Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2015

01-07-2015 | Original Paper

The Use of Sign Language Pronouns by Native-Signing Children with Autism

Auteurs: Aaron Shield, Richard P. Meier, Helen Tager-Flusberg

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 7/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We report the first study on pronoun use by an under-studied research population, children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) exposed to American Sign Language from birth by their deaf parents. Personal pronouns cause difficulties for hearing children with ASD, who sometimes reverse or avoid them. Unlike speech pronouns, sign pronouns are indexical points to self and other. Despite this transparency, we find evidence from an elicitation task and parental report that signing children with ASD avoid sign pronouns in favor of names. An analysis of spontaneous usage showed that all children demonstrated the ability to point, but only children with better-developed sign language produced pronouns. Differences in language abilities and self-representation may explain these phenomena in sign and speech.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Exceptions are the ASL signs we and our (Meier 1990).
 
2
As is conventional, we denote ASL signs with their English translations in small caps.
 
3
The linguistic status of personal pronouns in ASL and other signed languages has been a matter of continuing controversy (Cormier et al. 2013; McBurney 2002; Meier 1990; Meier and Lillo-Martin 2010). However, ASL unequivocally has points to self and points to addressee; for the purpose of this paper we label these points as first- and second-person pronouns.
 
4
Signed English is a system of manual signs that follows English grammar and thus is not considered an independent language.
 
5
We use non-parametric tests throughout this paper because assumptions of normality are violated.
 
6
We use Cohen’s d as a measure of effect size when comparing two means; a value greater than 0.8 typically represents a large effect.
 
