Introduction
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD), which include autistic disorder, Asperger’s disorder, and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) are characterized by variations in three symptom domains, namely, deficits in social interaction, deficits in verbal and non-verbal communication, and stereotypies and rigid patterns of behavior (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association
1994). It was recently reported that 9.0 per 1,000 8-year old children (95 % CI = 8.6–9.3) in the population fulfill DSM-IV criteria for ASD with a wide range in traits and severity (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
2009). Whereas a diagnosis of autistic disorder requires the presence of symptoms in all three domains, a diagnosis of Asperger’s disorder requires deficits in social interaction and stereotypies and rigid patterns of behavior, but no clinically significant problems in early language and communication. The diagnosis of PDD-NOS, a sub-threshold or atypical manifestation of autism, is typically made on the basis of deficits in social interaction, verbal and nonverbal communication or rigid and stereotyped behaviors that do not satisfy the full set of diagnostic criteria for autism (Buitelaar and van der Gaag
1998; Buitelaar et al.
1999; American Psychiatric Association
1994). Since, there are also individuals without any ASD diagnosis, who may have specific language disorders without clinically relevant social interaction problems, and other individuals who may show obsessive–compulsive problem behavior without clinically relevant deficits in social interaction and communication, it can be questioned to what extent the three symptom domains of ASD are phenomenologically independent at different levels of symptom severity.
In a classic paper, Wing and Gould (
1979) took a first step to answering this question and investigated the presence of autistic symptoms in children younger than 15 years who were included in case registers in the Camberwell area of London because of mental retardation or significant developmental problems. They found that the severity of social impairment was closely associated with abnormalities affecting comprehension and use of all forms of communication, as well as with repetitive patterns of interest (Wing and Gould
1979). Later studies using factor analysis and latent class analysis of scores for the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), a quantitative measure of autistic traits, showed in general population samples of children aged 7–15 years that the variation in autistic traits could be explained best by a single continuously distributed underlying factor (Constantino et al.
2000,
2003), and that distinct categories of subjects could be identified only on the basis of symptom severity (Constantino et al.
2000). This was confirmed in a clinical sample of patients with ASD and other psychiatric conditions (Constantino et al.
2004) and in siblings of children with ASD (Constantino et al.
2006). Similarly, other studies of multiplex ASD families identified subgroups on the basis of the degree of impairment (mild, moderate, or severe) across all three symptom domains of ASD rather than on the basis of distinct item endorsement profiles (Spiker et al.
2002) or significant correlations between the three symptom domains of ASD (Sung et al.
2005).
However, other multiplex ASD family and clinical sample studies reported the three symptom domains to be relatively separate from each other and in particular found deficits in social interaction to be independent of repetitive and stereotyped behaviors (Kolevzon et al.
2004; Silverman et al.
2002). Several other clinical sample studies also proposed a two factor structure of social-communicative behavior on one hand and stereotyped rigid behaviors on the other hand (Frazier et al.
2008; Gotham et al.
2007,
2008; Snow et al.
2009). The finding that joint attention in young children with ASD was associated with later social and language symptoms, but not with repetitive and stereotyped symptoms, also suggested that stereotyped and repetitive behavior may be an independent domain of ASD and have a separate developmental trajectory (Charman
2003). Further, rigid repetitive behaviors as part of the revised ADOS algorithm, make an independent contribution to diagnostic stability (Lord et al.
2006). As with the ASD multiplex studies and in clinical samples, a population-based study of 7-year-old twins found weak correlations (
r = 0.15–0.29) between social and communicative symptoms on the one hand and non-social obsessive repetitive behaviors on the other (Ronald et al.
2005).
Studies in clinical ASD populations also suggested differences in developmental trajectory for rigid and repetitive behavior compared to other ASD symptoms. The development of social-communicative behaviors in children with ASD deviates from the typical chain of smiles and warm, joyful expressions already by 6 months, obvious interest in other people by 12 months, use of single words at 16 months, and two-word meaningful phrases by 24 months (Dietz
2007; Filipek et al.
1999). Symptoms of repetitive behaviors might be less likely to develop until the second or third year of life in children with ASD (Charman and Swettenham
2001), These symptoms are also observed with less consistency and showed more variability in young toddlers with ASD than items related to social or communication symptoms (Stone et al.
1999), and are poorly predicted from early measures of imitation or language (Charman et al.
2003; Lord and Pickles
1996). On the other hand, recent studies found signs of repetitive behaviors, as early as 12 months of age both in children with ASD and in children with typical development (Ozonoff et al.
2008; Richler et al.
2007; Thelen
1979; Watt et al.
2008), but in children with typical development these behaviors showed a general decrease after 12 months of age (Thelen
1979). A recent study by Richler (
2010) also showed that low-order behaviors (repetitive sensorimotor: RSM) and high-order stereotypies (insistence on sameness: IS) have different developmental trajectories in children with ASD; RSM scores remained relatively high over time, indicating consistent severity, whereas IS scores started low and increased over time, indicating worsening (Richler et al.
