Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2016

28-03-2015 | Original Article

The relation between measures of cognitive and motor functioning in 5- to 6-year-old children

Auteurs: Tino Stöckel, Charmayne M. L. Hughes

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Specific relations between executive functions (working memory capacity, planning and problem-solving, inhibitory control) and motor skill performance (anticipatory motor planning, manual dexterity) were examined in 5- to 6-year-old children (N = 40). Results showed that the two motor skill components were not correlated. Additionally, it was found that response planning performance was a significant predictor of anticipatory motor planning performance, whereas inhibitory control and working memory capacity measures were significant predictors of manual dexterity scores. Taken together, these results suggest that cognitive and motor skills are linked, but that manual dexterity and anticipatory motor planning involve different specialized skills. The current study provides support for specific relations between cognitive and motor performance, which has implications for early childhood cognitive-motor training and intervention programs.
Literatuur
go back to reference Alloway, T. P., & Temple, K. J. (2007). A comparison of working memory skills and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and moderate learning difficulties. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 473–487.CrossRef Alloway, T. P., & Temple, K. J. (2007). A comparison of working memory skills and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and moderate learning difficulties. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 473–487.CrossRef
go back to reference Anderson, P., Anderson, V., & Lajoie, G. (1996). The Tower of London test: validation and standardization for pediatric populations. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(1), 54–65.CrossRef Anderson, P., Anderson, V., & Lajoie, G. (1996). The Tower of London test: validation and standardization for pediatric populations. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(1), 54–65.CrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews, 4, 829–839.CrossRefPubMed Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews, 4, 829–839.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bell, J. A., & Livesey, P. J. (1985). Cue significance and response regulation in 3-year-old to 6-year-old children’s learning of multiple-choice discrimination tasks. Developmental Psychobiology, 18(3), 229–245.CrossRefPubMed Bell, J. A., & Livesey, P. J. (1985). Cue significance and response regulation in 3-year-old to 6-year-old children’s learning of multiple-choice discrimination tasks. Developmental Psychobiology, 18(3), 229–245.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B, 57, 289–300. Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B, 57, 289–300.
go back to reference Berg, W. K., & Byrd, D. L. (2002). The Tower of London Spatial Problem Solving Task: enhancing Clinical and Research Implementation. Journal of Experimental and Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(5), 586–604.CrossRef Berg, W. K., & Byrd, D. L. (2002). The Tower of London Spatial Problem Solving Task: enhancing Clinical and Research Implementation. Journal of Experimental and Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(5), 586–604.CrossRef
go back to reference Bernard, J. A., & Seidler, R. D. (2013). Relationships between regional cerebellar volume and sensorimotor and cognitive function in young and older adults. The Cerebellum, 12(5), 721–737.CrossRefPubMed Bernard, J. A., & Seidler, R. D. (2013). Relationships between regional cerebellar volume and sensorimotor and cognitive function in young and older adults. The Cerebellum, 12(5), 721–737.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Campolo, D., Laschi, C., Keller, F., & Guglielmelli, E. (2007). A Mechatronic Platform for Early Diagnosis of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. RSJ Advanced Robotics Journal, 21(10), 1131–1150.CrossRef Campolo, D., Laschi, C., Keller, F., & Guglielmelli, E. (2007). A Mechatronic Platform for Early Diagnosis of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. RSJ Advanced Robotics Journal, 21(10), 1131–1150.CrossRef
go back to reference Chelune, G. J., & Baer, R. A. (1986). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin card sorting test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8(3), 219–228.CrossRefPubMed Chelune, G. J., & Baer, R. A. (1986). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin card sorting test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8(3), 219–228.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analyses for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analyses for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain [PhD-Thesis]. McGill University. Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain [PhD-Thesis]. McGill University.
