Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 12/2011

Open Access 01-12-2011 | Original Paper

The Swedish Version of the Ritvo Autism and Asperger Diagnostic Scale: Revised (RAADS-R). A Validation Study of a Rating Scale for Adults

Auteurs: Lisa M. J. Andersen, Katharina Näswall, Irina Manouilenko, Lena Nylander, Johan Edgar, Riva Ariella Ritvo, Edward Ritvo, Susanne Bejerot

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 12/2011

Abstract

There is a paucity of diagnostic instruments for adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This study evaluates the psychometric properties of the Swedish version of the Ritvo Autism and Asperger Diagnostic Scale-Revised (RAADS-R), an 80-item self-rating scale designed to assist clinicians diagnosing ASD in adults. It was administered to 75 adults with ASD and 197 comparison cases. Also, a subset completed the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Three out of four subscales had high internal consistency. Sensitivity was 91% and specificity was 93%. The ASD subjects had significantly higher mean scores on all subscales. ASD females had higher scores than ASD males on the sensory motor subscale, a dimension not included in the AQ. RAADS-R showed promising test re-test reliability.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by impairments in social relatedness, communication and restricted patterns of behavior and interests (APA 2000). The variability of symptom expression in ASD is striking, a fact which has contributed to the continuous revisions and broadening of definitions and diagnostic criteria since Kanner’s (1943) original descriptions. Symptoms vary depending on level of cognitive functioning, verbal ability and age, amongst other things (Volkmar et al. 1997). The current consensus is that these differences are due to variations in severity rather than distinct subtypes (Gilchrist et al. 2001; Howlin 2003). This is reflected in the proposed revisions for DSM-V, where the diagnosis Asperger’s disorder is subsumed under the diagnosis Autism Spectrum Disorder.
In Britain, the prevalence of undiagnosed autistic adults is found to be 1% (Brugha et al. 2009). This indicates undiagnosed ASD is a major public health issue in England, and most likely in other countries as well. Since most cases of ASD are diagnosed in childhood much effort has been put into developing strategies for early detection. Thus, many ASD individuals with milder impairments go through childhood and adolescence without receiving a diagnosis. In a large British study, Barnard et al. (2001) found that 29% of individuals with high functioning autism and 46% of those with Asperger’s disorder had not received this diagnosis until late adolescence or adulthood, which is in line with experiences in Sweden (Rydén and Bejerot 2008). One reason for this is probably the relatively recent inclusion of Asperger’s disorder in the diagnostic manuals DSM-IV and ICD-10. Other reasons may be that high intelligence and verbal ability can compensate for, and camouflage, other impairments, or that existing difficulties are mistaken for expressions of other psychiatric or psychosocial problems (Gillberg 2002). Also, other psychiatric disorders and symptoms often coexist with ASD (Bejerot and Wetterberg 2008). Impairments may become more pronounced in the transition to adulthood, when demands on self reliance and ability to structure one’s life increase, while social skill becomes even more important for academic as well as occupational achievements (Hendricks and Wehman 2009; Tantam 1991). Thus it is important to be able to make a diagnosis of ASD in adults.
The symptoms and impairments of ASD manifest themselves differently with increasing age. Several cross sectional and longitudinal studies suggest a trend towards general symptom abatement in adolescence and adulthood (Billstedt et al. 2007; Esbensen et al. 2009; Fecteau et al. 2003; Seltzer et al. 2003; Seltzer et al. 2004; Shattuck et al. 2007). There is some evidence that there is a risk of excluding older, high functioning individuals when standard diagnostic instruments and algorithms are utilized (Boelte and Puostka 2000; Fecteau et al. 2003; Lord et al. 1994).
There are several specific difficulties in diagnosing autism spectrum disorders in adults. Significant others who can provide information about childhood symptoms may be absent, which is necessary both for ADI-R (Lord et al. 1994) and DISCO (Leekam et al. 2002). Also, many subjects may not want to involve their parents in the diagnostic procedure. Presently there are only two validated, self-administered scales that purport to measure autistic symptoms in adults. One is the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ, Baron-Cohen et al. 2001) a research and screening instrument described in detail below. The other is the Ritvo Autism and Asperger Diagnostic Scale (RAADS, Ritvo et al. 2008).
The RAADS was developed (Ritvo et al. 2008) to accommodate the need for diagnostic tools specifically tailored for adults with ASD. The objective of the present study is to evaluate the Swedish version of the RAADS-R (a modified version of the RAADS) with respect to internal consistency, test re-test reliability, diagnostic accuracy and concurrent validity.

