCrossRef Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1989). Is neglect (only) lateral? A quadrant analysis of line cancellation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 793–798.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1991). Spatial compression in visual neglect: A case study. Cortex, 27, 623–629.PubMed Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1991). Spatial compression in visual neglect: A case study. Cortex, 27, 623–629.PubMed
go back to reference Heide, W., & Kömpf, D. (1998). Combined deficits of saccades and visuo-spatial orientation after cortical lesions. Experimental Brain Research, 23, 164–171.CrossRef Heide, W., & Kömpf, D. (1998). Combined deficits of saccades and visuo-spatial orientation after cortical lesions. Experimental Brain Research, 23, 164–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Himmelbach, M., Erb, M., & Karnath, H.-O. (2006). Exploring the visual world: The neural substrate of spatial orienting. Neuroimage, 32, 1747–1759.PubMedCrossRef Himmelbach, M., Erb, M., & Karnath, H.-O. (2006). Exploring the visual world: The neural substrate of spatial orienting. Neuroimage, 32, 1747–1759.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (1996). Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction. Journal of Neurology, 243, 652–657.PubMedCrossRef Husain, M., & Kennard, C. (1996). Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction. Journal of Neurology, 243, 652–657.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Husain, M., & Rorden, C. (2003). Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 26–36.PubMedCrossRef Husain, M., & Rorden, C. (2003). Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 26–36.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Husain, M., Mannan, S., Hodgson, T., Wojciulik, E., Driver, J., & Kennard, C. (2001). Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. Brain, 124, 941–952.PubMedCrossRef Husain, M., Mannan, S., Hodgson, T., Wojciulik, E., Driver, J., & Kennard, C. (2001). Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. Brain, 124, 941–952.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ignashchenkova, A., Dicke, P. W., Haarmeier, T., & Thier, P. (2004). Neuronspecific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 56–64.PubMedCrossRef Ignashchenkova, A., Dicke, P. W., Haarmeier, T., & Thier, P. (2004). Neuronspecific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 56–64.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O. (1997). Spatial orientation and the representation of space with parietal lobe lesions. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society, London B, 352, 1411–1419.CrossRef Karnath, H.-O. (1997). Spatial orientation and the representation of space with parietal lobe lesions. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society, London B, 352, 1411–1419.CrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O. (1998). Perception of horizontal distances in patients with spatial neglect. Experimental Brain Research, 23, 190–191.CrossRef Karnath, H.-O. (1998). Perception of horizontal distances in patients with spatial neglect. Experimental Brain Research, 23, 190–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., Ferber, S., & Himmelbach, M. (2001). Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature, 411, 950–953.PubMedCrossRef Karnath, H.-O., Ferber, S., & Himmelbach, M. (2001). Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature, 411, 950–953.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., & Fetter, M. (1995). Ocular space exploration in the dark and its relation to subjective and objective body orientation in neglect patients with parietal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 33, 371–377.PubMedCrossRef Karnath, H.-O., & Fetter, M. (1995). Ocular space exploration in the dark and its relation to subjective and objective body orientation in neglect patients with parietal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 33, 371–377.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., Fetter, M., & Dichgans, J. (1996). Ocular exploration of space as a function of neck proprioceptive and vestibular input. Observations in normal subjects and patients with spatial neglect after parietal lesions. Experimental Brain Research, 109, 333–342.CrossRef Karnath, H.-O., Fetter, M., & Dichgans, J. (1996). Ocular exploration of space as a function of neck proprioceptive and vestibular input. Observations in normal subjects and patients with spatial neglect after parietal lesions. Experimental Brain Research, 109, 333–342.CrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., Fruhmann-Berger, M., Küker, W., & Rorden, C. (2004). The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: A Study of 140 Patients. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1162–1172.CrossRef Karnath, H.-O., Fruhmann-Berger, M., Küker, W., & Rorden, C. (2004). The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: A Study of 140 Patients. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1162–1172.CrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., Himmelbach, M., & Rorden, C. (2002). The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: Putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. Brain, 125, 350–360.PubMedCrossRef Karnath, H.-O., Himmelbach, M., & Rorden, C. (2002). The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: Putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. Brain, 125, 350–360.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., & Niemeier, M. (2002). Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behavior of patients with spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1577–1585.PubMedCrossRef Karnath, H.-O., & Niemeier, M. (2002). Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behavior of patients with spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1577–1585.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., Niemeier, M., & Dichgans, J. (1998). Space exploration in neglect. Brain, 121, 2357–2367.PubMedCrossRef Karnath, H.-O., Niemeier, M., & Dichgans, J. (1998). Space exploration in neglect. Brain, 121, 2357–2367.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karussis, D., Leker, R. R., & Abramsky, O. (2000). Cognitive dysfunction following thalamic stroke: A study of 16 cases and review of the literature. Journal of The Neurological Sciences, 172, 25–29.PubMedCrossRef Karussis, D., Leker, R. R., & Abramsky, O. (2000). Cognitive dysfunction following thalamic stroke: A study of 16 cases and review of the literature. Journal of The Neurological Sciences, 172, 25–29.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kinsbourne, M. (1993). Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In I. H. Robertson & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 63–86). Hove (UK): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kinsbourne, M. (1993). Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In I. H. Robertson & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 63–86). Hove (UK): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.PubMedCrossRef Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kustov, A. A., & Robinson, D. L. (1996). Shared neural control of attention shifts and eye movements. Nature, 384, 74–77.PubMedCrossRef Kustov, A. A., & Robinson, D. L. (1996). Shared neural control of attention shifts and eye movements. Nature, 384, 74–77.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Losier, B. J. W., & Klein, R. M. (2001). A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 25, 1–13. Losier, B. J. W., & Klein, R. M. (2001). A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 25, 1–13.