go back to reference Cowan, N. (1998). Visual and auditory working memory capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 77–78.CrossRefPubMed Cowan, N. (1998). Visual and auditory working memory capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 77–78.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71, 44–56.CrossRefPubMed Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71, 44–56.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). The early years—Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387–1388.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). The early years—Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387–1388.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Drowatzky, J. N., & Zuccato, E. C. (1967). Interrelationships between selected measures of static and dynamic balance. Research Quarterly, 38, 509–510.PubMed Drowatzky, J. N., & Zuccato, E. C. (1967). Interrelationships between selected measures of static and dynamic balance. Research Quarterly, 38, 509–510.PubMed
go back to reference Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books. Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Edelman, G. M. (1989). The remembered present: a biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books. Edelman, G. M. (1989). The remembered present: a biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Eliasson, A. C., Rosblad, B., & Forssberg, H. (2004). Disturbances in programming goal-directed arm movements in children with ADHD. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46(1), 19–27.CrossRefPubMed Eliasson, A. C., Rosblad, B., & Forssberg, H. (2004). Disturbances in programming goal-directed arm movements in children with ADHD. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46(1), 19–27.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Espy, K. A. (2004). Using developmental, cognitive, and neuroscience approaches to understand executive control in young children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 379–384.CrossRefPubMed Espy, K. A. (2004). Using developmental, cognitive, and neuroscience approaches to understand executive control in young children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 379–384.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fliers, E., Rommelse, N., Vermeulen, S. H. H. M., Altink, M., Buschgens, C. J. M., Faraone, S. V., et al. (2008). Motor coordination problems in children and adolescents with ADHD rated by parents and teachers: effects of age and gender. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115(2), 211–220.CrossRefPubMed Fliers, E., Rommelse, N., Vermeulen, S. H. H. M., Altink, M., Buschgens, C. J. M., Faraone, S. V., et al. (2008). Motor coordination problems in children and adolescents with ADHD rated by parents and teachers: effects of age and gender. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115(2), 211–220.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1008–1017.CrossRefPubMed Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1008–1017.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Haga, M., Pedersen, A. V., & Sigmundsson, H. (2008). Interrelationship among selected measures of motor skills. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(2), 245–248. Haga, M., Pedersen, A. V., & Sigmundsson, H. (2008). Interrelationship among selected measures of motor skills. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(2), 245–248.
go back to reference Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1992). The movement assessment battery for children. London: Harcourt. Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1992). The movement assessment battery for children. London: Harcourt.
go back to reference Henderson, S. E., Sugden, D. A., & Barnett, A. L. (2007). Movement assessment battery for children—second edition (movement ABC-2). London: Harcourt Assessment. Henderson, S. E., Sugden, D. A., & Barnett, A. L. (2007). Movement assessment battery for children—second edition (movement ABC-2). London: Harcourt Assessment.
go back to reference Herbort, O., & Butz, M. V. (2011). Habitual and goal-directed factors in (everyday) object handling. Experimental Brain Research, 213, 371–382.CrossRefPubMed Herbort, O., & Butz, M. V. (2011). Habitual and goal-directed factors in (everyday) object handling. Experimental Brain Research, 213, 371–382.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hill, E. L., & Khanem, F. (2009). The development of hand preference in children: the effect of task demands and links with manual dexterity. Brain and Cognition, 71(2), 99–107.CrossRefPubMed Hill, E. L., & Khanem, F. (2009). The development of hand preference in children: the effect of task demands and links with manual dexterity. Brain and Cognition, 71(2), 99–107.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hughes, C. M. L., & Franz, E. A. (2008). Goal-related planning constraints in bimanual grasping and placing of objects. Experimental Brain Research, 188, 541–550.CrossRefPubMed Hughes, C. M. L., & Franz, E. A. (2008). Goal-related planning constraints in bimanual grasping and placing of objects. Experimental Brain Research, 188, 541–550.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hughes, C. M. L., Haddad, J. M., Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., & Ryu, J. H. (2011). Physically coupling two objects in a bimanual task alters kinematics but not end-state comfort. Experimental Brain Research, 211(2), 219–229.CrossRefPubMed Hughes, C. M. L., Haddad, J. M., Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., & Ryu, J. H. (2011). Physically coupling two objects in a bimanual task alters kinematics but not end-state comfort. Experimental Brain Research, 211(2), 219–229.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hughes, C. M. L., & Seegelke, C. (2013). Perturbations in action goal influence bimanual grasp posture planning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(6), 473–478.CrossRefPubMed Hughes, C. M. L., & Seegelke, C. (2013). Perturbations in action goal influence bimanual grasp posture planning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(6), 473–478.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hughes, C. M. L., Seegelke, C., & Schack, T. (2012). The influence of initial and final precision on motor planning: individual differences in end-state comfort during unimanual grasping and placing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 44(3), 1–7. Hughes, C. M. L., Seegelke, C., & Schack, T. (2012). The influence of initial and final precision on motor planning: individual differences in end-state comfort during unimanual grasping and placing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 44(3), 1–7.