2010). In sum, several studies in total populations as well as in clinical samples indicate a two factor structure of social-communicative behavior on one hand and stereotyped rigid behaviors on the other hand, and report a different developmental trajectory of these two domains.
Other studies provide support for a three factor structure. A later study in the same twins as above, conducted 1 year later, and using different measures of autistic traits showed modest phenotypic relationships between the three domains and in particular low correlations between social deficits and repetitive behaviors (Ronald et al.
2006a,
b). The fractionation of the three DSM-IV ASD domains is also found in studies with clinical samples (Georgiades et al.
2007; Lecavalier et al.
2006,
2009). The discrepant findings between the above-cited studies might be due to differences in the samples (clinical versus normal population), type of instruments used to measure the ASD phenotype and procedural variations in statistical analyses (Lecavalier et al.
2009).
To date, most studies of the structure of ASD symptoms have involved older children and adults, primarily from clinical samples or high-risk populations. It is important to examine the potential fractionation of ASD domains in the general population, in addition to diagnosed populations, for two reasons. First because the clinical diagnosis of autism itself requires impairments in each of the three key areas, and this would beg the question. Second because clinical samples particularly at young age may be biased by missing cases that have not been identified due to lack of clinical concerns. Studying the distribution of ASD symptoms in the general population is justified further because ASD symptoms have been shown being on a continuum from the normal population out to individuals on the autistic spectrum (Constantino et al.
2000,
2003). Furthermore, it is not known how the three symptom domains of ASD are interrelated and cluster together in very young children in the general population.
The present study was undertaken to examine (1) whether symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) in young children in the population fit the three-factor structure of ASD as described in DSM-IV, and (2) cluster together in individual subjects.
Discussion
In this large population based study (N = 11,332) among 18-month-old children the underlying structure of ASD symptoms was found generalizable to the general population at very young age. In the confirmatory factor analysis the three-factor model had a significantly better fit than the two- and one-factor model. Latent class analyses identified four classes based on the presence of different autistic symptoms, and with a distinction between the presence of social interaction and communication symptoms versus stereotypies and rigidity symptoms. These classes could further be differentiated by referral status, health problems and parental worries about the child at 18 months.
The continuous singular factor for ASD symptoms suggested by Constantino et al. (
2000,
2003,
2004) was not confirmed in the CFA by the current study. The one-factor model had a significantly poorer fit to the data than the two- and three-factor models, in which Social Interaction and Communication seemed to be closely associated, and in turn were only weekly associated with Stereotyped and Rigid Patterns of Behavior. Indeed, the analyses suggested that Stereotyped and Rigid Patterns of Behavior is a distinct domain. However, methodological differences may underlie the discrepancy between our findings and those of Constantino et al. For example, Constantino et al. looked at older children, used different items and performed bottom-up based exploratory factor analysis (EFA), whereas we used a top-down CFA. EFA of the current data or CFA of the SRS data of Constantino et al. (
2000,
2003,
2004) is needed to enable appropriate comparison of the findings of the two studies.
Based on fit indices, the two factor model of social/communication items and rigid repetitive behaviors was quite similar to the three-factor solution and better than the one-factor solution. The significant difference between the CFA fit indexes of the two and three factor model should be interpreted with some prudence due to the large sample size by which the significance levels were easily reached. Because the current data does not provide an unequivocal case for the two or three factor model of ASD symptoms and a two factor solution (with overlap between social and communication items) also has been reported in previous research, the similarity between the two factor model and the widely accepted three-factor DSM-IV model at least warrants discussion. The weak correlation between social (social interaction and communicative impairment) and non-social (rigid repetitive) behaviors in this study is consistent with the results of earlier population-based studies of twins at age 7 (Ronald et al.
2005) and age 8 (Ronald et al.
2006a,
b). However, our finding of a relatively strong association between Social Interaction and Communication symptoms was not reported in the twin study (Ronald et al.
2006a,
b), which showed modest phenotypic correlations between all three ASD symptom domains. This difference might be due to the relevance of the DSM-IV criteria to children of different ages (18 months in the present study versus 8 years in the twin study). According to the DSM-IV, gestures, non-verbal behavior and joint attention skills are part of the domain of Social Interaction; however, 18-month-old children have a very limited use of expressive language and non-verbal communication plays a greater role than it does in older children, where the distinction between Social Interaction and Communication may be more clear-cut.
Of the four classes identified by LCA on the basis of the autistic symptom profile, children classified in class 1 had high scores for all symptom domains, in particular on Social Interaction and Communication. Joint attention and language/communication items distinguished this class from the other classes. The high scores of the subjects of this class on social interaction and communication problems along with increased scores for repetitive behaviors suggest a similarity with high-risk or even clinical ASD children. This idea is supported by the finding that a high proportion of the children in this class were referred to educational services and child habilitation units. However, for now in the absence of final diagnoses, we could only confirm the high referral rate in this class, with children scoring high on autistic traits.