Methods

Participants

The total sample comprised 272 adult subjects ages 19–75. Two groups of participants were recruited: 75 with ASD (the ASD group) and 197 without ASD (comparison cases). See Table 1 for their sex ratio and age distribution. Subjects with ASD were recruited among patients diagnosed at the Neuropsychiatric unit, Northern Stockholm psychiatry (n = 17) or at a specialized unit in Lund (n = 6). In addition, 52 subjects with ASD who were participants in various research projects at Northern Stockholm Psychiatry were included. All subjects with ASD were examined by an experienced clinician and their diagnosis was confirmed by either the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G, Lord et al. 2000) (in Stockholm) or the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO) (in Lund). Administration of these instruments requires extensive training. Seventy-three subjects were diagnosed with Asperger’s disorder, and 2 with PPD-NOS (atypical autism). The standardized assessment of ASD in the Neuropsychiatric units in Stockholm and Lund include intelligence testing with WAIS. All included subjects had an IQ above 70, in other words no one fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for intellectual disability. All subjects had received their initial ASD diagnosis in adolescence or adulthood.
Table 1
Sex ratio and age by group
Subject group
N
Male: Female
Mean age (SD), min–max
ASD
75
36:35
31 (9), 26–62
Comparison cases
197
80:116
34 (13), 19–75
Total sample
272
120:152
33 (12), 19–75
Information on sex was missing for 1 subject in the comparison cases and 4 subjects in the ASD group
The comparison cases consisted of 61 doctors and medical students, 69 university students from three campuses in Sweden, and 60 subjects who comprised comparison cases in the research studies mentioned above. In addition, 7 psychiatric patients who were assessed for ASD but did not meet criteria were included. Out of these subjects, 6 met criteria for other psychiatric disorders (schizotypal personality disorder, ADHD, social anxiety disorder, depression, bipolar disorder, and delusional disorder). In the comparison cases another 6 subjects reported that they had a psychiatric diagnosis (depression, social anxiety disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, personality disorder NOS, and obsessive compulsive disorder). The study was approved by the Regional Ethic committee in Stockholm, and informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

Two measures were used in the study: RAADS-R (Ritvo et al. 2010) and the AQ (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). The RAADS-R is a revised version of the Ritvo Autism and Asperger Diagnostic Scale (RAADS), a self rating scale developed by Ritvo et al. (2008) to serve as an aid in the diagnosis of ASD in adults of normal intelligence. Items were formulated from DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria for autism, which were operationalized to match the symptom expression in adults based on the authors’ clinical experience. The original RAADS encompassed 78 items which were divided into three subscales to assess functioning in the domains of social interaction, language/communication and sensory motor/stereotypies (Ritvo et al. 2008). Following the 2008 pilot study, some alterations were made to the scale. Revisions included elimination of three items to improve internal consistency, adding more items on circumscribed interests, and some modifications to the subscales, like splitting up the sensory motor/stereotypies subscale into two separate scales.
Presently, the RAADS-R encompasses 80 items which are divided into four domains to assess functioning in: (a) social interaction, (b) language, (c) circumscribed interests and (d) sensory motor symptoms. Each item is formulated as a statement from the patient’s point of view (e.g. “I often don’t know how to act in social situations”). 17 items are reversed in order to avoid response bias and to elicit information about skills or preferences acquired throughout the life span (e.g. “I like to have close friends”) (Ritvo et al. 2008). See “Appendix” for the full content of the various subscales. The statements are answered on a four point Likert scale with the qualitative alternatives “never true”, “true only when I was young (before the age of 16)”, “true only now” and “true now and when I was young”. The 63 “positively worded” statements are scored from 0 to 3, so that the longer a symptom has been present the more points it yields, and the 17 reversed statements are scored in the reverse order (marked with an * in the “Appendix”). Higher scores are indicative of ASD in all subscales. The original RAADS pilot study (Ritvo et al. 2008) yielded promising results. In a sample comprising 37 subjects with autistic disorder or Asperger’s disorder, 41 subjects with no psychiatric condition and 16 subjects with various psychiatric disorders outside the autism spectrum, RAADS demonstrated perfect sensitivity and specificity, as all subjects with an ASD obtained scores of 77 or higher whereas all subjects without an ASD scored at or below 64. Internal consistencies for the three subscales ranged from poor (α = 0.60) to good (α = 0.84). In a recent multi-center study the RAADS-R also demonstrated excellent diagnostic accuracy as well as improved internal consistency (Ritvo et al. 2010).
The RAADS-R was translated into Swedish by Susanne Bejerot, M.D., with assistance of Dr Lena Nylander. It was back translated by a bilingual translator, after which it was compared to the original and no modifications were deemed necessary.
The AQ (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001) was designed as a brief, self-administered questionnaire purporting to measure the degree to which any adult with normal intelligence has “autistic traits”. The rationale underlying the scale is the assumption that autism lies at the upper end of a spectrum of traits which are normally distributed in the population (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). The AQ comprises 50 items, divided into five domains: (a) social skill, (b) communication, (c) attention switching, (d) attention to detail, and (e) imagination. The questions are answered on a 4-point Likert scale, where “definitely disagree” and “slightly disagree” are scored as 0, and “slightly agree” and “definitely agree” is scored as 1 for half the questions, while the rest are reversely worded and scored.
The AQ has been evaluated both as a research instrument and as a screening instrument. In a pilot study, Baron-Cohen et al. (2001) found that AQ scores produced the hypothesized group differences between subjects with and without ASD, between students of science versus humanities, and between men and women in the general population. AQ has also been found to have screening properties (Hoekstra et al. 2008; Woodbury-Smith et al. 2005), however, in one study it did not differentiate between patients with mild ASD and patients with other psychiatric conditions (Ketelaars et al. 2007). The internal consistencies of the subscales have ranged from poor to fair in different studies (Austin 2005; Baron-Cohen et al. 2001; Hoekstra et al. 2008; Hurst et al. 2007). Several studies examining the factor structure of the AQ have found that a two- or three-factor solution fitted the data better compared to the five originally proposed domains (Austin 2005; Hoekstra et al. 2008; Hurst et al. 2007). Swedish participants were administered a translated version of the AQ which has not been validated.