go back to reference Jongbloed-Pereboom, M., Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W. G., Saraber-Schiphorst, N., Craje, C., & Steenbergen, B. (2013). Anticipatory action planning increases from 3 to 10 years of age in typically developing children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 295–305.CrossRefPubMed Jongbloed-Pereboom, M., Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W. G., Saraber-Schiphorst, N., Craje, C., & Steenbergen, B. (2013). Anticipatory action planning increases from 3 to 10 years of age in typically developing children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 295–305.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kaplan, B. J., Wilson, B. N., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471–490.CrossRef Kaplan, B. J., Wilson, B. N., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471–490.CrossRef
go back to reference Kessels, R. P. C., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). The Corsi Block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology, 7(4), 252–258.CrossRefPubMed Kessels, R. P. C., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). The Corsi Block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology, 7(4), 252–258.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kilshaw, D., & Annett, M. (1983). Right- and left-hand skill I: effects of age, sex and hand preference showing superior skill in left-handers. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 253–268.CrossRefPubMed Kilshaw, D., & Annett, M. (1983). Right- and left-hand skill I: effects of age, sex and hand preference showing superior skill in left-handers. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 253–268.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Livesey, D., Keen, J., Rouse, J., & White, F. (2006). The relationship between measures of executive function, motor performance and externalising behaviour in 5- and 6-year-old children. Human Movement Science, 25(1), 50–64.CrossRefPubMed Livesey, D., Keen, J., Rouse, J., & White, F. (2006). The relationship between measures of executive function, motor performance and externalising behaviour in 5- and 6-year-old children. Human Movement Science, 25(1), 50–64.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, S. W., & Fischman, M. G. (2011). The relationship between end-state comfort effects and memory performance in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 137(3), 292–299.CrossRefPubMed Logan, S. W., & Fischman, M. G. (2011). The relationship between end-state comfort effects and memory performance in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 137(3), 292–299.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lorås, H., & Sigmundsson, H. (2012). Interrelations between three fine motor skills in young adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 115(1), 171–178.CrossRefPubMed Lorås, H., & Sigmundsson, H. (2012). Interrelations between three fine motor skills in young adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 115(1), 171–178.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lorås, H., Stensdotter, A.-K., Öhberg, F., & Sigmundsson, H. (2013). Individual differences in motor timing and its relation to cognitive and fine motor skills. PLoS One, 8(7), e69353.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lorås, H., Stensdotter, A.-K., Öhberg, F., & Sigmundsson, H. (2013). Individual differences in motor timing and its relation to cognitive and fine motor skills. PLoS One, 8(7), e69353.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Nassif, E. P., Levy, A. M., Leite, W. B., & Fuentes, D. (2008). Planning abilities of children aged 4 years and 9 months to 8 ½ years. Effects of age, fluid intelligence and school type on peformance in the Tower of London test. Dementia and Neuropsychologia, 2(1), 26–30. Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Nassif, E. P., Levy, A. M., Leite, W. B., & Fuentes, D. (2008). Planning abilities of children aged 4 years and 9 months to 8 ½ years. Effects of age, fluid intelligence and school type on peformance in the Tower of London test. Dementia and Neuropsychologia, 2(1), 26–30.
go back to reference Manjunath, N. K., & Telles, S. (2001). Improved performance in the Tower of London test following yoga. Indian Journal of Physiological Pharmacology, 45, 351–354. Manjunath, N. K., & Telles, S. (2001). Improved performance in the Tower of London test following yoga. Indian Journal of Physiological Pharmacology, 45, 351–354.