go back to reference Malhotra, P., Coulthard, E., & Husain, M. (2006). Hemispatial neglect, balance and eye-movement control. Current Opinion in Neurology, 19, 14–20.PubMedCrossRef Malhotra, P., Coulthard, E., & Husain, M. (2006). Hemispatial neglect, balance and eye-movement control. Current Opinion in Neurology, 19, 14–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mark, V. W., & Heilman, K. M. (1997). Diagonal neglect on cancellation. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1425–1436.PubMedCrossRef Mark, V. W., & Heilman, K. M. (1997). Diagonal neglect on cancellation. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1425–1436.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., & Robertson, I. H. (1993). Contemporary theories of unilateral neglect: A critical review. In I. H. Robertson & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 311–329). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., & Robertson, I. H. (1993). Contemporary theories of unilateral neglect: A critical review. In I. H. Robertson & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 311–329). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Mattingley, J. B., Bradshaw, J. L., Bradshaw, J. A., & Nettleton, N. C. (1994). Residual rightward attentional bias after apparent recovery from right hemisphere damage: Implications for a multicomponent model of neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 597–604.PubMedCrossRef Mattingley, J. B., Bradshaw, J. L., Bradshaw, J. A., & Nettleton, N. C. (1994). Residual rightward attentional bias after apparent recovery from right hemisphere damage: Implications for a multicomponent model of neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 597–604.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mennemeier, M., Wertman, E., & Heilman, K. M. (1992). Neglect of near peripersonal space. Brain, 115, 37–50.PubMedCrossRef Mennemeier, M., Wertman, E., & Heilman, K. M. (1992). Neglect of near peripersonal space. Brain, 115, 37–50.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mort, D. J., & Kennard, C. (2003). Visual search and its disorders. Current Opinion in Neurology, 16(1), 51–57.PubMedCrossRef Mort, D. J., & Kennard, C. (2003). Visual search and its disorders. Current Opinion in Neurology, 16(1), 51–57.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mort, D. J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S. K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C., et al. (2003). The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain, 126, 1986–1997.PubMedCrossRef Mort, D. J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S. K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C., et al. (2003). The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain, 126, 1986–1997.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Niemeier, M., & Karnath, H.-O. (2000). Exploratory saccades show no directionspecific deficit in neglect. Neurology, 54, 515–518.PubMed Niemeier, M., & Karnath, H.-O. (2000). Exploratory saccades show no directionspecific deficit in neglect. Neurology, 54, 515–518.PubMed
go back to reference Niemeier, M., & Karnath, H.-O. (2003). Stimulus-driven and voluntary saccades are coded in different coordinate systems. Current Biology, 13, 585–589.PubMedCrossRef Niemeier, M., & Karnath, H.-O. (2003). Stimulus-driven and voluntary saccades are coded in different coordinate systems. Current Biology, 13, 585–589.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nobre, A. C., Gitelman, D. R., Dias, E. C., & Mesulam, M. M. (2000). Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: Overlapping neural systems. NeuroImage, 11, 210–216.PubMedCrossRef Nobre, A. C., Gitelman, D. R., Dias, E. C., & Mesulam, M. M. (2000). Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: Overlapping neural systems. NeuroImage, 11, 210–216.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Olk, B., Harvey, M., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). First saccades reveal biases in recovered neglect. Neurocase, 8, 306–313.PubMedCrossRef Olk, B., Harvey, M., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). First saccades reveal biases in recovered neglect. Neurocase, 8, 306–313.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Parton, A., Malhotra, P., & Husain, M. (2004). Hemispatial neglect. Journal of Neurolgy, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 75, 13–21. Parton, A., Malhotra, P., & Husain, M. (2004). Hemispatial neglect. Journal of Neurolgy, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 75, 13–21.
