Discussion
Previous research indicates that for many adolescents cyberbullying victimization can be perpetuated over time (Rose & Tynes,
2015), thereby increasing the risk of negative outcomes, such as psychological problems (Gámez-Guadix et al.,
2015). The current study aimed to identify some of the mechanisms involved in the perpetuation of cyberbullying victimization over time and examined the role of executive functions in this process.
In line with our hypothesis, the findings indicate that cyberbullying victimization was perpetuated over time through an increase in cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms, which is consistent with some theoretical approaches, such as the general aggression model (Anderson & Bushman,
2002). Thus, adolescents who are victims of cyberbullying can develop depressive symptoms (sadness, tiredness, loneliness) and/or react aggressively. These outcomes could make them less attractive to peers and increase the likelihood of future cyberbullying victimization (Gámez-Guadix et al.,
2013). However, this result had a limitation in that a negative longitudinal path was also observed between cyberbullying perpetration at W1 and cyberbullying victimization at W2. Therefore, the results regarding the longitudinal relationship between cyberbullying perpetration and cyberbullying victimization were mixed, as they were opposite from W1 to W2 and from W2 to W3. The above-mentioned literature supported the notion that cyberbullying perpetration predicts an increase in cyberbullying victimization (Marciano et al.,
2020; Royuela-Colomer et al.,
2018). Nevertheless, one study found that traditional bullying perpetration predicted lower bullying victimization systematically in non-clinical Korean adolescents (Choi & Park,
2021). The authors of that study interpreted this finding to indicate that, by bullying others, the bullies would have more resources to escape victimization, which could also occur in cyberbullying. Nonetheless, due to the contrary longitudinal relationships found in this study, the findings should be considered with caution.
Interestingly, and consistent with previous research on preadolescents (Zhang et al.,
2020), this study found that depressive symptoms not only predicted more victimization but also the perpetration of cyberbullying. Delving into these relationships, it was found that cyberbullying perpetration mediated the association between depressive symptoms and cyberbullying victimization. It has been proposed that individuals with depressive symptoms are at a higher risk of experiencing interpersonal problems (Kochel et al.,
2012), and this could include both cyberbullying perpetration and victimization (Zhang et al.,
2020).
Regarding the role of executive functions, the results indicated that only cognitive flexibility moderated the predictive associations from cyberbullying victimization to both cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms. Namely, the association between W1 cyberbullying victimization and W2 cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms was lower when cognitive flexibility was high. Consequently, cognitive flexibility displayed a protective role against the development of cyberbullying behaviors and depressive symptoms after victimization, reducing future cyberbullying victimization. Thus, cognitive flexibility seems to contribute to breaking the cycle of perpetuation of victimization by decreasing cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms as reactions to the victimization. A possible explanation for this could be that adolescents who are high in cognitive flexibility respond to cyberbullying victimization with more helpful strategies (e.g., block the perpetrator on all social media and look for help; Kowalski et al.,
2014), which could reduce the negative impact of cyberbullying and, consequently, help them to avoid the perpetuation of victimization over time. Another finding of this study was that cyberbullying perpetration mediated the relationship between cognitive flexibility and cyberbullying victimization. Specifically, good cognitive flexibility predicted lower cyberbullying perpetration, which in turn led to lower victimization. Good cognitive flexibility leads to the consideration of different alternatives to a situation or problem (Diamond & Ling,
2019), which can lead the individual to avoid aggressive behavior and, consequently, reduce future victimization.
Selective attention did not attenuate the negative impact of cyberbullying victimization on the prediction of cyberbullying perpetration, depressive symptoms, and the perpetuation of cyberbullying victimization. Furthermore, selective attention played a dysfunctional role, as better selective attention led to higher cyberbullying perpetration, which, in turn, predicted higher cyberbullying victimization. This finding was unexpected since previous research involving a non-clinical sample of Swiss adolescents concluded the opposite, that is, that attention problems led to greater perpetration (Murray et al.,
2020). Accordingly, future research should attempt to replicate these findings and examine the circumstances under which selective attention is maladaptive. It is possible, for example, that selective attention leads to forms of attentional biases to potential threats in ambiguous situations in some adolescents. These attentional biases, in turn, could increase aggression and hostile intent attributions (Miller & Johnston,
2019). Future research should explore this potential mechanism in cyberbullying contexts. Moreover, in the current study, selective attention was assessed as a cold executive function, so future studies should examine whether selective attention to emotional stimuli (i.e., assessed as a hot process) plays a different role than cold selective attention in adolescents who experience victimization.
The last objective was to analyze sex differences in the predictive model. The results indicated that there were sex differences in the moderating role of selective attention in the stability of cyberbullying victimization from W1 to W2. In girls, it seems that selective attention acted as a risk factor increasing the stability of cyberbullying victimization, whereas in boys selective attention played a protective role. A possible explanation for this may be related to the fact that, as in previous research (Ghandour et al.,
2019), the girls experienced more depressive symptoms than boys, which could influence the functionality of their selective attention. Girls who are high in selective attention could focus greater attention on the negative aspects of internalizing symptoms, which would make them more vulnerable to future victimization. However, more research is needed in this field.
