Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2018

07-11-2016 | Original Article

The test of both worlds: identifying feature binding and control processes in congruency sequence tasks by means of action dynamics

Auteurs: Stefan Scherbaum, Simon Frisch, Maja Dshemuchadse, Matthias Rudolf, Rico Fischer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Cognitive control processes enable us to act flexibly in a world posing ever-changing demands on our cognitive system. To study cognitive control, conflict tasks and especially congruency sequence effects have been regarded as a fruitful tool. However, for the last decade a dispute has arisen whether or not congruency sequence effects are indeed a valid measure of cognitive control processes. This debate has led to the development of increasingly complex paradigms involving numerous, intricately designed experimental conditions which are aimed at excluding low-level, associative learning mechanisms like feature binding as an alternative explanation for the emergence of congruency sequence effects. Here, we try to go beyond this all-or-nothing thinking by investigating the assumption that both cognitive control processes as well as feature binding mechanisms occur within trials of the same task. Based on a theoretical dual-route-model of behavior under conflict, we show that both classes of cognitive mechanisms should affect behavior at different points of the decision process. By comparing these predictions to continuous mouse movements from an adapted Simon task, we find evidence that control processes and feature binding mechanisms do indeed coexist within the task but that they follow distinct timing patterns. We argue that this dynamic approach to cognitive processing opens up new ways to investigate the diversity of co-existing processes that contribute to the selection of behavior.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
It must be noted that substantial effort has been put into the identification of the temporal characteristics of control versus feature binding influences on congruency sequence effects by analyzing temporal dynamics across trials (e.g., aftereffects of conflict processing in various inter-trial intervals, Egner, Ely, & Grinband, 2010; see also Mayr & Awh, 2009; Notebaert, Gevers, Verbruggen, & Liefooghe, 2006), while our approach focusses on these influences within trials.
 
2
Note, that this prediction on conflict adaptation across trials is independent of another argument derived from dual route models that refers to conflict solution within the trial: analyses of response time distributions in the Simon task—so called delta plots—show reduced Simon effects for slower trials (Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Wijnen, & Burle, 2004), which has been interpreted to indicate a temporal lag in the solution of conflict within the current trial, e.g. by inhibitory processes.
 
3
In the following, complete repetitions will refer to trials in which the compound of stimulus identity (individual digit) and stimulus location (and hence congruency and response) are identical to the previous trial.
 
4
Since we use a two-response version of the Simon task, one might argue that full switches in a strict sense are impossible. However, due to our combination of eight stimuli and three conditions of congruency (i.e., congruent, neutral, and incongruent), the task already offers a high number of dimensions to switch between, so that full switches will refer to trials that switch in location, demanded response (and hence stimulus) and congruency compared to the previous trial.
 
5
We apply temporal smoothing for three reasons (similar to spatial smoothing in fMRI analysis; e.g. Mikl et al., 2008): first, it increases the signal to noise ratio. Since the movement angle is a differential measure, it shows a higher level of noise than raw movement data; this noise is reduced by smoothing. Second, smoothing improves the validity of statistical tests since it makes error distributions more normal. Third, it accommodates slight temporal variations between subjects. For temporal smoothing, we used a Gaussian kernel of ten time steps.
 
6
While a similar analysis could also be performed with multi-level models (see e.g. Mirman, Dixon, & Magnuson, 2008), we use the simpler regression approach as our main interest is in the analysis of within-subject variance. However, we validated our results by an additional hierarchical linear model analysis of decisive time-steps. This analysis showed no qualitative difference in the resulting beta-weights (see supplementary material), supporting our simpler approach.
 