Procedure

All participants completed RAADS-R. A subset of 39 ASD patients and 49 comparison cases completed AQ as well. If the subject did not understand a question an investigator was available to offer clarification. All personal data was coded and all data analyses were made in SPSS, version 17. The response rate was set at a minimum of 80% of the questions for inclusion in the study. This led to the exclusion of two subjects, both female ASD patients. For the remaining data, isolated missing scores were replaced with the individual mean.

Results

Internal Consistency and Test–Retest Reliability

The internal consistency was assessed separately in the ASD group and in the comparison cases. Cronbach’s coefficient alpha for the total scale was estimated at 0.92 in the ASD group and at 0.94 in the comparison cases. Internal consistencies for the four subscales were: social interaction α = 0.87/0.89 (ASD group/comparison cases), language, α = 0.58/0.22 (ASD group/comparison cases), circumscribed interests α = 0.73/0.73 (ASD group/comparison cases), and sensory motor α = 0.81/0.77(ASD group/comparison cases). Item 2 in the language subscale (I often use words and phrases from movies and television in conversations) had a negative corrected item-total correlation, and by removing it alpha for this subscale could be increased to 0.70/0.40 (ASD group/comparison cases).
Test -retest reliability was assessed in a subset of subjects comprising 12 with ASD who had completed RAADS-R on two separate occasions with 3–6 months interval. The total scores on the two occasions were strongly and positively correlated (r = 0.80, p = 0.002). Strong and significant correlations were also obtained for three of the subscale scores: social interaction (r = 0.76, p = 0.004), circumscribed interests (r = 0.73, p = 0.002) and sensory motor (r = 0.84, p = 0.001). For the language subscale the correlation was not statistically significant (r = 0.43, p = 0.161).

Correlation with the Autism-Spectrum Quotient

The degree of agreement between RAADS-R and AQ was assessed by comparing 35 subjects with ASD and 49 comparison cases. Correlation analyses between RAADS-R and AQ total and subscale scores were performed separately in the comparison cases and in the ASD subjects. In the ASD group there was a strong, positive correlation between RAADS-R and AQ (see Table 2).
Table 2
Correlations (Pearsons r) between RAADS-R and AQ total and domain scores in 35 subjects with ASD
AQ total and subscales
RAADS-R total and subscales
RAADS-R total score
Social interaction
Language
Circumscribed interests
Sensory motor
 AQ total score
0.84***
0.75***
0.63***
0.79***
0.60***
Social skill
0.79***
0.73***
0.65***
0.65***
0.56***
Communication
0.82***
0.79***
0.52***
0.72***
0.62***
Attention switching
0.68***
0.62***
0.53***
0.71***
0.44**
Attention to detail
0.30
0.12
0.22
0.40*
0.41*
Imagination
0.55***
0.54***
0.47**
0.52***
0.27
* Significant at the 0.05 level
** Significant at the 0.01 level
*** Significant at the 0.001 level
In the comparison cases, a Spearman’s rank order coefficient was computed as the variables were not normally distributed. The correlation between AQ and RAADS-R total scores was strong (ρ = 0.70, p < 0.0001). RAADS-R subscale; social interaction, circumscribed interests, and sensory motor all had moderate to strong correlations with AQ Total score (ρ = 0.51–0.72, all p < 0.0001). The language subscale however was not significantly correlated with AQ total or any of the subscale scores (ρ = 0.06–0.17, all p > 0.05).

Distribution of Scores

The distribution of scores is shown in Fig. 1a–e. As evident from the histograms, the scores of the comparison cases had strong positive skewness (2.17) and were markedly peaked (kurtosis = 6.78). The scores of the ASD group did not depart significantly from normality (skewness = 0.02, kurtosis = −0.33). The median, minimum, and maximum scores of the two groups are shown in Table 3.
Table 3
Median, minimum and maximum RAADS-R total and domain scores in the ASD group (N = 75) and the Comparison cases (N = 197)
Domain (no of items)
ASD
Comparison cases
Median
Min
Max
Median
Min
Max
Total RAADS-R (80)
114
34
198
28
0
175
Social interaction (39)
59
20
105
15
0
90
Language (7)
9
0
19
3
0
13
Circumscribed interests (14)
23
3
40
4.5
0
33
Sensory motor (20)
30
3
54
5.6
0
50

