Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2015

01-09-2015 | Original Article

The reversal of perceptual and motor compatibility effects differs qualitatively between metacontrast and random-line masks

Auteurs: Anne Atas, Estibaliz San Anton, Axel Cleeremans

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In masked priming tasks, participants typically respond faster to compatible than to incompatible primes, an effect that has been dubbed as the positive compatibility effect (PCE). However, when the interval between the prime and the mask is relatively long, responses are faster to incompatible than to compatible primes. This inversion is called the negative compatibility effect (NCE). Two main origins of the NCE have been proposed. The object-updating theory holds that when the masks share stimulus features with the primes, both perceptual and motor processes generate an NCE. As an example, for masks composed of overlaid left and right prime arrows, the NCE is thought to be positive priming induced by the arrow of the mask pointing in the opposite direction of the prime. In contrast, the motor inhibition theories hold that the origin of the NCE is purely motor and can be demonstrated when masks do not share features with primes. To test both hypotheses, the present study aims at delineating the respective contributions of perceptual and motor components of the NCE in the context of different types of masks. Consistent with the object-updating hypothesis, we found both perceptual and motor NCEs at the long SOA with metacontrast masks (with internal contours corresponding to left and right overlaid arrows). Consistent with the motor inhibition hypothesis, we found motor NCE but no perceptual NCE at the long SOA with random-line masks (containing no prime features). The study thus suggests that the origin of the NCE depends on the type of mask.
Voetnoten
1
For arrow stimuli, the relevant perceptual information is the orientation of the arrowhead, not the orientation of the lines that composed the head. By saying even more “visually different”, we refer to the orientation of the arrowhead. As a matter of fact, in RespConflict 180° trials, prime and target are composed of lines with identical orientation (a line rotated by 180° has the same orientation as a non-rotated line), only the spatial arrangement of the lines relative to each other differs. In contrast, in RespConflict 90° trials, the lines making up the target are at a 90° angle to those making up the prime. However, this is not the relevant perceptual information in the task. Also, the prime and target were never presented in the same location so that the influence of low-level similarity of line orientation is reduced.
 