7
Adding higher order contingencies, e.g. different kinds of partial repetitions or interaction terms, leads to strong correlations (and hence invalid levels of multicollinearity) of these new regressors with conflict adaptation. This is a particular concern the simpler the design of the task and offers typical criticism by proponents of feature binding accounts—we deliberately accepted this limitation as our focus is on unravelling specific influences and their timing patterns based on a theoretical approach instead of explaining all variance that could be present in the data. A consequence of the strong correlations of any additional terms is that performing the analysis with such regressors is methodologically infeasible and does not increase the quality of the statistical model as indicated by a constant R 2.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Band, G. P. H., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Van Der Molen, M. W. (2003). Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: The roles of activation and inhibition. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 67(4), 266–279.CrossRefPubMed Band, G. P. H., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Van Der Molen, M. W. (2003). Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: The roles of activation and inhibition. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 67(4), 266–279.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Blais, C., Stefanidi, A., & Brewer, G. A. (2014). The Gratton effect remains after controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions. Cognition, 5, 1207. Blais, C., Stefanidi, A., & Brewer, G. A. (2014). The Gratton effect remains after controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions. Cognition, 5, 1207.
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.CrossRefPubMed Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2014). The computational and neural basis of cognitive control: Charted territory and new frontiers. Cognitive Science, 38(6), 1249–1285.CrossRefPubMed Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2014). The computational and neural basis of cognitive control: Charted territory and new frontiers. Cognitive Science, 38(6), 1249–1285.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8(12), 539–546.CrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8(12), 539–546.CrossRef
go back to reference Dale, R., Kehoe, C., & Spivey, M. J. (2007). Graded motor responses in the time course of categorizing atypical exemplars. Memory and Cognition, 35(1), 15–28.CrossRefPubMed Dale, R., Kehoe, C., & Spivey, M. J. (2007). Graded motor responses in the time course of categorizing atypical exemplars. Memory and Cognition, 35(1), 15–28.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Davelaar, E. J. (2008). A computational study of conflict-monitoring at two levels of processing: Reaction time distributional analyses and hemodynamic responses. Brain Research, 1202,109–119. Davelaar, E. J. (2008). A computational study of conflict-monitoring at two levels of processing: Reaction time distributional analyses and hemodynamic responses. Brain Research, 1202,109–119.
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731–750.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731–750.PubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Dshemuchadse, M., Grage, T., & Scherbaum, S. (2015). Action dynamics reveal two components of cognitive flexibility in a homonym relatedness judgement task. Frontiers in Cognition, 6, 1244. Dshemuchadse, M., Grage, T., & Scherbaum, S. (2015). Action dynamics reveal two components of cognitive flexibility in a homonym relatedness judgement task. Frontiers in Cognition, 6, 1244.
go back to reference Dshemuchadse, M., Scherbaum, S., & Goschke, T. (2012). How decisions emerge: Action dynamics in intertemporal decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 151–185. Dshemuchadse, M., Scherbaum, S., & Goschke, T. (2012). How decisions emerge: Action dynamics in intertemporal decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 151–185.
go back to reference Duthoo, W., Abrahamse, E. L., Braem, S., Boehler, C. N., & Notebaert, W. (2014). The congruency sequence effect 3.0: A critical test of conflict adaptation. PLoS One, 9(10), e110462.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Duthoo, W., Abrahamse, E. L., Braem, S., Boehler, C. N., & Notebaert, W. (2014). The congruency sequence effect 3.0: A critical test of conflict adaptation. PLoS One, 9(10), e110462.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 380–390.CrossRef Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 380–390.CrossRef
go back to reference Egner, T. (2014). Creatures of habit (and control): A multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Cognition, 5, 1247. Egner, T. (2014). Creatures of habit (and control): A multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Cognition, 5, 1247.
go back to reference Egner, T., Ely, S., & Grinband, J. (2010). Going, going, gone: Characterizing the time-course of congruency sequence effects. Frontiers in Cognition, 1, 154. Egner, T., Ely, S., & Grinband, J. (2010). Going, going, gone: Characterizing the time-course of congruency sequence effects. Frontiers in Cognition, 1, 154.
go back to reference Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task. Neuroimage, 24(2), 539–547.CrossRefPubMed Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task. Neuroimage, 24(2), 539–547.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, R., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2008). Context-sensitive adjustments of cognitive control: Conflict-adaptation effects are modulated by processing demands of the ongoing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 712–718.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, R., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2008). Context-sensitive adjustments of cognitive control: Conflict-adaptation effects are modulated by processing demands of the ongoing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 712–718.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, R., & Plessow, F. (2015). Efficient multitasking: Parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1366. Fischer, R., & Plessow, F. (2015). Efficient multitasking: Parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1366.