Meanwhile, the paths from W2 cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms to W3 cyberbullying victimization were stronger in girls than in boys. These mechanisms could contribute to the higher stability of victimization among girls. The stronger association between perpetration and victimization in girls is contrary to previous findings in German adolescents (Festl & Quandt,
2016), whereas the stronger association between depressive symptoms and victimization is consistent with previous studies on Finnish adolescents involving non-clinical samples (Kaltiala-Heino et al.,
2010). Because girls suffer more depressive symptoms than boys (Ghandour et al.,
2019), they could be more vulnerable to victimization by peers. Nonetheless, these findings should be considered with caution since they only occurred between W2 and W3 and not between W1 and W2.
This study has some limitations that provide opportunities for future research. First, the predictive model included victimization and perpetration of cyberbullying. However, it would have also been interesting to study the role of cyberbystanders in this model, as they can influence cyberbullying situations by helping the victim, reinforcing the aggressor, and/or worsening the situation with their passivity (Machackova,
2020). Thus, future research could replicate this work including the experience of witnessing cyberbullying and analyze its relationship with executive functions and internalizing symptoms. Second, the present study focused on two cold executive functions: cognitive flexibility and selective attention. Future studies could examine whether other cold executive functions, such as working memory and inhibitory control, are longitudinally related to cyberbullying, both of which have been studied in relation to bullying (Medeiros et al.,
2016). Furthermore, given the unexpected results regarding selective attention, future studies should examine the role of hot executive functions, which include emotional components. There is a gap in the research on cyberbullying and executive functions, so their relationships require further study. Third, the only internalizing symptoms that were evaluated were depressive symptoms. It would be useful to consider other internalizing symptoms, such as anxiety, as well as other psychological problems, including eating disorders and self-injury behaviors, which have been identified as potential outcomes of cyberbullying victimization in non-clinical Spanish adolescents (Faura-Garcia et al.,
2021; Marco & Tormo-Irun,
2018). Fourth, executive functions were only measured at the beginning of the study. However, they could have changed over the course of the study and been influenced by other study variables, such as victimization and depressive symptoms. Stressful situations (e.g., victimization) and some depressive symptoms (e.g., loneliness and sadness) can influence executive functions and the prefrontal cortex, so that they can mimic an executive function disorder, such as ADHD (Diamond,
2013). Finally, the sample was a community sample, and the results need to be replicated in clinical samples of adolescents. In fact, although missingness was low, the adolescents who failed to respond at W2 and W3 scored higher on cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms. The power analysis indicated that the required sample size was ≈ 700 participants, which could not be met because the loss of participants was between 10.3% and 15.3% in each wave. Moreover, the sample only comes from the Basque Country (Spain). Therefore, the findings of the study are not generalizable to the adolescent populations in other geographical areas and should be considered with caution.
Despite these limitations, the current study also has several strengths. First, it used a longitudinal design with three waves, which included mediation and moderation relationships. With this design, the study contributes to determining the mediating role of cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms in the perpetuation of cyberbullying victimization. In addition, the study fills a gap in the literature by examining the moderating role of cognitive flexibility and selective attention when cyberbullying victimization occurs. The findings contribute to knowledge on the longitudinal relationships between executive functions, cyberbullying, and depressive symptoms while providing opportunities for future research to examine other possible mediating and/or moderating mechanisms between these variables (e.g., personality traits and psychological symptoms). Finally, despite the fact that the discrepancy in the results between performance and self-report measures has been discussed in the literature (Teglasi et al.,
2017), in this study significant relationships were found between measures obtained with different methods (i.e., performance tasks to assess executive functions and self-report questionnaires for the rest of the variables). This provides support to the findings of this study since the performance tasks are less biased by variables such as depressive symptoms, which could lead an adolescent to minimize his or her skills in the use of executive functions.
To conclude, the current study claims that cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms contribute to perpetuating cyberbullying victimization over time. This emphasizes the need to break the cyberbullying victimization–cyberbullying perpetration and cyberbullying victimization–depressive symptoms cycles to reduce cyberbullying victimization. Interventions targeting cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms are essential for adolescents who are victims of cyberbullying.
Additionally, the study also highlights that cognitive flexibility plays an important role in buffering the negative impact of being victimized by cyberbullying on the development of cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms. Because of this protective role of cognitive flexibility, training in this executive function should be included in cyberbullying prevention and intervention programs. This could contribute to diminishing the persistence of victimization by reducing cyberbullying perpetration and depressive symptoms. In fact, cognitive flexibility training is often included as part of some cognitive-behavioral therapy programs, such as the Unified Protocol-Adolescents, for the treatment of depression in adolescents (Ehrenreich-May et al.,
2017). The possible benefits of its extension to traditional bullying and cyberbullying programs should be considered in future research.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.