Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2-3/2004

01-04-2004 | Original Article

Variable action effects: response control by context-specific effect anticipations

Auteurs: Andrea Kiesel, Joachim Hoffmann

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2-3/2004

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The ideomotor principle (IMP) claims that bidirectional associations between actions and their contingent effects are acquired so that voluntary actions are accessed by the anticipation of intended effects. Until now, evidence for the IMP exists only for stable action-effect relations. The present paper explores whether the IMP also holds true for the initiation of actions for which no unconditional contingent action-effect relations exist. Participants responded with left and right key presses in two different contexts. They selected the responses according to the vertical (context A) or horizontal (context B) position of a target. Responses were followed by short/fast movements of the target in context A and comparatively long/slow movements in context B. Consequently, each response produced short and long effects equally often in both contexts. Nevertheless, RTs decreased in contexts with short effects and increased in contexts with long effects. Data confirm that action-effect associations were acquired context-specifically and that the same actions were accessed by different effect anticipations.
Voetnoten
1
The variable experimental half was included in the analysis to check whether participants stuck to an acquired context-effect assignment or whether they also flexibly relearned context-effect assignments.
 
2
The action-effect started with the offset of the response as we also measured response duration in this experiment. However, the results concerning response duration were unsystematic and will not be reported here.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing. Attention and performance series (pp. 421–452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing. Attention and performance series (pp. 421–452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Elsner, B. & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed Elsner, B. & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 308–339. Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 308–339.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). A double stimulation test of ideomotor theory with implications for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 392–398. Greenwald, A. G. (1970). A double stimulation test of ideomotor theory with implications for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 392–398.
go back to reference Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73. Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73.
go back to reference Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik. Königsberg, Germany: August Wilhelm Unzer. Herbart, J. F. (1825). Psychologie als Wissenschaft neu gegründet auf Erfahrung, Metaphysik und Mathematik. Königsberg, Germany: August Wilhelm Unzer.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Die Funktion von Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Die Funktion von Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J. & Sebald, A. (2000). Lernmechanismen zum Erwerb verhaltenssteuernden Wissens. Psychologische Rundschau, 51, 1–9.CrossRef Hoffmann, J. & Sebald, A. (2000). Lernmechanismen zum Erwerb verhaltenssteuernden Wissens. Psychologische Rundschau, 51, 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482. Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.PubMed Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action-concept model of stimulus response compatibility. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action-concept model of stimulus response compatibility. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Pösse, B., & Waszak, F. (2000). Contextualization in perception and action. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 227–245. Hommel, B., Pösse, B., & Waszak, F. (2000). Contextualization in perception and action. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 227–245.
go back to reference James, W. (1890/1981). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. James, W. (1890/1981). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62, 300–312.CrossRef Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62, 300–312.CrossRef
go back to reference Koch, I. (2001). Automatic and intentional activation of task-sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 1474–1486. Koch, I. (2001). Automatic and intentional activation of task-sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 1474–1486.
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus response compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus response compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394. Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2003). Temporal response-effect compatibility. Psychological Research, 67, 153–159.PubMed Kunde, W. (2003). Temporal response-effect compatibility. Psychological Research, 67, 153–159.PubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., & Stöcker, C. (2002). A Simon effect for stimulus-response duration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 55, 581–592. Kunde, W., & Stöcker, C. (2002). A Simon effect for stimulus-response duration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 55, 581–592.
go back to reference Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder die Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung. Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder die Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.
go back to reference Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.CrossRef Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.CrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000a). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.CrossRefPubMed Meiran, N. (2000a). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000b). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance 18: Controll of Cognitive Processes. Attention and performance series (pp. 377–399). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Meiran, N. (2000b). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance 18: Controll of Cognitive Processes. Attention and performance series (pp. 377–399). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Meiran, N., & Gotler, A. (2001). Modeling cognitive control in task switching and ageing. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 165–186.CrossRef Meiran, N., & Gotler, A. (2001). Modeling cognitive control in task switching and ageing. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 165–186.CrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N., Chorev, Z., & Sapir, A. (2000). Component processes in task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 211–253.PubMed Meiran, N., Chorev, Z., & Sapir, A. (2000). Component processes in task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 211–253.PubMed
go back to reference Münsterberg, H. (1889). Beiträge zur experimentalen Psychologie, Heft 1. Freiburg, Germany: Mohr. Münsterberg, H. (1889). Beiträge zur experimentalen Psychologie, Heft 1. Freiburg, Germany: Mohr.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231. Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMed Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMed
go back to reference Stock, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Intentional fixation of behavioural learning, or how R-O learning blocks S-R learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–153.CrossRef Stock, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2002). Intentional fixation of behavioural learning, or how R-O learning blocks S-R learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–153.CrossRef
go back to reference Stock, A., & Stock, C. (in press). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research. Stock, A., & Stock, C. (in press). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research.
go back to reference Stöcker, C., Sebald, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). The influence of response-effect compatibility in a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 685–703. Stöcker, C., Sebald, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). The influence of response-effect compatibility in a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 685–703.
go back to reference Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Ideo-motor action. Psychological Review, 20, 91–106. Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Ideo-motor action. Psychological Review, 20, 91–106.
go back to reference Wundt, W. (1893). Grundzüge der Physiologischen Psychologie, Vol. II, 4th ed. Leipzig: Engelmann. Wundt, W. (1893). Grundzüge der Physiologischen Psychologie, Vol. II, 4th ed. Leipzig: Engelmann.
go back to reference Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978. Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962–978.
go back to reference Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 595–613. Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 595–613.
go back to reference Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2002). Effect anticipation in action planning. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action. Attention & Performance, Vol. XIX (pp. 645–672). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2002). Effect anticipation in action planning. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action. Attention & Performance, Vol. XIX (pp. 645–672). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Variable action effects: response control by context-specific effect anticipations
Auteurs
Andrea Kiesel
Joachim Hoffmann
Publicatiedatum
01-04-2004
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2-3/2004
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0152-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2-3/2004

Psychological Research 2-3/2004 Naar de uitgave