go back to reference Fischer, R., Plessow, F., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2015). Individual differences in the context-dependent recruitment of cognitive control: Evidence from action versus state orientation. Journal of Personality, 83(5), 575–583.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, R., Plessow, F., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2015). Individual differences in the context-dependent recruitment of cognitive control: Evidence from action versus state orientation. Journal of Personality, 83(5), 575–583.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, R., & Schubert, T. (2008). Valence processing bypassing the response selection bottleneck? Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Experimental Psychology, 55(3), 203–211.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, R., & Schubert, T. (2008). Valence processing bypassing the response selection bottleneck? Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Experimental Psychology, 55(3), 203–211.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frisch, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Görner, M., Goschke, T., & Scherbaum, S. (2015). Unraveling the sub-processes of selective attention: insights from dynamic modeling and continuous behavior. Cognitive Processing, 16(4), 377–388.CrossRefPubMed Frisch, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Görner, M., Goschke, T., & Scherbaum, S. (2015). Unraveling the sub-processes of selective attention: insights from dynamic modeling and continuous behavior. Cognitive Processing, 16(4), 377–388.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 480–506.CrossRef Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 480–506.CrossRef
go back to reference Haazebroek, P., van Dantzig, S., & Hommel, B. (2011). A computational model of perception and action for cognitive robotics. Cognitive Processing, 12(4), 355–365.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Haazebroek, P., van Dantzig, S., & Hommel, B. (2011). A computational model of perception and action for cognitive robotics. Cognitive Processing, 12(4), 355–365.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56(4), 261–268.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56(4), 261–268.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998a). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1–2), 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998a). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1–2), 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998b). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 1368–1384.PubMed Hommel, B. (1998b). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 1368–1384.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2002). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–878.CrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2002). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–878.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1–17.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1–17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132(1), 30–43.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132(1), 30–43.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303(5660), 1023–1026.CrossRefPubMed Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303(5660), 1023–1026.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koch, I., & Prinz, W. (2002). Process interference and code overlap in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(1), 192–201. Koch, I., & Prinz, W. (2002). Process interference and code overlap in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(1), 192–201.
go back to reference Koop, G. J., & Johnson, J. G. (2011). Response dynamics: A new window on the decision process. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(8), 750–758. Koop, G. J., & Johnson, J. G. (2011). Response dynamics: A new window on the decision process. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(8), 750–758.
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.CrossRefPubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kutner, M., Nachtsheim, C., & Neter, J. (2004). Applied linear regression methods (4th ed.). Chicago: McGraw-Hill/Irwin. Kutner, M., Nachtsheim, C., & Neter, J. (2004). Applied linear regression methods (4th ed.). Chicago: McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
go back to reference Larson, M. J., Clayson, P. E., Kirwan, C. B., & Weissman, D. H. (2016). Event-related potential indices of congruency sequence effects without feature integration or contingency learning confounds. Psychophysiology, 53(6), 814–822. Larson, M. J., Clayson, P. E., Kirwan, C. B., & Weissman, D. H. (2016). Event-related potential indices of congruency sequence effects without feature integration or contingency learning confounds. Psychophysiology, 53(6), 814–822.
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1072–1090.PubMed Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1072–1090.PubMed
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Awh, E. (2009). The elusive link between conflict and conflict adaptation. Psychological Research, 73(6), 794–802.CrossRefPubMed Mayr, U., & Awh, E. (2009). The elusive link between conflict and conflict adaptation. Psychological Research, 73(6), 794–802.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6(5), 450–452.CrossRefPubMed Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6(5), 450–452.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McKinstry, C., Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2008). Action dynamics reveal parallel competition in decision making. Psychological Science, 19(1), 22–24.CrossRefPubMed McKinstry, C., Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2008). Action dynamics reveal parallel competition in decision making. Psychological Science, 19(1), 22–24.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mikl, M., Marecek, R., Hlustík, P., Pavlicová, M., Drastich, A., Chlebus, P., et al. (2008). Effects of spatial smoothing on fMRI group inferences. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 26(4), 490–503.CrossRefPubMed Mikl, M., Marecek, R., Hlustík, P., Pavlicová, M., Drastich, A., Chlebus, P., et al. (2008). Effects of spatial smoothing on fMRI group inferences. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 26(4), 490–503.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, Special Issue: Emerging Data Analysis, 59(4), 475–494.CrossRef Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, Special Issue: Emerging Data Analysis, 59(4), 475–494.CrossRef
go back to reference Navon, D., & Miller, J. (2002). Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychology, 44(3), 193–251.CrossRefPubMed Navon, D., & Miller, J. (2002). Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychology, 44(3), 193–251.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verbruggen, F., & Liefooghe, B. (2006). Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(1), 112–117.CrossRefPubMed Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verbruggen, F., & Liefooghe, B. (2006). Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(1), 112–117.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2007). Dissociating conflict adaptation from feature integration: A multiple regression approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1256–1260.PubMed Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2007). Dissociating conflict adaptation from feature integration: A multiple regression approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1256–1260.PubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(3), 358–377.PubMed Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(3), 358–377.PubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220–244.CrossRefPubMed Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220–244.