go back to reference Michel, E., Roethlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., & Roebers, C. M. (2011). Development of cognitive skills in children with motor coordination impairments at 12-month follow-up. Child Neuropsychology, 17(2), 151–172.CrossRefPubMed Michel, E., Roethlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., & Roebers, C. M. (2011). Development of cognitive skills in children with motor coordination impairments at 12-month follow-up. Child Neuropsychology, 17(2), 151–172.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘‘frontal lobe’’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.CrossRefPubMed Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘‘frontal lobe’’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 257–263.CrossRefPubMed Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 257–263.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pangelinan, M. M., Zhang, G. Y., VanMeter, J. W., Clark, J. E., Hatfield, B. D., & Haufler, A. J. (2011). Beyond age and gender: relationships between cortical and subcortical brain volume and cognitive-motor abilities in school-age children. Neuroimage, 54(4), 3093–3100.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pangelinan, M. M., Zhang, G. Y., VanMeter, J. W., Clark, J. E., Hatfield, B. D., & Haufler, A. J. (2011). Beyond age and gender: relationships between cortical and subcortical brain volume and cognitive-motor abilities in school-age children. Neuroimage, 54(4), 3093–3100.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.CrossRefPubMed Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Petermann, F. (2009). Movement assessment battery for children-2 (M-ABC-2) (2. überarb. erw. Aufl.). Frankfurt/M.: Pearson Assessment. Petermann, F. (2009). Movement assessment battery for children-2 (M-ABC-2) (2. überarb. erw. Aufl.). Frankfurt/M.: Pearson Assessment.
go back to reference Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: International Universities Press. (Original work published in 1936). Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: International Universities Press. (Original work published in 1936).
go back to reference Piek, J. P., Dyck, M. J., Francis, M., & Conwell, A. (2007). Working memory, processing speed, and set-shifting in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(9), 678–683.CrossRefPubMed Piek, J. P., Dyck, M. J., Francis, M., & Conwell, A. (2007). Working memory, processing speed, and set-shifting in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(9), 678–683.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Piek, J. P., Pitcher, T. M., & Hay, D. A. (1999). Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41(3), 159–165.CrossRefPubMed Piek, J. P., Pitcher, T. M., & Hay, D. A. (1999). Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41(3), 159–165.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Piper, B. J., Li, V., Eiwaz, M. A., Kobel, Y. V., Benice, T. S., Chu, A. M., et al. (2012). Executive function on the Psychology Experiment Building Language tests. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 110–123.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Piper, B. J., Li, V., Eiwaz, M. A., Kobel, Y. V., Benice, T. S., Chu, A. M., et al. (2012). Executive function on the Psychology Experiment Building Language tests. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 110–123.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Rademaker, K. J., Lam, J. N. G. P., Van Haastert, I. C., Uiterwaal, C. S. P. M., Lieftink, A. F., Groenendaal, F., & De Vries, L. S. (2004). Larger corpus callosum size with better motor performance in prematurely born children. Seminars in Perinatology, 28(4), 279–287.CrossRefPubMed Rademaker, K. J., Lam, J. N. G. P., Van Haastert, I. C., Uiterwaal, C. S. P. M., Lieftink, A. F., Groenendaal, F., & De Vries, L. S. (2004). Larger corpus callosum size with better motor performance in prematurely born children. Seminars in Perinatology, 28(4), 279–287.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Roebers, C. M., & Kauer, M. (2009). Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Developmental Science, 12, 175–181.CrossRefPubMed Roebers, C. M., & Kauer, M. (2009). Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Developmental Science, 12, 175–181.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J. D., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1990). Constraints for action selection: overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J. D., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1990). Constraints for action selection: overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Roth, M., McCaul, E., & Barnes, K. (1993). Who becomes an at-risk student—the predictive value of a kindergarten screening battery. Exceptional Children, 59(4), 348–358.PubMed Roth, M., McCaul, E., & Barnes, K. (1993). Who becomes an at-risk student—the predictive value of a kindergarten screening battery. Exceptional Children, 59(4), 348–358.PubMed
go back to reference Schack, T., & Mechsner, F. (2006). Representation of motor skills in human long-term-memory. Neuroscience Letters, 391, 77–81.CrossRefPubMed Schack, T., & Mechsner, F. (2006). Representation of motor skills in human long-term-memory. Neuroscience Letters, 391, 77–81.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 7–12.CrossRefPubMed Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 7–12.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairment of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209.CrossRefPubMed Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairment of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stöckel, T., Hughes, C. M. L., & Schack, T. (2012). Representation of postures and anticipatory motor planning in children. Psychological Research, 76(6), 768–776.CrossRefPubMed Stöckel, T., Hughes, C. M. L., & Schack, T. (2012). Representation of postures and anticipatory motor planning in children. Psychological Research, 76(6), 768–776.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRef Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRef
go back to reference Sugden, D. (Ed.). (2006). Developmental coordination disorder as a specific learning difficulty. Leeds Consensus Statement. Cardiff: The Dyscovery Trust. Sugden, D. (Ed.). (2006). Developmental coordination disorder as a specific learning difficulty. Leeds Consensus Statement. Cardiff: The Dyscovery Trust.