Group and Sex Differences

To explore group differences ANOVAs were performed comparing RAADS-R total and subscale scores by Diagnosis (ASD versus comparison cases) and Sex. Five subjects were excluded from the analysis due to missing information on sex. Mean total and subscale RAADS-R scores for the ASD and comparison cases are shown in Table 4, together with the results of the ANOVAs. As indicated, main effects of Diagnosis were found across all tests, the ASD subjects scoring higher than the comparison cases on the full scale as well as all four subscales. There was no main effect of Sex on the Total score, but there was a significant two-way interaction between Diagnosis and Sex. In the comparison cases the males obtained higher total scores than females, whereas in the ASD group females obtained higher total scores than males. T-tests revealed that these sex differences were not significant when assessed either in the comparison cases (t = 1.193, df = 137, p = 0.166) or in the ASD subjects (t = −1.847, df = 69, p = 0.069).
Table 4
Mean RAADS-R total and domain scores and standard deviations for males and females in the comparison cases versus ASD group
Domain
Comparison cases M (SD)
ASD M (SD)
Diagnosis
Sex
Group × Sex
Males
Females
Tot
Males
Females
Tot
F
p
η 2
F
p
η 2
F
p
η 2
Total score
37.2 (32.1)
31.4 (23.9)
33.8 (27.6)
110.5 (36.5)
127.2 (39.8)
118.7 (38.8)
391.61
0.000
0.598
1.61
0.206
0.006
7.00
0.009
0.026
Social interaction
20.1 (17.8)
15.5 (13.5)
17.4 (15.6)
56.7 (18.7)
58.2 (20.9)
57.4 (19.7)
291.11
0.000
0.525
0.43
0.515
0.002
1.73
0.189
0.007
Language
3.4 (2.9)
2.6 (2.3)
2.9 (2.6)
7.7 (4.2)
9.7 (5.1)
8.7 (4.8)
159.08
0.000
0.377
1.52
0.219
0.006
10.13
0.002
0.037
Circumscribed interests
6.5 (6.4)
5.9 (5.9)
6.1 (6.1)
22.5 (9.8)
24.9 (8.2)
23.7 (9.1)
320.75
0.000
0.549
0.84
0.360
0.003
2.29
0.132
0.009
Sensory motor
7.6 (9.3)
7.6 (7.2)
7.6 (8.1)
24.3 (11.5)
34.3 (10.1)
29.2 (12.3)
298.57
0.000
0.532
15.98
0.000
0.057
15.97
0.000
0.057
The table also shows the results of the ANOVAs: F, p-values and partial η 2 (N = 266*, df = 1)
* Information on sex was missing for 1 subject in the comparison cases and 4 subjects in the ASD group, which were excluded from the analysis
On the social interaction and circumscribed interests subscales there were no significant main effects of Sex, nor any Diagnosis × Sex interaction. On the language subscale there was no main effect of Sex, but a significant Diagnosis × Sex interaction, comparison case males scored higher than comparison case females (t = 2.370, df = 194, p = 0.019). Females in the ASD group scored somewhat higher than males, though this difference did not reach significance (t = −1.801, df = 69, p = 0.076). Lastly, on the sensory motor subscale women generally obtained higher scores than men. There was also a significant Diagnosis × Sex interaction. T tests showed that women in the ASD group, but not in the comparison cases, obtained significantly higher scores than males (t = −3,769, df = 69, p < 0.0001).
Eight comparison cases and 3 subjects in the ASD group had scores that deviated markedly from the mean (at least 2 standard deviations). If these outliers were excluded from the analysis, the patterns of which Diagnosis and Sex differences were significant did not change.

Ability to Differentiate Between ASD and Comparison Subjects

In order to further examine the ability of RAADS-R to distinguish between the two groups, a ROC-graph was generated. The area under the ROC curve (AUC) was estimated at 0.96 (Std. err. 0.012, 95% CI 0.94–0.98), indicating high overall accuracy. This means that the probability of a randomly selected subject with ASD scoring higher than a randomly selected subject with no ASD was approximately 96% in this sample. Table 5 shows the sensitivity and specificity of RAADS-R total score at various cut-offs between 50 and 100. If sensitivity and specificity are given equal priority, a cut-off of 72 achieved the best compromise, with sensitivity 0.907 and specificity 0.929.
Table 5
Sensitivity and specificity of RAADS-R at various cut-off scores (N = 272)
Cut-off
Sensitivity
Specificity
50
0.960
0.817
51
0.960
0.827
53
0.960
0.838
54
0.960
0.848
55
0.960
0.853
56
0.947
0.863
57
0.947
0.868
58
0.947
0.873
59
0.947
0.878
60
0.933
0.883
61
0.920
0.883
62
0.907
0.883
63
0.907
0.888
64
0.907
0.904
65
0.907
0.909
66
0.907
0.914
69
0.907
0.919
71
0.907
0.924
72
0.907
0.929
73
0.893
0.929
73
0.880
0.929
74
0.867
0.929
75
0.867
0.934
76
0.867
0.939
78
0.840
0.939
80
0.827
0.939
81
0.813
0.939
82
0.813
0.949
83
0.800
0.949
84
0.800
0.954
85
0.787
0.954
87
0.787
0.959
89
0.787
0.964
90
0.773
0.964
91
0.773
0.970
93
0.760
0.970
97
0.747
0.970
100
0.733
0.970

Discussion

The present study evaluates the psychometric properties of the Swedish version of the RAADS-R. The results indicate that RAADS-R is reliable, has good diagnostic validity and thus can be a useful aid in the diagnostic assessment of ASD in adults.