7
A sign that functions as a unique name for a person, often invented by Deaf parents (Supalla 1992).
 
8
We also analyzed whether the group differences were significant in terms of who responded with names and who did not (rather than who produced pronouns and who did not), since some children produced both in their answer. The group difference was again significant under this criterion for both the first-person task (Fisher’s Exact Test, p < .05, one-tailed) and the second-person task (Fisher’s Exact Test, p < .0001, one-tailed).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
go back to reference Asendorpf, J. B. (2002). Self-awareness, other-awareness, and secondary representation. In A. N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: development, evolution, and brain bases. Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 63–73). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Asendorpf, J. B. (2002). Self-awareness, other-awareness, and secondary representation. In A. N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: development, evolution, and brain bases. Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 63–73). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113–127.CrossRef Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113–127.CrossRef
go back to reference Bates, E., & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 293–310.PubMedCrossRef Bates, E., & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 293–310.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beykirch, H. L., Holcomb, T. A., & Harrington, J. F. (1990). Iconicity and sign vocabulary acquisition. American Annals of the Deaf, 135, 306–311.PubMedCrossRef Beykirch, H. L., Holcomb, T. A., & Harrington, J. F. (1990). Iconicity and sign vocabulary acquisition. American Annals of the Deaf, 135, 306–311.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bischof-Kohler, D. (1994). Self-objectification and other-oriented emotions: Self-recognition, empathy, and prosocial behavior in the second year. Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie, 202, 349–377.PubMed Bischof-Kohler, D. (1994). Self-objectification and other-oriented emotions: Self-recognition, empathy, and prosocial behavior in the second year. Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie, 202, 349–377.PubMed
go back to reference Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (2010). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (4th ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (2010). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (4th ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
go back to reference Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., & Milone, A. (1997). A longitudinal examination of the communicative gestures deficit in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 715–725.PubMedCrossRef Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., & Milone, A. (1997). A longitudinal examination of the communicative gestures deficit in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 715–725.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., Parrinini, B., & Cesari, A. (2003). The communicative use of pointing in autism: Developmental profile and factors related to change. European Psychiatry, 18, 6–12.PubMedCrossRef Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., Parrinini, B., & Cesari, A. (2003). The communicative use of pointing in autism: Developmental profile and factors related to change. European Psychiatry, 18, 6–12.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carmody, D. P., & Lewis, M. (2012). Self representation in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43, 227–237.PubMedCrossRef Carmody, D. P., & Lewis, M. (2012). Self representation in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43, 227–237.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Casey, S. (2003). Relationships between gestures and signed languages: Indicating participants in action. In A. Baker, B. van den Bogaerde, & O. Crasborn (Eds.), Cross-linguistic perspectives in sign language research: Selected papers from TISLR 2000 (pp. 95–117). Hamburg: Signum. Casey, S. (2003). Relationships between gestures and signed languages: Indicating participants in action. In A. Baker, B. van den Bogaerde, & O. Crasborn (Eds.), Cross-linguistic perspectives in sign language research: Selected papers from TISLR 2000 (pp. 95–117). Hamburg: Signum.
go back to reference Charney, R. (1980). Pronoun errors in autistic children: Support for a social explanation. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 15, 39–43.CrossRef Charney, R. (1980). Pronoun errors in autistic children: Support for a social explanation. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 15, 39–43.CrossRef
go back to reference Chiat, S. (1982). If I were you and you were me: The analysis of pronouns in a pronoun-reversing child. Journal of Child Language, 9, 359–379.PubMedCrossRef Chiat, S. (1982). If I were you and you were me: The analysis of pronouns in a pronoun-reversing child. Journal of Child Language, 9, 359–379.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clark, E. V. (1978). From gesture to word: On the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In J. S. Bruner & A. Garton (Eds.), Human growth and development: Wolfson College lectures 1976 (pp. 85–120). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Clark, E. V. (1978). From gesture to word: On the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In J. S. Bruner & A. Garton (Eds.), Human growth and development: Wolfson College lectures 1976 (pp. 85–120). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Cormier, K., Schembri, A., & Woll, B. (2013). Pronouns and pointing in sign languages. Lingua, 137, 230–247.CrossRef Cormier, K., Schembri, A., & Woll, B. (2013). Pronouns and pointing in sign languages. Lingua, 137, 230–247.CrossRef