Class 2 (15.8 % of the sample) could represent a sub-clinical class with somewhat elevated scores on symptoms of Social Interaction and Communication and low scores on symptoms of Stereotyped and Rigid Patterns of Behavior. The proportion of children from class 2 who were referred to specialized services was only slightly higher than that of class 4, the reference group. One may hypothesize that some of these children from class 2 might later present with the broad ASD phenotype, or be diagnosed with milder forms of ASD, language disorders, or mental retardation.
Class 3 (10.7 % of the population) had a different profile of ASD symptoms by having high scores for symptoms of Stereotyped and Rigid Patterns of Behavior, but baseline scores for Social Interaction and Communication. The way Stereotyped and Rigid Patterns of Behavior seemed to be separated from the other two ASD domains was demonstrated by both the CFA on the level of items (factors) and the LCA in the clustering of individuals, and might be consistent with a study by Charman (
2003), who reported that the developmental trajectory for stereotypic behavior might be different from that for social and language deficits in ASD. However, although well-described in ASD (American Psychiatric Association
1994; Lewis and Bodfish
1998; Watt et al.
2008), repetitive and stereotypic behavior is also seen among individuals with mental retardation and other disorders (Bodfish et al.
1995,
2000; Lender et al.
1998) as well as in typically developing infants and children (Foster
1998; Leekam et al.
2007; MacDonald et al.
2007; Thelen
1979; Troster
1994). While stereotyped behavior in typically developing children becomes less varied and less frequent or remains stable with increasing age (Thelen
1979), it increases with age in children with ASD (MacDonald et al.
2007), particularly high-order stereotypies like rituals and insistence on sameness (Richler et al.
2010). The overall development and stability (increase, decrease, or remain the same) of these stereotypies will show whether the children of class 3 have a typical or aberrant development. For example, the hypothesis should be tested that these children might have an increased risk of developing an obsessive and rigid temperament, as described by Garland and Weiss (
1996). Separating low order behaviors from high order stereotypies could also add to the knowledge about developmental trajectories of stereotyped and rigid patterns of behavior and thereof of identifying an aberrant development. For the moment, the behavior of these children was apparently not perceived by the parents or kindergarten as warranting referral to specialized services.
There were more boys than girls in the two supposedly clinical classes (classes 1 and 2), but the male predominance was not as great as that found among ASD cases at a later age. For example, Baird et al. (
2006) found a male: female ratio of 3.3:1 at 9 and 10 years for all ASD with an overall prevalence of 1.2 %.
The results of this study should be interpreted in the context of its strengths and limitations. A strength of this study is the large and homogeneous sample of very young subjects. A possible limitation may be that the results of our multivariate analyses are dependent on and limited by the various items chosen to represent the three domains. The majority of the relevant ASD items were from the Social Interaction domain. Fewer items of the Communication and Stereotyped and Rigid Patterns of Behavior domains were included, which means that some ASD symptoms in these domains might not have been fully covered, which could have biased the outcome of the CFA. However, the accuracy of the factor analysis does not only depend on the number of items in a factor, but also for example on the relation between the items and on the extent the items reflect behaviors that are easily assessed by parents. Another possible limitation is that information about symptoms were based on parent reports only. The use of more objective measures, such as test results or more formal diagnostic observations and procedures, might have led to other results. However, parents are the main and in almost all cases sole informant about very young children’s behavior problems, and there is a body of evidence indicating the merit and validity of parent information. Previous studies by Glascoe (
1999,
2003) and Tervo (
2005) for example, found that parental concerns relate directly to their child’s wellbeing and development. Our prior work on population screening on autism spectrum disorders found that parental judgment about whether or not to comply with professional recommendations did reflect a rather accurate estimate of the severity of autistic symptoms of their child (Dietz et al.
2007).
This population-based study showed that ASD symptoms cluster together in the three domains as defined by the current classification system DSM-IV, and can be retrieved in our latent classes with subjects with a similar profile of ASD symptoms. At least one of these classes includes subjects with high scores on social interaction and communication problems and increased score of repetitive and stereotyped behaviors rather similar to high-risk or even clinical ASD cases. This information is relevant to improve current screening instruments and screening methods. It is for example of interest to know how many children show certain behavior in the general population to establish the point at which this behavior is considered abnormal. Further, since the validity of the distinction between ASD subtypes is unclear and the current diagnostic criteria in the DSM-IV are under discussion, these results can add to more knowledge on the development of the new DSM-V. Further follow-up of this cohort is required to examine how the classes develop with age and to characterize the children included in each class in terms of measures of external validity, such as cognitive and language skills, temperament, measure of neural structure and function, impairment of psychosocial functioning, and family loading for psychiatric disorders.