Internal Consistency and Test Re-test Reliability

The internal consistency was fair or good for three of the subscales: social interaction, circumscribed interests and sensory motor. The language subscale however demonstrated poor internal consistency as measured with Cronbach’s alpha. Part of the explanation for this is likely attributable to the fact that this subscale only consists of 7 items, as Cronbach’s alpha is a function of both intercorrelation among items and scale length (Nunnally 1978), but it could also be a result of cultural nuances between the English speaking world and Sweden. For example, one item (I often use words and phrases from movies and television in conversations) was reversely correlated with the scale, implying that this item does not work the way it was intended, and that modification or removal of this item should be considered in the Swedish version. Preliminary estimates of the test re-test reliability of the total score and three of the domain scores were very promising, again with exception of the language subscale. However, these results should be interpreted with caution as the sample size for the test–retest analysis was very small.

Diagnostic Validity

As expected, the ASD group obtained significantly higher scores than the comparison cases group on the total RAADS-R score as well as all four domain scores, indicating that the Swedish RAADS-R captures symptoms, characteristics and experiences that are relevant to the differentiation of patients with ASD from neurotypical subjects. Ritvo et al. (2010) suggests a cut-off of 66 for differentiating between patients with and without autism spectrum disorders in a study including nine-centers in four English-speaking countries. In the present study specificity could be somewhat increased, while maintaining the same level of sensitivity, if the cut-off was set somewhat higher, at 72. Although lower than in the Ritvo et al. (2008) study, the levels of sensitivity and specificity obtained in the present study must be considered good for a self rating instrument. Self rating thus seems to be a viable method of assessing impairments in adults of normal intelligence with ASD. This is supported by previous studies which have also found that these individuals generally have insight into, and are able to reliably report on, their own difficulties and way of functioning (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001; Cederlund 2007; Ritvo et al. 2008; Woodbury-Smith et al. 2005).
However, the overlapping by nine percent in each group serves as a reminder that self ratings are not exact or perfect. Moreover, some individuals with severe forms of ASD tend to lack sufficient insight, and for this reason give normal responses. This underscores the need for complementary basic instruments for systematic observations, such as the High functioning Autism Asperger Scale (HAGS) (Bejerot et al. 2001). It is also recommended that a clinician be present during the completion of RAADS-R in order to clarify any confusion and to assess the reliability of the patients’ responses (Ritvo et al. 2008).
As previously noted, 8 comparison cases (4 males and 4 females) obtained very high RAADS-R scores. 4 of these had undergone neuropsychiatric assessments and 3 were diagnosed with social anxiety disorder, ADHD, bipolar disorder, and schizotypal personality disorder. Another individual demonstrated many characteristics of ASD, and was assessed as having fulfilled diagnostic criteria in childhood; however presently he had no significant impairments. The fact that 3 out of the 12 subjects with a psychiatric disorder other than ASD obtained very high scores could be considered a problematic result. This underscores the importance of examining whether RAADS-R can differentiate between ASD and other psychiatric conditions, which may have overlapping symptoms. In addition, self-rating instruments alone are not sufficient in ambiguous cases; here clinical interviews are crucial to obtaining an accurate diagnosis.

Concurrent Validity

The overall strong and positive correlations between RAADS-R and AQ support the concurrent validity of the two instruments, although correlations provide only a rough estimate of the similarities and differences between them. The correlations between the domain scores are difficult to interpret as no factor analysis was performed on either instrument in the present study and previous factor analytic studies have suggested that the internal structure of the AQ does not fit the suggested domains (Austin 2005; Hoekstra et al. 2008; Hurst et al. 2007). The language and sensory motor domains within the RAADS-R had relatively modest correlations with the total AQ score compared to the social interaction and circumscribed interests domain scores. For the language subscale this might be due to poor internal consistency. The sensory motor subscale however showed good internal consistency and produced large group differences, implying that this subscale measures something unique and which is not included in the AQ conceptualization of autism. If one examines the content of the two scales this makes sense, as the AQ does not include questions on abnormal responses to sensory stimuli but stresses cognitive factors.

Sex Differences

A trend for females with ASD to score slightly higher than males with ASD is indicated, and this pattern was either the reverse or not replicated in the comparison cases. The higher scores in females with ASD could have several explanations: females may have greater insight into their symptoms than males; they may exaggerate their symptoms more; they may in fact have more symptoms than the males; female ASD might be more difficult to detect, thus the ones that are detected may have more extreme symptoms; or it could simply be a Type I Error. The sex difference in the comparison cases supports the male brain theory for autism, i.e. that males in general have more “autistic traits” than females in the normal population. Females with ASD scored higher than males on the sensory motor subscale. This may point towards a true sex difference in the symptomatology. Perhaps, in the future, these traits could serve as markers in genetic studies. In the comparison cases men scored slightly higher than women on the language subscale, but due to various problems with this subscale, one should be cautious with interpretations at this stage.