go back to reference Pflugshaupt, T., Bopp-Almoslochner, S., Heinemann, D., Mosimann, U. P., von Wartburg, R., Nyffeler, T., et al. (2004). Residual oculomotor and exploratory deficits in patients with recovered hemineglect. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1203–1211.PubMedCrossRef Pflugshaupt, T., Bopp-Almoslochner, S., Heinemann, D., Mosimann, U. P., von Wartburg, R., Nyffeler, T., et al. (2004). Residual oculomotor and exploratory deficits in patients with recovered hemineglect. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1203–1211.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pitzalis, S., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (1997). Vertical neglect: Behavioral and electrophysiological data. Cortex, 33, 679–688.PubMedCrossRef Pitzalis, S., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (1997). Vertical neglect: Behavioral and electrophysiological data. Cortex, 33, 679–688.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863–1874.PubMed Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863–1874.PubMed
go back to reference Ptak, R., Schnider, A., Golay, L., & Müri, R. M. (2007). A non-spatial bias favouring fixated stimuli revealed in patients with spatial neglect. Brain, 130, 3211–3222.PubMedCrossRef Ptak, R., Schnider, A., Golay, L., & Müri, R. M. (2007). A non-spatial bias favouring fixated stimuli revealed in patients with spatial neglect. Brain, 130, 3211–3222.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rapcsak, S. Z., Cimino, C. R., & Heilman, K. M. (1988). Altitudinal neglect. Neurology, 38, 277–281.PubMedCrossRef Rapcsak, S. Z., Cimino, C. R., & Heilman, K. M. (1988). Altitudinal neglect. Neurology, 38, 277–281.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Richard, C., Rousseaux, M., & Honoré, J. (2005). The egocentric reference deviation of neglect patients is influenced by visuospatial attention. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1784–1791.PubMedCrossRef Richard, C., Rousseaux, M., & Honoré, J. (2005). The egocentric reference deviation of neglect patients is influenced by visuospatial attention. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1784–1791.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ro, T., Rorden, C., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. (2001). Ipsilesional biases in saccades but not perception after lesions of the human inferior parietal lobule. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 920–929.PubMedCrossRef Ro, T., Rorden, C., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. (2001). Ipsilesional biases in saccades but not perception after lesions of the human inferior parietal lobule. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 920–929.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shelton, P. A., Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1990). Peripersonal and vertical neglect. Brain, 113, 191–205.PubMedCrossRef Shelton, P. A., Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1990). Peripersonal and vertical neglect. Brain, 113, 191–205.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smania, N., Martini, M. C., Gambina, G., Tomelleri, G., Palamara, A., Natale, E., et al. (1998). The spatial distribution of visual attention in hemineglect and extinction patients. Brain, 121, 1759–1770.PubMedCrossRef Smania, N., Martini, M. C., Gambina, G., Tomelleri, G., Palamara, A., Natale, E., et al. (1998). The spatial distribution of visual attention in hemineglect and extinction patients. Brain, 121, 1759–1770.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sprenger, A., Kömpf, D., & Heide, W. (2002). Visual search in patients with left visual hemineglect. Progress in Brain Research, 140, 395–416.PubMedCrossRef Sprenger, A., Kömpf, D., & Heide, W. (2002). Visual search in patients with left visual hemineglect. Progress in Brain Research, 140, 395–416.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vallar, G. (2001). Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanatomy. NeuroImage, 14, 52–58.CrossRef Vallar, G. (2001). Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanatomy. NeuroImage, 14, 52–58.CrossRef
go back to reference Walker, R., & Findlay, J. M. (1996). Saccadic eye movement programming in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 34, 493–508.PubMedCrossRef Walker, R., & Findlay, J. M. (1996). Saccadic eye movement programming in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 34, 493–508.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Visual exploration pattern in hemineglect
Auteurs
René M. Müri
D. Cazzoli
T. Nyffeler
T. Pflugshaupt
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2009
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2009
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0204-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2009

Psychological Research 2/2009 Naar de uitgave