go back to reference Enns, C. J., Zimmer, K., Boudreault, P., Rabu, S., & Broszeit, C. (2013). American Sign Language: Receptive Skills Test. Winnipeg, MB: Northern Signs Research Inc. Enns, C. J., Zimmer, K., Boudreault, P., Rabu, S., & Broszeit, C. (2013). American Sign Language: Receptive Skills Test. Winnipeg, MB: Northern Signs Research Inc.
go back to reference Evans, K. E., & Demuth, K. (2012). Individual differences in pronoun reversal: Evidence from two longitudinal case studies. Journal of Child Language, 39, 162–191.PubMedCrossRef Evans, K. E., & Demuth, K. (2012). Individual differences in pronoun reversal: Evidence from two longitudinal case studies. Journal of Child Language, 39, 162–191.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hatzopoulou, M. (2010). Acquisition of reference to self and others in Greek Sign Language (Stockholm University, 2008). Sign Language & Linguistics, 13, 83–91.CrossRef Hatzopoulou, M. (2010). Acquisition of reference to self and others in Greek Sign Language (Stockholm University, 2008). Sign Language & Linguistics, 13, 83–91.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoza, J. (2011). The discourse and politeness functions of HEY and WELL in American Sign Language. In C. B. Roy (Ed.), Discourse in signed languages (pp. 69–95). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press. Hoza, J. (2011). The discourse and politeness functions of HEY and WELL in American Sign Language. In C. B. Roy (Ed.), Discourse in signed languages (pp. 69–95). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
go back to reference Jackson, C. A. (1989). Language acquisition in two modalities: The role of nonlinguistic cues in linguistic mastery. Sign Language Studies, 62, 1–22.CrossRef Jackson, C. A. (1989). Language acquisition in two modalities: The role of nonlinguistic cues in linguistic mastery. Sign Language Studies, 62, 1–22.CrossRef
go back to reference Johnston, T. (2013). Towards a comparative semiotics of pointing actions in signed andspoken languages. Gesture, 13, 109–142.CrossRef Johnston, T. (2013). Towards a comparative semiotics of pointing actions in signed andspoken languages. Gesture, 13, 109–142.CrossRef
go back to reference Jordan, R. (1989). An experimental comparison of the understanding and use of speaker-addressee personal pronouns in autistic children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24, 169–179.PubMedCrossRef Jordan, R. (1989). An experimental comparison of the understanding and use of speaker-addressee personal pronouns in autistic children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24, 169–179.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jure, R., Rapin, I., & Tuchman, R. (1991). Hearing-impaired autistic children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 1062–1072.PubMedCrossRef Jure, R., Rapin, I., & Tuchman, R. (1991). Hearing-impaired autistic children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 1062–1072.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
go back to reference Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Konstantareas, M. M., Oxman, J., & Webster, C. D. (1982). The representational and information-processing foundations of linguistic functioning in autistic children. Neurolinguistics, 11, 93–123. Konstantareas, M. M., Oxman, J., & Webster, C. D. (1982). The representational and information-processing foundations of linguistic functioning in autistic children. Neurolinguistics, 11, 93–123.
go back to reference Lee, A., Hobson, R. P., & Chiat, S. (1994). I, you, me, and autism: An experimental study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 155–176.PubMedCrossRef Lee, A., Hobson, R. P., & Chiat, S. (1994). I, you, me, and autism: An experimental study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 155–176.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRef Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2004). Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the 2nd year. Child Development, 75, 1821–1831.PubMedCrossRef Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2004). Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the 2nd year. Child Development, 75, 1821–1831.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, (ADOS-2) (2nd ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, (ADOS-2) (2nd ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Mayberry, R. I., & Eichen, E. B. (1991). The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 486–512.CrossRef Mayberry, R. I., & Eichen, E. B. (1991). The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 486–512.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayberry, R. I., Lock, E., & Kazmi, H. (2002). Development: Linguistic ability and early language exposure. Nature, 417, 38.PubMedCrossRef Mayberry, R. I., Lock, E., & Kazmi, H. (2002). Development: Linguistic ability and early language exposure. Nature, 417, 38.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McBurney, S. (2002). Pronominal reference in signed and spoken language: Are grammatical categories modality-dependent? In R. P. Meier, K. Cormier, & D. Quinto-Pozos (Eds.), Modality and structure in signed and spoken languages (pp. 329–369). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. McBurney, S. (2002). Pronominal reference in signed and spoken language: Are grammatical categories modality-dependent? In R. P. Meier, K. Cormier, & D. Quinto-Pozos (Eds.), Modality and structure in signed and spoken languages (pp. 329–369). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Meier, R. P. (1982). Icons, analogues and morphemes: The acquisition of verb agreement in ASL (Doctoral Dissertation). San Diego: University of California. Meier, R. P. (1982). Icons, analogues and morphemes: The acquisition of verb agreement in ASL (Doctoral Dissertation). San Diego: University of California.