Limitations and Suggestions for Further Research

Some methodological limitations should be noted. First, the participants of the two groups compared in the study were not matched with respect to gender, age or intelligence. The age distributions of the two groups were roughly similar and all participants were in the range of intelligence above intellectual disability (i.e. IQ > 70). However, the ratio of females to males was proportionally greater in the comparison cases compared to the ASD group. The fact that women were in majority in the comparison cases might possibly have led to a slight overestimation of the specificity of the RAADS-R as comparison case females obtained slightly lower mean scores than comparison case males, although this difference was not statistically significant. Furthermore, not all of the subjects in the comparison case group were seen in person by the investigators or screened for psychiatric disorders. This is true for 29 of the students as well as for the 61 doctors and medical students. A few of these subjects did obtain remarkably high scores on the RAADS-R. As it was completed anonymously the investigators did not have the possibility to examine those who were high scorers.
A third limitation has to do with the fact that the scores in the comparison cases were essentially non-normally distributed (which is to be expected with this type of instrument), thus using parametric statistics would be somewhat dubious. ANOVAs have proven to be rather robust against a deviation from normality, as long as it is not caused by outliers (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007). In an attempt to compensate for this, the analyses were also performed with outliers removed, and results were identical with the exception of larger effect sizes overall. However it is possible that the non-normality of the distribution may have affected the results in some way; the sex effects are probably the most vulnerable as the differences in means between men and women are much smaller than the differences between the ASD group versus the comparison cases.
Finally, future studies are needed to assess the sensitivity and specificity of the RAADS-R for subjects with other specific DSM diagnoses such as OCD, Social Anxiety Disorder, severe personality disorder and schizophrenia. It should be noted that different cut-off limits may be optimal with other comparison populations.

Conclusion

The results of the present study indicate that the Swedish RAADS-R is a reliable and valid instrument that can be a useful tool for clinicians when diagnosing the possibility of ASD in adults. This self-administered rating-scale is easily administered and user-friendly, properties which both are valuable and cost-effective. Three of the subscales have adequate psychometric properties, with the language subscale being the weakest for reasons discussed.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Open AccessThis is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License (https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by-nc/​2.​0), which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Appendix

See Tables 6, 7, 8, 9.
Table 6
RAADS-R subscale—Social interaction items
Social interaction
1.* I am a sympathetic person
20. I like to copy the way certain people speak and act. It helps me appear more normal
6.* I can “put myself in other people’s shoes”
21. It can be very intimidating for me to talk to more than one person at the same time
8. I only like to talk to people who share my special interests
22. I have to “act normal” to please others and make them like me
11. I miss my friends or family when we are apart for a long time
23.* Meeting new people is usually easy for me
14. I’d rather go out to eat in a restaurant by myself than with someone I know
26.* I like having a conversation with several people, for instance around a dinner table, at school or at work
17. Others consider me odd or different
31. I have never wanted or needed to have what other people call an “intimate relationship”
18.* I understand when friends need to be comforted
43.* I like to talk things over with my friends
25. It is difficult for me to understand how other people are feeling when we are talking
47.* I feel very comfortable with dating or being in social situations with others
37.* I am an understanding type of person
48.* I try to be as helpful as I can when other people tell me their personal problems
38. I do not connect with characters in movies and cannot feel what they feel
53.* I am considered a compassionate type of person
3. I am often surprised when others tell me I have been rude
54. I get along with other people by following a set of specific rules that help me look normal
5. I often don’t know how to act in social situations
55. It is very difficult for me to work and function in groups
12. Sometimes I offend others by saying what I am thinking, even if I don’t mean to
60. When talking to someone, I have a hard time telling when it is my turn to talk or to listen
28. It is very difficult for me to understand when someone is embarrassed or jealous
61. I am considered a loner by those who know me best
39. I cannot tell when someone is flirting with me
64. How to make friends and socialize is a mystery to me
44. I cannot tell if someone is interested or bored with what I am saying
68.* I can tell when someone says one thing but means something else
45. It can be very hard to read someone’s face, hand and body movements when they are talking
69. I like to be by myself as much as I can
76. It is difficult to figure out what other people expect of me
72.* I enjoy spending time eating and talking with my family and friends
79. I am often told that I ask embarrassing questions
77.* I like to have close friends
80. I tend to point out other peoples mistakes
 
* = reversed item
Table 7
RAADS-R subscale—Language items
Language
2. I often use words and phrases from movies and television in conversations
35. The phrase “I’ve got you under my skin” makes me very uncomfortable
7. I have a hard time figuring out what some phrases mean, like “you are the apple of my eye”
58.* I can chat and make small talk with people
15. I cannot imagine what it would be like to be someone else
66. The phrase “he wears his heart on his sleeve” does not make sense to me
27. I take things too literally, so I often miss what people are trying to say
 