go back to reference Thibaut, J. P., & Toussaint, L. (2010). Developing motor planning over ages. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 116–129.CrossRefPubMed Thibaut, J. P., & Toussaint, L. (2010). Developing motor planning over ages. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 116–129.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tiemeier, H., Lenroot, R. K., Greenstein, D. K., Tran, L., Pierson, R., & Giedd, J. N. (2010). Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal morphometric MRI study. Neuroimage, 49(1), 63–70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tiemeier, H., Lenroot, R. K., Greenstein, D. K., Tran, L., Pierson, R., & Giedd, J. N. (2010). Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal morphometric MRI study. Neuroimage, 49(1), 63–70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Verté, S., Geurts, H. M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2006). The relationship of working memory, inhibition, and response variability in child psychopathology. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 151(1), 5–14.CrossRefPubMed Verté, S., Geurts, H. M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2006). The relationship of working memory, inhibition, and response variability in child psychopathology. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 151(1), 5–14.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wassenberg, R., Feron, F., Kessels, A., Hendriksen, J., Kalff, A., Kroes, M., et al. (2005). Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: results from a largescale cross-sectional study. Child Development, 76(5), 1092–1103.CrossRefPubMed Wassenberg, R., Feron, F., Kessels, A., Hendriksen, J., Kalff, A., Kroes, M., et al. (2005). Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: results from a largescale cross-sectional study. Child Development, 76(5), 1092–1103.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weigelt, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., Huelshorst, S., & Schack, T. (2009). Moving and memorizing: motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 132, 68–79.CrossRefPubMed Weigelt, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., Huelshorst, S., & Schack, T. (2009). Moving and memorizing: motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 132, 68–79.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weigelt, M., & Schack, T. (2010). The development of end-state comfort planning in preschool children. Experimental Psychology, 57(6), 476–482.CrossRefPubMed Weigelt, M., & Schack, T. (2010). The development of end-state comfort planning in preschool children. Experimental Psychology, 57(6), 476–482.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weimer, W. B. (1977). A conceptual framework for cognitive psychology: Motor theories of the mind. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: toward an ecological psychology (pp. 267–311). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Weimer, W. B. (1977). A conceptual framework for cognitive psychology: Motor theories of the mind. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: toward an ecological psychology (pp. 267–311). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Wright, I., Waterman, M., Prescott, H., & Murdoch-Eaton, D. (2003). A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 561–575.CrossRefPubMed Wright, I., Waterman, M., Prescott, H., & Murdoch-Eaton, D. (2003). A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 561–575.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wunsch, K., Henning, A., Aschersleben, G., & Weigelt, M. (2013). A systematic review of the end-state comfort effect in normally developing children and in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 1(3), 59–76. Wunsch, K., Henning, A., Aschersleben, G., & Weigelt, M. (2013). A systematic review of the end-state comfort effect in normally developing children and in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 1(3), 59–76.
go back to reference Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Resnick, J. S., & Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive function: a problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1, 198–226.CrossRef Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Resnick, J. S., & Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive function: a problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1, 198–226.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The relation between measures of cognitive and motor functioning in 5- to 6-year-old children
Auteurs
Tino Stöckel
Charmayne M. L. Hughes
Publicatiedatum
28-03-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0662-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2016

Psychological Research 4/2016 Naar de uitgave