go back to reference Meier, R. P. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in American Sign Language: Iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 26(3), 362–376.CrossRef Meier, R. P. (1987). Elicited imitation of verb agreement in American Sign Language: Iconically or morphologically determined? Journal of Memory and Language, 26(3), 362–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Meier, R. P. (1990). Person deixis in American Sign Language. In S. D. Fischer & P. Siple (Eds.), Theoretical issues in sign language research: Linguistics (Vol. 1, pp. 175–190). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Meier, R. P. (1990). Person deixis in American Sign Language. In S. D. Fischer & P. Siple (Eds.), Theoretical issues in sign language research: Linguistics (Vol. 1, pp. 175–190). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Meier, R. P. (2002). The acquisition of verb agreement: Pointing out arguments for the linguistic status of agreement in signed languages. In G. Morgan & B. Woll (Eds.), Directions in sign language acquisition (pp. 115–141). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef Meier, R. P. (2002). The acquisition of verb agreement: Pointing out arguments for the linguistic status of agreement in signed languages. In G. Morgan & B. Woll (Eds.), Directions in sign language acquisition (pp. 115–141). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
go back to reference Meier, R. P., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2010). Does spatial make it special? On the grammar of pointing signs in American Sign Language. In D. B. Gerdts, J. Moore, & M. Polinsky (Eds.), Hypothesis A/hypothesis B: Linguistic explorations in honor of David M. Perlmutter (pp. 345–360). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Meier, R. P., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2010). Does spatial make it special? On the grammar of pointing signs in American Sign Language. In D. B. Gerdts, J. Moore, & M. Polinsky (Eds.), Hypothesis A/hypothesis B: Linguistic explorations in honor of David M. Perlmutter (pp. 345–360). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Meinzen-Derr, J., Wiley, S., Bishop, S., Manning-Courtney, P., Choo, D. I., & Murray, D. (2014). Autism spectrum disorders in 24 children who are deaf or hard of hearing. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78, 112–118.PubMedCrossRef Meinzen-Derr, J., Wiley, S., Bishop, S., Manning-Courtney, P., Choo, D. I., & Murray, D. (2014). Autism spectrum disorders in 24 children who are deaf or hard of hearing. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78, 112–118.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morgan, G., Barriere, I., & Woll, B. (2006). The influence of typology and modality on the acquisition of verb agreement morphology in British Sign Language. First Language, 26, 19–43.CrossRef Morgan, G., Barriere, I., & Woll, B. (2006). The influence of typology and modality on the acquisition of verb agreement morphology in British Sign Language. First Language, 26, 19–43.CrossRef
go back to reference Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. P. (1985). The acquisition of American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. The data (Vol. 1, pp. 881–938). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. P. (1985). The acquisition of American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. The data (Vol. 1, pp. 881–938). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationship conceptualization, and gesture imitations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 45–62.PubMedCrossRef Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationship conceptualization, and gesture imitations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 45–62.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oshima-Takane, Y. (1992). Analysis of pronominal errors: A case-study. Journal of Child Language, 19, 111–131.PubMedCrossRef Oshima-Takane, Y. (1992). Analysis of pronominal errors: A case-study. Journal of Child Language, 19, 111–131.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oshima-Takane, Y., & Benaroya, S. (1989). An alternative view of pronominal errors in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 73–85.PubMedCrossRef Oshima-Takane, Y., & Benaroya, S. (1989). An alternative view of pronominal errors in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 73–85.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Padden, C. (1983). Interaction of morphology and syntax in American Sign Language (Doctoral Dissertation). San Diego: University of California. Padden, C. (1983). Interaction of morphology and syntax in American Sign Language (Doctoral Dissertation). San Diego: University of California.
go back to reference Petitto, L. A. (1983). From gesture to symbol: The relationship between form and meaning in the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 22, 100–107. Petitto, L. A. (1983). From gesture to symbol: The relationship between form and meaning in the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 22, 100–107.
go back to reference Petitto, L. A. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27, 1–52.PubMedCrossRef Petitto, L. A. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27, 1–52.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pizzuto, E. (1990). The early development of deixis in American Sign Language: What is the point? In V. Volterra & C. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 142–162). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.CrossRef Pizzuto, E. (1990). The early development of deixis in American Sign Language: What is the point? In V. Volterra & C. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 142–162). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Ritvo, E. R., Ritvo, R., Freeman, B. J., & Mason-Brothers, A. (1994). Clinical characteristics of mild autism in adults. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 35, 149–156.PubMedCrossRef Ritvo, E. R., Ritvo, R., Freeman, B. J., & Mason-Brothers, A. (1994). Clinical characteristics of mild autism in adults. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 35, 149–156.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.CrossRef Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.CrossRef
