Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2019

19-12-2018 | Original Article

The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: an exploratory study

Auteurs: Adriana del Palacio-Gonzalez, Dorthe Berntsen

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Involuntary mental time travel (MTT) refers to projecting oneself into the past or into the future without prior conscious effort. The previous studies have shown high inter-individual variability in the frequency of involuntary MTT, but a few systematic studies exist. In three exploratory studies, we investigated the relation between individual differences in experiencing involuntary past and future MTT, and selected emotional and cognitive processes, with a special focus on thought suppression. Across all three studies, thought suppression emerged as a robust predictor of involuntary MTT above and beyond emotion-related variables, mind-wandering, daydreaming styles, and demographic variables. Findings from Studies 1 and 2 showed that higher thought suppression consistently predicted both more frequent involuntary past and future MTT across an American and a Danish sample, whereas rumination and emotion regulation were less consistently related to involuntary MTT. In Study 3, thought suppression reliably predicted more frequent involuntary MTT, even when controlling for mind-wandering, as well as for positive and negative daydreaming styles, which were all related to greater involuntary MTT. Overall, the individual differences assessed showed similar relationships to the tendency for having past and future involuntary MTT, with the possible exception of daydreaming styles, which appeared more strongly related to future-directed involuntary MTT.
Voetnoten
1
The following steps were taken to increase the reliability of the data collection process. First, the entire survey consisted of 275 single items, and thus, it was determined that spending a minimum of 8 min answering the survey was a mandatory inclusion criterion (1.5 s per item plus instructions). Note that some of the questionnaires in the survey were not analyzed for the present study; however, the 8-min rule was based on the entire set of questionnaires completed by participants. Second, an attention check was inserted in the IAMI collected via Amazon MTurk (“If you are reading this item attentively, select the last option ‘Once an hour or more’”). Of the original 510 participants, 42 (8.2%) completed the survey in less than 8 min. Forty additional participants (7.8% of the 510) did not pass the attention check, thus resulting in N = 428.
 
2
The entire survey consisted of 275 single items. All Danish participants took 8 min or more in the survey (N = 481); thus, we did not eliminate anybody because of speed. We did not employ an attention check for the Danish participants; thus, data from all 481 Danish participants were analyzed. The data from this sample derived from two other larger projects and, thus, the questionnaires were given in two different orders. (Analyses not overlapping with the present study are published in del Palacio-Gonzalez & Berntsen, 2018b.)
 
3
The data preparation for Study 3 followed the same steps as Study 1. With 127 items, we estimated that the minimum time required to answer the survey was 4 min. Note that the entire survey was shorter than that of Studies 1 and 2, and we analyzed data from all questionnaires. Initially, 567 participants accessed the survey, of which 7.9% (n = 45) were too fast. An additional 5.6 % of the original (n = 32) did not pass the attention check, thus leaving a final sample of 490 participants for analysis.
 