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.CrossRefPubMed Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3218–3227.CrossRefPubMed Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3218–3227.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2006). Stimulus–response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2006). Stimulus–response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro-and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66(4), 312–323.CrossRefPubMed Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro-and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66(4), 312–323.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004a). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306(5695), 443–447.CrossRefPubMed Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004a). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306(5695), 443–447.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Wijnen, J., & Burle, B. (2004b). Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots. In M. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 369–377). New York: Guilford Press. Ridderinkhof, K., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Wijnen, J., & Burle, B. (2004b). Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots. In M. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 369–377). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2010). How decisions evolve: The temporal dynamics of action selection. Cognition, 115(3), 407–416.CrossRefPubMed Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2010). How decisions evolve: The temporal dynamics of action selection. Cognition, 115(3), 407–416.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., & Kalis, A. (2008). Making decisions with a continuous mind. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(4), 454–474.CrossRef Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., & Kalis, A. (2008). Making decisions with a continuous mind. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(4), 454–474.CrossRef
go back to reference Scherbaum, S., Gottschalk, C., Dshemuchadse, M., & Fischer, R. (2015). Action dynamics in multitasking: The impact of additional task factors on the execution of the prioritized motor movement. Frontiers in Cognition, 6, 934. Scherbaum, S., Gottschalk, C., Dshemuchadse, M., & Fischer, R. (2015). Action dynamics in multitasking: The impact of additional task factors on the execution of the prioritized motor movement. Frontiers in Cognition, 6, 934.
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R. (2013). Questioning conflict adaptation: Proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(4), 615–630.CrossRefPubMed Schmidt, J. R. (2013). Questioning conflict adaptation: Proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(4), 615–630.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R., De Schryver, M., & Weissman, D. H. (2014). Removing the influence of feature repetitions on the congruency sequence effect: Why regressing out confounds from a nested design will often fall short. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(6), 2392–2402.PubMed Schmidt, J. R., De Schryver, M., & Weissman, D. H. (2014). Removing the influence of feature repetitions on the congruency sequence effect: Why regressing out confounds from a nested design will often fall short. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(6), 2392–2402.PubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174–176.CrossRefPubMed Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174–176.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R., Acosta, E., Mewaldt, S. P., & Speidel, C. R. (1976). The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: Duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing. Perception and Psychophysics, 19(1), 16–22.CrossRef Simon, J. R., Acosta, E., Mewaldt, S. P., & Speidel, C. R. (1976). The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: Duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing. Perception and Psychophysics, 19(1), 16–22.CrossRef
go back to reference Song, J. H., & Nakayama, K. (2008). Numeric comparison in a visually-guided manual reaching task. Cognition, 106(2), 994–1003.CrossRefPubMed Song, J. H., & Nakayama, K. (2008). Numeric comparison in a visually-guided manual reaching task. Cognition, 106(2), 994–1003.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Song, J. H., & Nakayama, K. (2009). Hidden cognitive states revealed in choice reaching tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 360–366.CrossRefPubMed Song, J. H., & Nakayama, K. (2009). Hidden cognitive states revealed in choice reaching tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 360–366.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spapé, M. M., & Hommel, B. (2014). Sequential modulations of the Simon effect depend on episodic retrieval. Cognition, 5, 855. Spapé, M. M., & Hommel, B. (2014). Sequential modulations of the Simon effect depend on episodic retrieval. Cognition, 5, 855.
go back to reference Spivey, M. J. (2007). The continuity of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Spivey, M. J. (2007). The continuity of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Spivey, M. J., & Dale, R. (2004). On the continuity of mind: Toward a dynamical account of cognition. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, 45, 87–142.CrossRef Spivey, M. J., & Dale, R. (2004). On the continuity of mind: Toward a dynamical account of cognition. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, 45, 87–142.CrossRef
go back to reference Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(29), 10393–10398.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(29), 10393–10398.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schroter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(6), 1345–1363.PubMed Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schroter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(6), 1345–1363.PubMed
go back to reference Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict-adaptation effect: it’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 467–472.CrossRef Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict-adaptation effect: it’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 467–472.CrossRef
go back to reference Weissman, D. H., Jiang, J., & Egner, T. (2014). Determinants of congruency sequence effects without learning and memory confounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 2022–2037.PubMed Weissman, D. H., Jiang, J., & Egner, T. (2014). Determinants of congruency sequence effects without learning and memory confounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 2022–2037.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
The test of both worlds: identifying feature binding and control processes in congruency sequence tasks by means of action dynamics
Auteurs
Stefan Scherbaum
Simon Frisch
Maja Dshemuchadse
Matthias Rudolf
Rico Fischer
Publicatiedatum
07-11-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0823-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2018

Psychological Research 2/2018 Naar de uitgave