2
Because participants started each block by pressing the spacebar of the keyboard and then responded to target by choosing between the two extreme keys of the button box, the RT of the very first trial was affected by the changing of device. For this reason, it was systematically not analyzed.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Atas, A., Vermeiren, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2013). Repeating a strongly masked stimulus increases priming and awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1422–1430.CrossRefPubMed Atas, A., Vermeiren, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2013). Repeating a strongly masked stimulus increases priming and awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1422–1430.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bowman, H., Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2006). A neural network model of inhibitory processes in subliminal priming. Visual Cognition, 13(4), 401–480.CrossRef Bowman, H., Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2006). A neural network model of inhibitory processes in subliminal priming. Visual Cognition, 13(4), 401–480.CrossRef
go back to reference Boy, F., Evans, C. J., Edden, R. A., Singh, K. D., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2010a). Individual differences in subconscious motor control predicted by GABA concentration in SMA. Current Biology, 20(19), 1779–1785.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Boy, F., Evans, C. J., Edden, R. A., Singh, K. D., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2010a). Individual differences in subconscious motor control predicted by GABA concentration in SMA. Current Biology, 20(19), 1779–1785.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Boy, F., Husain, M., Singh, K. D., & Sumner, P. (2010b). Supplementary motor area activations in unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Experimental Brain Research, 206(4), 441–448.CrossRefPubMed Boy, F., Husain, M., Singh, K. D., & Sumner, P. (2010b). Supplementary motor area activations in unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Experimental Brain Research, 206(4), 441–448.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Boy, F., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2010c). Unconscious inhibition separates two forms of cognitive control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(24), 11134–11139.CrossRef Boy, F., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2010c). Unconscious inhibition separates two forms of cognitive control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(24), 11134–11139.CrossRef
go back to reference Boy, F., & Sumner, P. (2010). Tight coupling between positive and reversed priming in the masked prime paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 892–905.PubMedCentralPubMed Boy, F., & Sumner, P. (2010). Tight coupling between positive and reversed priming in the masked prime paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 892–905.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Boy, F., & Sumner, P. (2014). Visibility predicts priming within but not between people: a cautionary tale for studies of cognitive individual differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1011–1025.CrossRef Boy, F., & Sumner, P. (2014). Visibility predicts priming within but not between people: a cautionary tale for studies of cognitive individual differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1011–1025.CrossRef
go back to reference Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200–227.CrossRefPubMed Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200–227.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec, H. G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597–600.CrossRefPubMed Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec, H. G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597–600.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychology (Amsterdam), 101(2–3), 293–313.CrossRef Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychology (Amsterdam), 101(2–3), 293–313.CrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1737–1747.PubMed Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1737–1747.PubMed
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: evidence from masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 514–520.CrossRef Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: evidence from masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 514–520.CrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biological Psychology, 64(1–2), 7–26.CrossRefPubMed Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biological Psychology, 64(1–2), 7–26.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.CrossRef Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.CrossRef
go back to reference Ferrand, L. (1996). The masked repetition priming effect dissipates when increasing the inter-stimulus interval: evidence from word naming. Acta Psychologica, 91(1), 15–25.CrossRef Ferrand, L. (1996). The masked repetition priming effect dissipates when increasing the inter-stimulus interval: evidence from word naming. Acta Psychologica, 91(1), 15–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282), 1699–1702.CrossRefPubMed Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282), 1699–1702.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P. (2008). The negative compatibility effect with nonmasking flankers: a case for mask-triggered inhibition hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 765–777.CrossRefPubMed Jaśkowski, P. (2008). The negative compatibility effect with nonmasking flankers: a case for mask-triggered inhibition hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 765–777.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P., Bialunska, A., Tomanek, M., & Verleger, R. (2008). Mask- and distractor-triggered inhibitory processes in the priming of motor responses: an EEG study. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 70–85.PubMed Jaśkowski, P., Bialunska, A., Tomanek, M., & Verleger, R. (2008). Mask- and distractor-triggered inhibitory processes in the priming of motor responses: an EEG study. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 70–85.PubMed
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P., & Przekoracka-Krawczyk, A. (2005). On the role of mask structure in subliminal priming. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars), 65(4), 409–417. Jaśkowski, P., & Przekoracka-Krawczyk, A. (2005). On the role of mask structure in subliminal priming. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars), 65(4), 409–417.
go back to reference Jaśkowski, P., & Verleger, R. (2007). What determines the direction of subliminal priming. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1–2), 181–192.PubMedCentralCrossRef Jaśkowski, P., & Verleger, R. (2007). What determines the direction of subliminal priming. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1–2), 181–192.PubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kim, C. Y., & Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible ‘invisible’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(8), 381–388.CrossRefPubMed Kim, C. Y., & Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible ‘invisible’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(8), 381–388.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klapp, S. T. (2005). Two versions of the negative compatibility effect: comment on Lleras and Enns (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 431–435.CrossRef Klapp, S. T. (2005). Two versions of the negative compatibility effect: comment on Lleras and Enns (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 431–435.CrossRef
go back to reference Klapp, S. T., & Hinkley, L. B. (2002). The negative compatibility effect: unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 255–269.CrossRef Klapp, S. T., & Hinkley, L. B. (2002). The negative compatibility effect: unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 255–269.CrossRef
go back to reference Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 976–992. Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 976–992.
go back to reference Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 362(1481), 857–875.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 362(1481), 857–875.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Subliminal number priming within and across the visual and auditory modalities. Experimental Psychology, 56(6), 418–433.CrossRefPubMed Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Subliminal number priming within and across the visual and auditory modalities. Experimental Psychology, 56(6), 418–433.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krüger, D., Klapötke, S., & Mattler, U. (2011). PRP-paradigm provides evidence for a perceptual origin of the negative compatibility effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 866–881.CrossRefPubMed Krüger, D., Klapötke, S., & Mattler, U. (2011). PRP-paradigm provides evidence for a perceptual origin of the negative compatibility effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 866–881.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lingnau, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). The time course of response inhibition in masked priming. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(3), 545–557.CrossRefPubMed Lingnau, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). The time course of response inhibition in masked priming. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(3), 545–557.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(4), 475–493.CrossRef Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(4), 475–493.CrossRef