4
The variance corresponds to the ΔR2 and sr2 explained by the MWQ in an additional model in which the MWQ was entered alone after the SIPI subscales. The model is not reported in the manuscript, but is available upon request.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 974–983.PubMedCrossRef Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 974–983.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Alle, M., Berna, F., & Berntsen, D. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memory and future thought predicting hallucination proneness. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(6), 891–898.CrossRef Alle, M., Berna, F., & Berntsen, D. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memory and future thought predicting hallucination proneness. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(6), 891–898.CrossRef
go back to reference Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., Goldstein, S., & Fortgang, M. (1970). Mind-wandering and cognitive structure. Transactions of the New York Academy of SCIence (Series 11), 32, 242–252.CrossRef Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., Goldstein, S., & Fortgang, M. (1970). Mind-wandering and cognitive structure. Transactions of the New York Academy of SCIence (Series 11), 32, 242–252.CrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D. (1996). Involuntary autobiographical memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(5), 435–454.CrossRef Berntsen, D. (1996). Involuntary autobiographical memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(5), 435–454.CrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D. (2009). Involuntary autobiographical memories: An introduction to the unbidden past. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Berntsen, D. (2009). Involuntary autobiographical memories: An introduction to the unbidden past. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D., & Bohn, A. (2010). Remembering and forecasting: The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Memory and Cognition, 38(3), 265–278.PubMedCrossRef Berntsen, D., & Bohn, A. (2010). Remembering and forecasting: The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Memory and Cognition, 38(3), 265–278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 17(4), 1093–1104.CrossRef Berntsen, D., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 17(4), 1093–1104.CrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 36, 352–372.CrossRef Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 36, 352–372.CrossRef
go back to reference Buhrmester, M. D., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2018). An evaluation of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 149–154.PubMedCrossRef Buhrmester, M. D., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2018). An evaluation of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 149–154.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. NeurosCIence, Nature Reviews 17, 718–731.PubMedCrossRef Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. NeurosCIence, Nature Reviews 17, 718–731.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: SCIence and practice. New York: Guilford Press. Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: SCIence and practice. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Cole, S. N., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Do future thoughts reflect personal goals? Current concerns and mental time travel into the past and future. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 273–284.PubMedCrossRef Cole, S. N., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Do future thoughts reflect personal goals? Current concerns and mental time travel into the past and future. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 273–284.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 44(3), 376–389.PubMedCrossRef Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 44(3), 376–389.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12(11), 671–684.CrossRef Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12(11), 671–684.CrossRef
go back to reference Cummings, G. (2009). Inference by eye: Reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Statistics in MediCIne, 28, 205–220.CrossRef Cummings, G. (2009). Inference by eye: Reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Statistics in MediCIne, 28, 205–220.CrossRef
go back to reference D’Argembeau, A. (2012). Autobiographical memory and future thinking. In D. Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.). Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches (pp. 311–330). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. D’Argembeau, A. (2012). Autobiographical memory and future thinking. In D. Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.). Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches (pp. 311–330). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Lindeen, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 15, 342–350.PubMedCrossRef D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Lindeen, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 15, 342–350.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018a). Emotional responses to everyday mental time travel in social anxiety: A naturalistic study. (Manuscript in preparation). del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018a). Emotional responses to everyday mental time travel in social anxiety: A naturalistic study. (Manuscript in preparation).
go back to reference del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018b). Emotion regulation of events central to identity and their relationship with concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 49, 604–616.PubMedCrossRef del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018b). Emotion regulation of events central to identity and their relationship with concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 49, 604–616.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Berntsen, D., & Watson, L. A. (2017). Emotional intensity and emotion regulation in response to autobiographical memories during dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(4), 530–542.CrossRef del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Berntsen, D., & Watson, L. A. (2017). Emotional intensity and emotion regulation in response to autobiographical memories during dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(4), 530–542.CrossRef
go back to reference Erskine, J. A. K., Kvavilashvili, L., & Kornbrot, D. E. (2007). The predictors of thought suppression in young and old adults: Effects of rumination, anxiety, and other variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1047–1057.CrossRef Erskine, J. A. K., Kvavilashvili, L., & Kornbrot, D. E. (2007). The predictors of thought suppression in young and old adults: Effects of rumination, anxiety, and other variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1047–1057.CrossRef