go back to reference Roper, L., Arnold, P., & Monteiro, B. (2003). Co-occurrence of autism and deafness: Diagnostic considerations. Autism, 7, 245–253.PubMedCrossRef Roper, L., Arnold, P., & Monteiro, B. (2003). Co-occurrence of autism and deafness: Diagnostic considerations. Autism, 7, 245–253.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78, 376–396.PubMedCrossRef Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78, 376–396.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schiff-Myers, N. B. (1983). From pronoun reversals to correct pronoun usage: A case study of a normally developing child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 394–402.PubMedCrossRef Schiff-Myers, N. B. (1983). From pronoun reversals to correct pronoun usage: A case study of a normally developing child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 394–402.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schlosser, R. W., & Wendt, O. (2008). Effects of augmentative and alternative communication intervention on speech production in children with autism: A systematic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 212.PubMedCrossRef Schlosser, R. W., & Wendt, O. (2008). Effects of augmentative and alternative communication intervention on speech production in children with autism: A systematic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 212.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shield, A., & Meier, R. P. (2012). Palm reversal errors in native-signing children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45, 439–454. Shield, A., & Meier, R. P. (2012). Palm reversal errors in native-signing children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45, 439–454.
go back to reference Smiley, P. A., Chang, L. K., & Allhoff, A. K. (2011). Can Toddy give me an orange? Parent input and young children’s production of I and You. Language Learning and Development, 7, 77–106.CrossRef Smiley, P. A., Chang, L. K., & Allhoff, A. K. (2011). Can Toddy give me an orange? Parent input and young children’s production of I and You. Language Learning and Development, 7, 77–106.CrossRef
go back to reference Smith, I. M. (1998). Gesture imitation in autism I: Nonsymbolic postures and sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 747–770.PubMedCrossRef Smith, I. M. (1998). Gesture imitation in autism I: Nonsymbolic postures and sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 747–770.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Supalla, S. J. (1992). The book of name signs: Naming in American Sign Language. San Diego, CA: DawnSignPress. Supalla, S. J. (1992). The book of name signs: Naming in American Sign Language. San Diego, CA: DawnSignPress.
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Dissociations in form and function in the acquisition of language by autistic children. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Constraints on language acquisition: Studies of atypical children (pp. 175–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Dissociations in form and function in the acquisition of language by autistic children. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Constraints on language acquisition: Studies of atypical children (pp. 175–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H., & Kasari, C. (2013). Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder: The neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Research, 6, 468–478.PubMedCrossRef Tager-Flusberg, H., & Kasari, C. (2013). Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder: The neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Research, 6, 468–478.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thompson, R. L., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). The link between form and meaning in American Sign Language: Lexical processing effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 550–557.PubMedCentralPubMed Thompson, R. L., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). The link between form and meaning in American Sign Language: Lexical processing effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 550–557.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Vinson, D. P., Cormier, K., Denmark, T., Schembri, A., & Vigliocco, G. (2008). The British Sign Language (BSL) norms for age of acquisition, familiarity, and iconicity. Behavioral Research Methods, 40, 1079–1087.CrossRef Vinson, D. P., Cormier, K., Denmark, T., Schembri, A., & Vigliocco, G. (2008). The British Sign Language (BSL) norms for age of acquisition, familiarity, and iconicity. Behavioral Research Methods, 40, 1079–1087.CrossRef
go back to reference Whiten, A., & Brown, J. (1998). Imitation and the reading of other minds: Perspectives from the study of autism, normal children and non-human primates. In S. Bråten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 260–280). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Whiten, A., & Brown, J. (1998). Imitation and the reading of other minds: Perspectives from the study of autism, normal children and non-human primates. In S. Bråten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 260–280). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
The Use of Sign Language Pronouns by Native-Signing Children with Autism
Auteurs
Aaron Shield
Richard P. Meier
Helen Tager-Flusberg
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 7/2015
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2377-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2015

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2015 Naar de uitgave