go back to reference Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2006). How much like a target can a mask be? Geometric, spatial, and temporal similarity in priming: a reply to Schlaghecken and Eimer (2006). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(3), 495–500.CrossRef Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2006). How much like a target can a mask be? Geometric, spatial, and temporal similarity in priming: a reply to Schlaghecken and Eimer (2006). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(3), 495–500.CrossRef
go back to reference Mattler, U. (2006). On the locus of priming and inverse priming effects. Perception and Psychophysics, 68(6), 975–991.CrossRefPubMed Mattler, U. (2006). On the locus of priming and inverse priming effects. Perception and Psychophysics, 68(6), 975–991.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McBride, J., Boy, F., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2012). Automatic motor activation in the executive control of action. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 82.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed McBride, J., Boy, F., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2012). Automatic motor activation in the executive control of action. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 82.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424.CrossRefPubMed Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 123–150). Cambridge: MIT Press. Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 123–150). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Nieuwenhuis, S., Forstmann, B. U., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: a problem of significance. Nature Neuroscience, 14(9), 1105–1107.CrossRefPubMed Nieuwenhuis, S., Forstmann, B. U., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: a problem of significance. Nature Neuroscience, 14(9), 1105–1107.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nigbur, R., Cohen, M. X., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Sturmer, B. (2012). Theta dynamics reveal domain-specific control over stimulus and response conflict. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(5), 1264–1274.CrossRefPubMed Nigbur, R., Cohen, M. X., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Sturmer, B. (2012). Theta dynamics reveal domain-specific control over stimulus and response conflict. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(5), 1264–1274.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Praamstra, P., & Seiss, E. (2005). The neurophysiology of response competition: motor cortex activation and inhibition following subliminal response priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 483–493.CrossRefPubMed Praamstra, P., & Seiss, E. (2005). The neurophysiology of response competition: motor cortex activation and inhibition following subliminal response priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 483–493.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Raghunathan, T. E., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1996). Comparing correlated but nonoverlapping correlations. Psychological Methods, 1, 178–183.CrossRef Raghunathan, T. E., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1996). Comparing correlated but nonoverlapping correlations. Psychological Methods, 1, 178–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2000). A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming. Perception and Psychophysics, 62(7), 1367–1382.CrossRefPubMed Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2000). A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming. Perception and Psychophysics, 62(7), 1367–1382.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2002). Motor activation with and without inhibition: evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control. Perception and Psychophysics, 64(1), 148–162.CrossRefPubMed Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2002). Motor activation with and without inhibition: evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control. Perception and Psychophysics, 64(1), 148–162.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2004). Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(3), 463–468.CrossRef Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2004). Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(3), 463–468.CrossRef
go back to reference Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2006). Active masks and active inhibition: a comment on Lleras and Enns (2004) and on Verleger, Jaśkowski, Aydemir, van der Lubbe, and Groen (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(3), 484–494.CrossRef Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2006). Active masks and active inhibition: a comment on Lleras and Enns (2004) and on Verleger, Jaśkowski, Aydemir, van der Lubbe, and Groen (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(3), 484–494.CrossRef
go back to reference Schlaghecken, F., Klapp, S. T., & Maylor, E. A. (2009). Either or neither, but not both: locating the effects of masked primes. Proceedings in Biological Sciences, 276(1656), 515–521.CrossRef Schlaghecken, F., Klapp, S. T., & Maylor, E. A. (2009). Either or neither, but not both: locating the effects of masked primes. Proceedings in Biological Sciences, 276(1656), 515–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Seiss, E., & Praamstra, P. (2004). The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 127(Pt 2), 330–339.CrossRefPubMed Seiss, E., & Praamstra, P. (2004). The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 127(Pt 2), 330–339.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sumner, P. (2007). Negative and positive masked-priming: implications for motor inhibition. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 317–326.PubMedCentralCrossRef Sumner, P. (2007). Negative and positive masked-priming: implications for motor inhibition. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 317–326.PubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Sumner, P. (2008). Mask-induced priming and the negative compatibility effect. Experimental Psychology (formerly “Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie”), 55(2), 133–141.CrossRef Sumner, P. (2008). Mask-induced priming and the negative compatibility effect. Experimental Psychology (formerly “Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie”), 55(2), 133–141.CrossRef
go back to reference Sumner, P., Nachev, P., Morris, P., Peters, A. M., Jackson, S. R., Kennard, C., et al. (2007). Human medial frontal cortex mediates unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Neuron, 54(5), 697–711.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Sumner, P., Nachev, P., Morris, P., Peters, A. M., Jackson, S. R., Kennard, C., et al. (2007). Human medial frontal cortex mediates unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Neuron, 54(5), 697–711.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van Gaal, S., & Lamme, V. A. (2012). Unconscious high-level information processing: implication for neurobiological theories of consciousness. Neuroscientist, 18(3), 287–301.CrossRefPubMed van Gaal, S., & Lamme, V. A. (2012). Unconscious high-level information processing: implication for neurobiological theories of consciousness. Neuroscientist, 18(3), 287–301.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 593–602.CrossRefPubMed Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 593–602.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van Veen, V., Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. Neuroimage, 14(6), 1302–1308.CrossRefPubMed van Veen, V., Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. Neuroimage, 14(6), 1302–1308.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Verleger, R., Jaśkowski, P., Aydemir, A., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative Differences Between Conscious and Nonconscious Processing? On Inverse Priming Induced by Masked Arrows. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(4), 494–515.CrossRef Verleger, R., Jaśkowski, P., Aydemir, A., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative Differences Between Conscious and Nonconscious Processing? On Inverse Priming Induced by Masked Arrows. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(4), 494–515.CrossRef
go back to reference Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of National Academic Science USA, 100(10), 6275–6280.CrossRef Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of National Academic Science USA, 100(10), 6275–6280.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The reversal of perceptual and motor compatibility effects differs qualitatively between metacontrast and random-line masks
Auteurs
Anne Atas
Estibaliz San Anton
Axel Cleeremans
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0611-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2015

Psychological Research 5/2015 Naar de uitgave