go back to reference Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2011). Involuntary and voluntary mental time travel in high and low worriers. Memory, 19(6), 625–640.PubMedCrossRef Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2011). Involuntary and voluntary mental time travel in high and low worriers. Memory, 19(6), 625–640.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2013). Involuntary future projections are as frequent as involuntary memories, but more positive. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 22(1), 272–280.CrossRef Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2013). Involuntary future projections are as frequent as involuntary memories, but more positive. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 22(1), 272–280.CrossRef
go back to reference Giambra, L. M. (1993). The influence of aging on spontaneous shifts of attention from external stimuli to the contents of consciousness. Experimental Gerontology, 28, 485–492.PubMedCrossRef Giambra, L. M. (1993). The influence of aging on spontaneous shifts of attention from external stimuli to the contents of consciousness. Experimental Gerontology, 28, 485–492.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.PubMedCrossRef Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grysman, A. (2015). Collecting narrative data on amazon’s mechanical turk. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(4), 573–583.CrossRef Grysman, A. (2015). Collecting narrative data on amazon’s mechanical turk. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(4), 573–583.CrossRef
go back to reference Hintzman, D. L. (2011). Research strategy in the study of memory: Fads, fallacies, and the search for the “coordinates of truth”. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 6(3), 253–271.PubMedCrossRef Hintzman, D. L. (2011). Research strategy in the study of memory: Fads, fallacies, and the search for the “coordinates of truth”. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 6(3), 253–271.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huba, G. J., Segal, B., & Singer, J. L. (1977). Consistency of daydreaming styles across samples of college male and female drug and alcohol users. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(1), 99–102.PubMedCrossRef Huba, G. J., Segal, B., & Singer, J. L. (1977). Consistency of daydreaming styles across samples of college male and female drug and alcohol users. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(1), 99–102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huba, G. J., Singer, J. L., Aneshenel, C. S., & Antrobus, J. S. (1982). Short Imaginal Process Inventory. In Short Imaginal Porcesses Inventory. Manual. London: Research Psychologists Press. Huba, G. J., Singer, J. L., Aneshenel, C. S., & Antrobus, J. S. (1982). Short Imaginal Process Inventory. In Short Imaginal Porcesses Inventory. Manual. London: Research Psychologists Press.
go back to reference Hyman, I. E. Jr., Cutshaw, K. I., Hall, C. M., Snyders, M. E., Masters, S. A., Au, V. S. K., & Graham, J. M. (2015). Involuntary to intrusive: Using involuntary musical imagery to explore individual differences and the nature of intrusive thoughts. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 14–27.CrossRef Hyman, I. E. Jr., Cutshaw, K. I., Hall, C. M., Snyders, M. E., Masters, S. A., Au, V. S. K., & Graham, J. M. (2015). Involuntary to intrusive: Using involuntary musical imagery to explore individual differences and the nature of intrusive thoughts. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 14–27.CrossRef
go back to reference Jones, S. R., & Fernyhough, C. (2009). Rumination, reflection, intrusive thoughts, and hallucination-proneness: Towards a new model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(1), 54–59.PubMedCrossRef Jones, S. R., & Fernyhough, C. (2009). Rumination, reflection, intrusive thoughts, and hallucination-proneness: Towards a new model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(1), 54–59.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kamiya, S. (2014). Relationship between frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and cognitive failure. Memory, 22(7), 839–851.PubMedCrossRef Kamiya, S. (2014). Relationship between frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and cognitive failure. Memory, 22(7), 839–851.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koster, E. H. W., Soetens, B., Braet, C., & De Raedt, R. (2008). How to control a white bear? Individual differences involved in self-perceived and actual thought-suppression ability. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1068–1080.CrossRef Koster, E. H. W., Soetens, B., Braet, C., & De Raedt, R. (2008). How to control a white bear? Individual differences involved in self-perceived and actual thought-suppression ability. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1068–1080.CrossRef
go back to reference Krans, J., de Bree, J., & Moulds, M. L. (2015). Involuntary cognitions in everyday life: Exploration of type, quality, content, and function. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 11.CrossRef Krans, J., de Bree, J., & Moulds, M. L. (2015). Involuntary cognitions in everyday life: Exploration of type, quality, content, and function. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 11.CrossRef
go back to reference Lavy, E. H., & Van, dH. (1990). Thought suppression induces intrusions. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 18(4), 251–258.CrossRef Lavy, E. H., & Van, dH. (1990). Thought suppression induces intrusions. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 18(4), 251–258.CrossRef
go back to reference Mace, J. H. (2007). Involuntary memory: Concept and theory. In J. H. Mace (Ed.), Involuntary memory; involuntary memory (pp. 1–19). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRef Mace, J. H. (2007). Involuntary memory: Concept and theory. In J. H. Mace (Ed.), Involuntary memory; involuntary memory (pp. 1–19). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRef
go back to reference Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., Klinger, E., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: The dark side of the wandering mind. Clinical Psychological SCIence, 4(5), 835–857.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., Klinger, E., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: The dark side of the wandering mind. Clinical Psychological SCIence, 4(5), 835–857.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference McMillan, R. L., Kaufman, S. B., & Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–9. McMillan, R. L., Kaufman, S. B., & Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–9.
go back to reference Meyer, T., Otgaar, H., & Smeets, T. (2015). Flashbacks, intrusions, mind-wandering - instances of an involuntary memory spectrum: A commentary on Takarangi, Strange, and Lindsay (2014). ConsCIousness and Cognition, 33, 24–29.PubMedCrossRef Meyer, T., Otgaar, H., & Smeets, T. (2015). Flashbacks, intrusions, mind-wandering - instances of an involuntary memory spectrum: A commentary on Takarangi, Strange, and Lindsay (2014). ConsCIousness and Cognition, 33, 24–29.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the mind-wandering questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 7.CrossRef Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the mind-wandering questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 7.CrossRef