* = reversed item
Table 8
RAADS-R subscale—Circumscribed interests items
Circumscribed interests
9. I focus on details rather than the overall idea
50. Sometimes a thought or a subject gets stuck in my mind and I have to talk about it even if no one is interested
13. I only like to think and talk about a few things that interest me
52. I have never been interested in what most of the people I know consider interesting
24. I get highly confused when someone interrupts me when I am talking about something I am very interested in
56. When I am talking to someone, it is hard to change the subject. If the other person does so, I can get very upset and confused
30. I get extremely upset when the way I like to do things is suddenly changed
63. I like things to be exactly the same day after day and even small changes in my routines upset me
32. It is difficult for me to start and stop a conversation. I need to keep going until I am finished
70. I keep my thoughts stacked in my memory like they are on filing cards, and I pick up the ones I need by looking through the stack and finding the right one (or another unique way)
40. I can see in my mind in exact detail things that I am interested in
75. When I go somewhere, I have to follow a familiar route or I can get very confused and upset
41. I keep lists of things that interest me, even when they have no practical use (for example sports statistics, train schedules, calendar dates, historical facts and dates)
78. People tell me that I give too much detail
* = reversed item
Table 9
RAADS-R subscale—Sensory motor items
Sensory motor
4. Sometimes I talk too loudly or too softly, and I am not aware of it
49. I have been told that I have an unusual voice (for example flat, monotone, childish or high-pitched)
10. I always notice how food feels in my mouth. This is more important to me than how it tastes
51. I do certain things with my hands over and over again (like flapping, twirling sticks or strings, waving things by my eyes)
16. I have been told that I am clumsy or uncoordinated
57. Sometimes I have to cover my ears to block out painful noises (like vacuum cleaners or people talking too much or too loudly)
19. I am very sensitive to the way my clothes feel when I touch them. How they feel is more important to me than how they look
59. Sometimes things that should feel painful are not (for instance when I hurt myself or burn my hand on a stove)
29. Some ordinary textures that do not bother others feel very offensive when they touch my skin
62.* I usually speak in a normal tone
33*. I speak with a normal rhythm
65. It calms me to spin around or to rock in a chair when I am feeling stressed
34. The same sound, color or texture can suddenly change from very sensitive to very dull
67. If I am in a place where there are many smells, textures to feel, noises or bright lights, I feel anxious or frightened
36. Sometimes the sound of a word or a high-pitched noise can be painful to my ears
71. The same sound sometimes seems very loud or very soft, even though I know it has not changed
42. When I feel overwhelmed by my senses, I have to isolate myself to shut them down
73. I can’t tolerate things I dislike (like smells, textures, sounds or colors)
46. The same thing (like clothes or temperatures) can feel very different to me at different times
74. I don’t like to be hugged or held
* = reversed item
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th text revised edn.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th text revised edn.). Washington, DC: Author.
go back to reference Austin, E. J. (2005). Personality correlates of the Broader autism phenotype as assessed by the Autism spectrum quotient (AQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 451–460.CrossRef Austin, E. J. (2005). Personality correlates of the Broader autism phenotype as assessed by the Autism spectrum quotient (AQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 451–460.CrossRef
go back to reference Barnard, J., Harvey, D., Prior, V., & Potter, A. (2001). Ignored or ineligible? The reality for adults with autism spectrum disorders. London: National Autistic Society. Barnard, J., Harvey, D., Prior, V., & Potter, A. (2001). Ignored or ineligible? The reality for adults with autism spectrum disorders. London: National Autistic Society.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bejerot, S., Nylander, L., & Lindström, E. (2001). Autistic traits in obsessive compulsive disorder. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 169–176.PubMedCrossRef Bejerot, S., Nylander, L., & Lindström, E. (2001). Autistic traits in obsessive compulsive disorder. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 169–176.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bejerot, S., & Wetterberg, L. (2008). Autism spectrum disorders and psychiatric co-morbidity in adolescents and adults. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 5, 3–9. Bejerot, S., & Wetterberg, L. (2008). Autism spectrum disorders and psychiatric co-morbidity in adolescents and adults. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 5, 3–9.
go back to reference Billstedt, E., Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, C. (2007). Autism in adults: symptom patterns and early childhood predictors. Use of the DISCO in a community sample followed from childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1102–1110.PubMedCrossRef Billstedt, E., Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, C. (2007). Autism in adults: symptom patterns and early childhood predictors. Use of the DISCO in a community sample followed from childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1102–1110.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Boelte, S., & Puostka, F. (2000). Diagnosis of autism: The connection between current and historical information. Autism, 4, 382–390.CrossRef Boelte, S., & Puostka, F. (2000). Diagnosis of autism: The connection between current and historical information. Autism, 4, 382–390.CrossRef
go back to reference Cederlund, M. (2007). Boys with Asperger syndrome grown up: A longitudinal follow up study of 100 cases more than 5 years after original diagnosis. Gothenburg: Gothenburg University. Cederlund, M. (2007). Boys with Asperger syndrome grown up: A longitudinal follow up study of 100 cases more than 5 years after original diagnosis. Gothenburg: Gothenburg University.
go back to reference Esbensen, A. J., Seltzer, M., Lam, K. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Age-related differences in restricted repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 57–66.PubMedCrossRef Esbensen, A. J., Seltzer, M., Lam, K. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Age-related differences in restricted repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 57–66.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fecteau, S., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Burack, J. A. (2003). Developmental changes of autistic symptoms. Autism, 7(3), 255–268.PubMedCrossRef Fecteau, S., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Burack, J. A. (2003). Developmental changes of autistic symptoms. Autism, 7(3), 255–268.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (2001). Development and current functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: A comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 227–240.PubMedCrossRef Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (2001). Development and current functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: A comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 227–240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gillberg, C. (2002). A guide to Asperger syndrome. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Gillberg, C. (2002). A guide to Asperger syndrome. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Hendricks, D. R., & Wehman, P. (2009). Transition from school to adulthood for youth with autism spectrum disorders: Review and recommendations. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 24, 77–88.CrossRef Hendricks, D. R., & Wehman, P. (2009). Transition from school to adulthood for youth with autism spectrum disorders: Review and recommendations. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 24, 77–88.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.PubMedCrossRef Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: Implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 3–13.PubMedCrossRef Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: Implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 3–13.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hurst, R. M., Mitchell, J. T., Kimbrel, N. A., Kwapil, T. K., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (2007). Examination of the reliability and factor structure of the autism spectrum quotient (AS) in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1938–1949.CrossRef Hurst, R. M., Mitchell, J. T., Kimbrel, N. A., Kwapil, T. K., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (2007). Examination of the reliability and factor structure of the autism spectrum quotient (AS) in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1938–1949.CrossRef
