In the realm of conscious awareness, we navigate through diverse thought landscapes, continuously moulded by our interactions with our internal and external worlds (Mildner & Tamir,
2019; Smallwood,
2013). This phenomenon, what William James called our “stream of thought” occupie
2018s the majority of our waking life and is considered a key component of the brain’s default process (Buckner et al.,
2008; James,
2007; Killingsworth & Gilbert,
2010; Smith et al.,
2018 ). Recent decades have seen a burgeoning interest in understanding the nature of this ‘stream’, its contents, and implications (i.e., Belardi et al.,
2022; Callard et al.,
2013; Krans et al.,
2015; Sekiguchi,
2023; Seli, Risko, Smilek, & Schacter,
2016a; Smallwood & Schooler,
2006; Smith et al., 2018). This self-generated mental activity also goes by a variety of names and encompasses a variety of phenomena, including mind wandering, daydreaming, task-unrelated thought, spontaneous thought, stimulus-independent thought, fantasy, and imagination (Callard et al.,
2013; Mildner & Tamir,
2019; Seli et al.,
2018). A critical distinction among these phenomena is whether the thought occurs in a restricted or unrestricted context—that is, respectively, whether the thought occurs in spite of the presence of an external and often focal task or in the absence of such a task. The present study investigates the types of thoughts that arise in these contexts, what we call restricted and unrestricted mind wandering, using the presence or absence of a common thought-restricting task—a mindfulness task. Do the thoughts that emerge during restricted mind wandering differ from those that arise during unrestricted mind wandering?
Restricted and unrestricted mind wandering
Mind wandering is often defined as arising in what we refer to as restricted contexts. That is, mind wandering is the unintentional shifting of attention away from a primary task to self-generated and task-unrelated thoughts (Smallwood & Schooler,
2006). Various terms such as unintentional thought, undirected thought, unguided attention, and perceptual decoupling have been used to emphasize mind wandering’s spontaneous and involuntary nature (see Barnett and Kaufman (
2020); Blouin-Hudon and Zelenski (
2016); Schooler et al. (
2011); Irving
2016; Christoff
2012; Zedelius and Schooler (
2016)). In contrast, others have emphasized the importance of thoughts arising in unrestricted contexts, such as in the case of Singer’s positive constructive daydreaming, which R. McMillan et al. (
2013) described as “characterized by playful, wishful imagery, and planful, creative thought” (Dorsch,
2015; see also Klinger,
1978). Such thoughts are often considered deliberate and intentional cognitive activities, with individuals consciously selecting the focus and direction of their thoughts.
Restricted and unrestricted thoughts may diverge along various dimensions. For example, they may differ with respect to intentionality, deliberateness, and spontaneity (Christoff et al.,
2016; Dorsch,
2015; Irving,
2016; Mills et al.,
2018). However, to what extent restricted (e.g., mind wandering) or unrestricted (e.g., positive constructive daydreaming) thoughts fall into these categories is an area of active debate (Christoff et al.,
2016; Dorsch,
2015; e.g., Irving,
2016; Murray & Krasich,
2020). Nonetheless, the objective distinction remains with respect to whether the thoughts arose in a restricted or unrestricted context. Our research therefore shifts the focus away from the contentious issues of intentionality and spontaneity, focuses instead on qualities of thoughts that arise in these various contexts. That is, we are concerned not with the origins of thoughts in restricted or unrestricted contexts, but with their content, aiming to understand the content and character of these thoughts in each context.
This distinction poses intriguing questions: What types of thoughts occur during episodes of restricted versus unrestricted contexts? For instance, using survey measures, some studies have suggested that engaging in unrestricted thought might lead to more positive emotions and enhanced life satisfaction (Brenner et al.,
2022; Sugiura & Sugiura,
2020). In contrast, Killingsworth and Gilbert (
2010) used experience sampling with an online app to probe people’s thoughts throughout the day. In this large-scale study involving several thousand participants, they found that ‘a wandering mind’ (i.e., restricted thoughts arising during another task) were a sign of an unhappy mind. Relevant to our current query, however, their approach—did not differentiate between restricted and unrestricted thought. In other words, it may be the case that both restricted and unrestricted thoughts are similarly negatively valenced. By explicitly examining thoughts that arise during restricted and unrestricted contexts, this present study aims to evaluate the qualities of these thoughts and elucidate their differences.
Specifically, the positivity of thought quality might vary depending on its temporal focus. Klinger (
1978) observed that people’s minds wander through various topics, including worries and occurrences from the past, present, and future, as well as imaginary fantasies. Research has often found that restricted thoughts (so-called mind wandering) tends to be more future-oriented than the past-oriented (Smallwood & Schooler, 2015; Song & Wang,
2012; Stawarczyk et al.,
2011). However, Kvavilashvili and Rummel (2020) makes a distinction between spontaneous future thinking—which may arise in restricted contexts—versus deliberate future thinking—which may arise during unrestricted contexts. Notably, future thinking is proposed to involve more cognitive effort and may therefore be more likely during unrestricted than restricted thought (Schacter et al.,
2008; Smallwood et al.,
2009). This has been further supported by evidence that individuals with higher working-memory capacity have more future-oriented task-unrelated thoughts (Baird et al.,
2011). However, many of these studies rely on external judges to assess the future orientation of thoughts. In our study, we have participants self-assess their thoughts for future orientation.