go back to reference Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Horselenberg, R. (1996). Individual differences in thought suppression. the white bear suppression inventory: Factor structure, reliability, validity and correlates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(5–6), 501.PubMedCrossRef Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Horselenberg, R. (1996). Individual differences in thought suppression. the white bear suppression inventory: Factor structure, reliability, validity and correlates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(5–6), 501.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 77(5), 1061–1072.PubMedCrossRef Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 77(5), 1061–1072.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Clark, D. A., & O’Sullivan, L.,F. (2017). Cognitive processing in the aftermath of relationship dissolution: Associations with concurrent and prospective distress and posttraumatic growth. Stress and Health, 33(5), 540–548.PubMedCrossRef Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Clark, D. A., & O’Sullivan, L.,F. (2017). Cognitive processing in the aftermath of relationship dissolution: Associations with concurrent and prospective distress and posttraumatic growth. Stress and Health, 33(5), 540–548.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Plimpton, B., Patel, P., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2015). Role of triggers and dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A laboratory study. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 33, 261–276.CrossRef Plimpton, B., Patel, P., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2015). Role of triggers and dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A laboratory study. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 33, 261–276.CrossRef
go back to reference Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 20(4), 1842–1846.CrossRef Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 20(4), 1842–1846.CrossRef
go back to reference Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2013). The reality of the past versus the ideality of the future: Emotional valence and functional differences between past and future mental time travel. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 187–200.CrossRef Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2013). The reality of the past versus the ideality of the future: Emotional valence and functional differences between past and future mental time travel. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 187–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Rasmussen, A. S., Ramsgaard, S. B., & Berntsen, D. (2015). Frequency and functions of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories across the day. Psychology of ConsCIousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2(2), 185–205. Rasmussen, A. S., Ramsgaard, S. B., & Berntsen, D. (2015). Frequency and functions of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories across the day. Psychology of ConsCIousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2(2), 185–205.
go back to reference Rubin, D. C., Dennis, M. F., & Beckham, J. C. (2011). Autobiographical memory for stressful events: The role of autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 20(3), 840–856.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rubin, D. C., Dennis, M. F., & Beckham, J. C. (2011). Autobiographical memory for stressful events: The role of autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 20(3), 840–856.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Salkovskis, P. M., & Campbell, P. (1994). Thought suppression induces intrusion in naturally occurring negative intrusive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(1), 1.PubMedCrossRef Salkovskis, P. M., & Campbell, P. (1994). Thought suppression induces intrusion in naturally occurring negative intrusive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(1), 1.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Salkovskis, P. M., & Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 193.PubMedCrossRef Salkovskis, P. M., & Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 193.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews NeurosCIence, 8(9), 657–661.PubMedCrossRef Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews NeurosCIence, 8(9), 657–661.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. New York: Cambridge University Press. Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive SCIences, 20(8), 605–617.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive SCIences, 20(8), 605–617.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Singer, J. L. (1974). Daydreaming and the stream of thought. American SCIentist, 62(4), 417–425.PubMed Singer, J. L. (1974). Daydreaming and the stream of thought. American SCIentist, 62(4), 417–425.PubMed
go back to reference Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in daydreaming and related inner experience. American Psychologist, 30(7), 727–738.CrossRef Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in daydreaming and related inner experience. American Psychologist, 30(7), 727–738.CrossRef
go back to reference Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 6.CrossRef Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 6.CrossRef
go back to reference Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958.PubMedCrossRef Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487–518.CrossRefPubMed Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487–518.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smets, J., Wessel, I., Schreurs, E., & Raes, F. (2012). The interplay between rumination and intrusions in the prediction of concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms in two nonclinical samples. The Psychological Record, 62(4), 777–788.CrossRef Smets, J., Wessel, I., Schreurs, E., & Raes, F. (2012). The interplay between rumination and intrusions in the prediction of concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms in two nonclinical samples. The Psychological Record, 62(4), 777–788.CrossRef
go back to reference Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought: An emerging concept. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 5(2), 142–162.PubMedCrossRef Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought: An emerging concept. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 5(2), 142–162.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.CrossRef Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.CrossRef
go back to reference Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.CrossRef Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Turk, I. R. Rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence. 13, 149–154. Turk, I. R. Rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence. 13, 149–154.
go back to reference Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., I.,II, & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 53(1), 5.PubMedCrossRef Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., I.,II, & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 53(1), 5.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62(4), 615.CrossRef Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62(4), 615.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: an exploratory study
Auteurs
Adriana del Palacio-Gonzalez
Dorthe Berntsen
Publicatiedatum
19-12-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1132-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2019

Psychological Research 4/2019 Naar de uitgave