go back to reference Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Reprinted in L. Kanner, Childhood psychosis: Initial studies and new insights (Washington, DC: V. H. Winston, pp. 1–43). Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Reprinted in L. Kanner, Childhood psychosis: Initial studies and new insights (Washington, DC: V. H. Winston, pp. 1–43).
go back to reference Ketelaars, C., Horwitz, E., Sytema, S., Bos, J., Wiersma, D., Minderaa, R., et al. (2007). Brief report: Adults with mild autism spectrum disorders (ASD): Scores on the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) and comorbid psychopathology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 176–180.PubMedCrossRef Ketelaars, C., Horwitz, E., Sytema, S., Bos, J., Wiersma, D., Minderaa, R., et al. (2007). Brief report: Adults with mild autism spectrum disorders (ASD): Scores on the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) and comorbid psychopathology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 176–180.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Leekam, S. R., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Taylor, C. (2002). The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: Algorithms for ICD-10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43, 327–342.CrossRef Leekam, S. R., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Taylor, C. (2002). The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: Algorithms for ICD-10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43, 327–342.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr, Leventhal, B. L., Di Lavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observations schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr, Leventhal, B. L., Di Lavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observations schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for Cargivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for Cargivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). McGraw-Hill: New York. Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). McGraw-Hill: New York.
go back to reference Ritvo, R. A., Ritvo E. R., Guthrie, D., Ritvo, M. J., Hufnagel D. H., McMahon W. et al. (2010). The Ritvo Autism Asperger diagnostic scale-revised (RAADS-R): A scale to assist the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults: An international validation study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Nov 18. [Epub ahead of print]. Ritvo, R. A., Ritvo E. R., Guthrie, D., Ritvo, M. J., Hufnagel D. H., McMahon W. et al. (2010). The Ritvo Autism Asperger diagnostic scale-revised (RAADS-R): A scale to assist the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults: An international validation study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Nov 18. [Epub ahead of print].
go back to reference Ritvo, R. A., Ritvo, E. R., Guthrie, D., Yuwiler, A., Ritvo, M. J., & Weisbender, L. (2008). A scale to assist the diagnosis of autism and Asperger’s disorder in adults (RAADS): A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 213–223.PubMedCrossRef Ritvo, R. A., Ritvo, E. R., Guthrie, D., Yuwiler, A., Ritvo, M. J., & Weisbender, L. (2008). A scale to assist the diagnosis of autism and Asperger’s disorder in adults (RAADS): A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 213–223.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Shattuck, P. T., Orsmond, G., Swe, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The symptoms of autism spectrum disorders in adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 565–581.PubMedCrossRef Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Shattuck, P. T., Orsmond, G., Swe, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The symptoms of autism spectrum disorders in adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 565–581.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Seltzer, M. M., Shattuck, P., Abbeduto, L., & Greenberg, J. S. (2004). Trajectory of development in adolescents and adults with autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 234–247.PubMedCrossRef Seltzer, M. M., Shattuck, P., Abbeduto, L., & Greenberg, J. S. (2004). Trajectory of development in adolescents and adults with autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 234–247.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shattuck, P. T., Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Orsmond, G. I., Bolt, D., Kring, S., et al. (2007). Change in autism symptoms and maladaptive behaviours in adolescents and adults with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1735–1747.PubMedCrossRef Shattuck, P. T., Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Orsmond, G. I., Bolt, D., Kring, S., et al. (2007). Change in autism symptoms and maladaptive behaviours in adolescents and adults with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1735–1747.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc.
go back to reference Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger syndrom in adulthood. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 147–183). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University press.CrossRef Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger syndrom in adulthood. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 147–183). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University press.CrossRef
go back to reference Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., & Cohen, D. J. (1997). Diagnosis and classification of autism and related conditions: Consensus and issues. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 5–40). New York, NY: Wiley. Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., & Cohen, D. J. (1997). Diagnosis and classification of autism and related conditions: Consensus and issues. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 5–40). New York, NY: Wiley.
go back to reference Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Screening adults for Asperger syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 331–335.PubMedCrossRef Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Screening adults for Asperger syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 331–335.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The Swedish Version of the Ritvo Autism and Asperger Diagnostic Scale: Revised (RAADS-R). A Validation Study of a Rating Scale for Adults
Auteurs
Lisa M. J. Andersen
Katharina Näswall
Irina Manouilenko
Lena Nylander
Johan Edgar
Riva Ariella Ritvo
Edward Ritvo
Susanne Bejerot
Publicatiedatum
01-12-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 12/2011
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1191-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 12/2011

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 12/2011 Naar de uitgave