Reality orientation is another aspect where restricted and unrestricted mind wandering might differ. Research suggests that off-task thoughts and imagination may activate similar brain regions (Villena-Gonzalez & Cosmelli,
2020). Moreover, the ability to guide imaginative thoughts has been linked with a greater likelihood of generating positive emotional outcomes (Holmes et al.,
2016). This positive tendency is usually more evident in self-directed, unrestricted thought, where imagination is an actively engaged component. This distinction fits with the broader concept of voluntary thoughts, highlighting their capacity for meaningful insights and positive impacts, as discussed in the literature (McMillan et al.,
2013).
Two common approaches to collecting thoughts in typical mind wandering research are self-caught and probe-caught (Schooler et al.,
2011; Smallwood & Schooler,
2015): The self-caught method involves participants reporting their spontaneous thoughts, while the probe-caught method interrupts tasks to inquire if participants are currently engaged in spontaneous thought processes (e.g., Killingsworth & Gilbert,
2010). Because we are interested in capturing a rich representation of individuals’ thoughts over a short interval of time, we will focus on self-caught thoughts. This allows us to capture more fully formed and concrete thoughts, avoiding the potential issue of incomplete thoughts often associated with the probe-caught approach. It also allows us to comprehensively capture thoughts as they occur during a short time period. However, we note that probe-caught and self-caught methods differ in their findings (Schooler et al.,
2004) and we do not argue that the self-caught approach can be generalized to probe-caught methods. Furthermore, while previous research has predominantly used the probe-caught method due to beliefs about individuals’ limited meta-awareness in reliably reporting spontaneous thoughts (Jackson & Balota,
2012; Schooler et al.,
2004), our study focuses on the characteristic differences between thoughts in restricted and unrestricted contexts, allowing us to use the same method in both contexts. Recent studies by Varao-Sousa and Kingstone (
2019) and Tortella-Feliu et al. (
2020) suggest that individuals are quite capable of noticing and reporting their mind wandering experiences, indicating the viability of the self-caught method for our research objectives.
To compare restricted mind wandering with unrestricted mind wandering, we use tasks that either encourage participants to limit off-task thoughts or to think freely without constraints. For restricting thoughts, we implement a mindfulness task following previous research (Arch & Craske,
2006; Chu et al.,
2023; Colzato et al.,
2012; Vago & David,
2012). As described above, mind wandering typically occurs spontaneously, leading to a shift in attention. In contrast, mindfulness is about intentionally focusing attention away from spontaneous thoughts to the present moment (Girardeau et al., 2020). Belardi et al. (
2022) has shown that mind wandering and mindfulness exist on opposite ends of a spectrum in terms of attention focus on the current moment (see also Mrazek et al.,
2012). Therefore, contrary to the goals of previous researchers, we instruct participants to maintain a mindful state, which inherently involves suppressing any thoughts other than their focal point. Any deviation from this focus is classified as an unrestricted thought.
Additionally, this approach mitigates potential confounding factors related to working memory. Prior research has established that individual differences in working memory affect the frequency of task-unrelated thoughts (i.e., Kane & McVay,
2012; McVay & Kane,
2010; Mcvay & Kane,
2011; Robison & Unsworth,
2017). Levinson et al. (
2012) found that using a breath-awareness task (a mindfulness task) did not result in differences in performance related to working memory capacity. Similarly, Meier (
2019) reported no correlation between working memory and task-unrelated thoughts during a mindfulness task. These findings suggest that employing a mindfulness task helps control for the influence of individual differences in working memory on mind wandering. The current study will adopt this method and, in addition, we will ask participants to summarise their self-caught thoughts in a few words.
For the unrestricted mind wandering task, inspired by Dorsch (
2015), participants will be encouraged to generate a series of self-directed thoughts, while withdrawing their attention from external stimuli. Instead of restricting their thoughts, participants will be instructed to freely explore their mental landscape, thinking anything that comes to mind, and then document each thought sequentially. This method enables the capture of a broad spectrum of naturally occurring thoughts.
The key distinction between restricted and unrestricted mind wandering in our study therefore lies in the nature of the tasks. Restricted mind wandering involves participants focusing on their breath, with the aim to directly inhibit thoughts and the linkage that may arise between one thought and the next. Conversely, in unrestricted mind wandering, there is an active directive to produce thoughts without inhibition. Participants are free to let their thoughts guide them, exploring their mental landscape without restrictions. Employing these distinct task contexts allows us to effectively study and compare the different types of thoughts that arise in restricted and unrestricted mind wandering.
Finally, to assess participants’ affects and understand how it influences different thought properties (i.e., Berntsen et al.,
2015; Brenner et al.,
2022; Marchetti et al.,
2014; Poerio et al.,
2013; Sugiura & Sugiura,
2020), we use the emotional recall task(ERT) and positive and negative affect scale (PANAS-SF). The ERT, designed by Li et al. (
2020), requires participants to list recent emotions and rate them in terms of frequency and valence. The PANAS-SF, created by Watson et al. (
1988), will be used to gain a more comprehensive picture of mood’s impact on the characteristics of thought.
In summary, to explore the distinctions between restricted and unrestricted mind wandering, we will evaluate thought characteristics in relation to valence, future orientation, and reality orientation. Further, we can compare thought qualities with participants’ affect measures.