Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 3/2008

01-06-2008 | Original Article

Training Non-Threatening Interpretations in Spider Fear

Auteurs: Bethany A. Teachman, Lindsay M. Addison

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Uitgave 3/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

To evaluate a causal relationship between biased information processing and fear responding, as posited by many cognitive models of anxiety disorders, spider-relevant interpretations were trained to be non-threatening in an analog phobic sample. Participants high in spider fear (N = 61) were randomly assigned to a ‘Positive training’ condition, or to a ‘Neutral training’ or ‘No training’ control condition. ‘Positive training’ involved learning to ascribe non-threatening meanings to emotionally ambiguous scenarios. Results suggested this training was successful at inducing interpretation biases to be non-threatening, as indicated by faster responses to positive (versus negative) word fragments, as well as more positive and less negative interpretations of novel scenarios (relative to control conditions). Notwithstanding, the impact of training on subsequent avoidance and fear when presented with a live spider was minimal. No differences across training conditions were found; however, faster responding to positive word fragments predicted less avoidance and fear for participants receiving ‘Positive training’.
Voetnoten
1
There was a non-significant trend for higher baseline fear ratings in the Neutral training condition relative to the No training condition.
 
2
Note that the alternate analytic approach—conducting a three-way repeated measures MANOVA with training condition as the between-subjects factor, and valence (positive, negative) and spider-relevance (spider, non-spider) interpretation options as two separate within-subjects factors—was also significant (F(2, 58) = 34.63, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.54).
 
3
Thank you to an anonymous reviewer for this interesting suggestion.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Beard, C., Brady, R., & Amir, N. (2003). Malleability of interpretation bias in social anxiety. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Boston, MA. Beard, C., Brady, R., & Amir, N. (2003). Malleability of interpretation bias in social anxiety. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Boston, MA.
go back to reference Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: Automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 49–58.PubMedCrossRef Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: Automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 49–58.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. I. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias. New York: Basic Books. Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. I. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck depression inventory—II manual. San Antoino, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck depression inventory—II manual. San Antoino, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Chambless, D. L., & Gillis, M. M. (1993). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 248–260.PubMedCrossRef Chambless, D. L., & Gillis, M. M. (1993). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 248–260.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference de Jong, P. J., Merckelbach, H., & Arntz, A. (1995). Covariation bias in phobic women: The relationship between a priori expectancy, on-line expectancy, autonomic responding, and a posteriori contingency judgment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 55–62.PubMedCrossRef de Jong, P. J., Merckelbach, H., & Arntz, A. (1995). Covariation bias in phobic women: The relationship between a priori expectancy, on-line expectancy, autonomic responding, and a posteriori contingency judgment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 55–62.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference de Jong, P. J., & Muris, P. (2002). Spider phobia: Interaction of disgust and perceived likelihood of involuntary physical contact. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 51–65.PubMedCrossRef de Jong, P. J., & Muris, P. (2002). Spider phobia: Interaction of disgust and perceived likelihood of involuntary physical contact. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 51–65.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35.PubMedCrossRef Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grey, S., & Mathews, A. (2000). Effects of training on interpretation of emotional ambiguity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 1143–1162.PubMedCrossRef Grey, S., & Mathews, A. (2000). Effects of training on interpretation of emotional ambiguity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 1143–1162.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Harris, L. M., & Menzies, R. G. (1998). Changing attentional bias: Can it effect self-reported anxiety? Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 11, 167–179.CrossRef Harris, L. M., & Menzies, R. G. (1998). Changing attentional bias: Can it effect self-reported anxiety? Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 11, 167–179.CrossRef
go back to reference Hertel, P. T., Mathews, A., Peterson, S., & Kintner, K. (2003). Transfer of training emotionally biased interpretations. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 775–784.CrossRef Hertel, P. T., Mathews, A., Peterson, S., & Kintner, K. (2003). Transfer of training emotionally biased interpretations. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 775–784.CrossRef
go back to reference Klorman, R., Weerts, T. C., Hastings, J. E., Melamed, B. G., & Lang, P. J. (1974). Psychometric description of some specific-fear questionnaires. Behavior Therapy, 5, 401–409.CrossRef Klorman, R., Weerts, T. C., Hastings, J. E., Melamed, B. G., & Lang, P. J. (1974). Psychometric description of some specific-fear questionnaires. Behavior Therapy, 5, 401–409.CrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 107–123.PubMedCrossRef MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 107–123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (2000). Induced emotional interpretation bias and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 602–615.PubMedCrossRef Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (2000). Induced emotional interpretation bias and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 602–615.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 331–354.CrossRef Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 331–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Realini, J. P., & Katerndahl, D. A. (1993). Factors affecting threshold for seeking care. Journal of American Board of Family Practice, 6, 215–223. Realini, J. P., & Katerndahl, D. A. (1993). Factors affecting threshold for seeking care. Journal of American Board of Family Practice, 6, 215–223.
go back to reference Szymanski, J., & O’Donohue, W. (1995). Fear of Spiders Questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 31–34.PubMedCrossRef Szymanski, J., & O’Donohue, W. (1995). Fear of Spiders Questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 31–34.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Teachman, B. A., Gregg, A., Woody, S. (2001). Implicit attitudes toward fear-relevant stimuli in individuals with snake and spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 226–235.PubMedCrossRef Teachman, B. A., Gregg, A., Woody, S. (2001). Implicit attitudes toward fear-relevant stimuli in individuals with snake and spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 226–235.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Teachman, B. A., & Woody, S. (2003). Automatic processing among individuals with spider phobia: Change in implicit fear associations following treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 100–109.PubMedCrossRef Teachman, B. A., & Woody, S. (2003). Automatic processing among individuals with spider phobia: Change in implicit fear associations following treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 100–109.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vasey, M. W., Hazen, R., & Schmidt, N. B. (2002). Attentional retraining for chronic worry and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). Paper presented at the 36th annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Reno, NV. Vasey, M. W., Hazen, R., & Schmidt, N. B. (2002). Attentional retraining for chronic worry and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). Paper presented at the 36th annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Reno, NV.
go back to reference Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule (expanded form). IA: University of Iowa. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule (expanded form). IA: University of Iowa.
go back to reference Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester, England, UK: Wiley. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester, England, UK: Wiley.
go back to reference Wolpe, J., & Lang, P. J. (1964). A fear survey schedule for use in behaviour therapy. Behavior Research and Therapy, 2, 27–30.CrossRef Wolpe, J., & Lang, P. J. (1964). A fear survey schedule for use in behaviour therapy. Behavior Research and Therapy, 2, 27–30.CrossRef
go back to reference Yiend, J., Mackintosh, B., & Mathews, A. (2005). Enduring consequences of experimentally induced biases in interpretation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 779–797.PubMedCrossRef Yiend, J., Mackintosh, B., & Mathews, A. (2005). Enduring consequences of experimentally induced biases in interpretation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 779–797.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Training Non-Threatening Interpretations in Spider Fear
Auteurs
Bethany A. Teachman
Lindsay M. Addison
Publicatiedatum
01-06-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Uitgave 3/2008
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9084-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2008

Cognitive Therapy and Research 3/2008 Naar de uitgave