Discussion
The goal of the current study was to elucidate the relationships among Chinese parents’ emotion regulation, parenting stress, and parenting behaviors in families of children with ASD while considering the effects of the dynamic interactions between fathers and mothers. This study is among the first to utilize dyadic analysis (i.e., actor partner interdependence mediation model) to investigate the potential spillover and crossover effects within families of children with ASD. The findings of the current study improve our understanding of how emotional factors contribute to parenting stress and how emotional factors are associated with the behaviors of fathers and mothers when parenting their children with ASD. These results have important implications for potential targeted intervention programs for parents of children with ASD, that the emotion regulation training programs for parents may decrease perceived parenting stress and thus promote more adaptive parenting behaviors toward children with ASD.
First, the correlational results suggested nonindependence between fathers and mothers. Specifically, significant correlations were found between fathers and mothers for emotion dysregulation, parenting stress and parenting behaviors (i.e., care, overprotection, and optimal bonding). Therefore, it is important to assess fathers’ and mothers’ influences in the same model to understand the potential spillover and crossover effects in the families of children with ASD. In addition, the correlational results suggested that Chinese mothers’ heightened levels of difficulties with emotion regulation were associated with an increase of their own parenting stress but not that of their spouses, while when fathers reported more difficulties with emotion regulation, both parents’ reported higher perceived stress when providing care to their children with ASD. We also found significant correlations between Chinese parenting stress and both parents’ care, overprotection and optimal parenting behaviors. The only exception was the link between maternal stress and paternal care; specifically, mothers’ parenting stress was not significantly associated with their spouses’ care behaviors. These correlational results suggest that fathers and mothers may have differential roles within family systems when co-parenting children with ASD (Cridland et al.
2014): compared to fathers, Chinese and that mothers may be more subject to spillover and crossover effects within family systems when facing emotional challenges.
The APIMeMs were then applied to test our main hypotheses. We first hypothesized that a higher level of emotion dysregulation would be directly associated with heightened parenting stress, increased negative parenting behaviors, and decreased positive parenting behaviors for both parents in Chinese families of children with ASD. Additionally, we hypothesized that both fathers’ and mothers’ perceived stress from being a parent would be associated with their own and their partners’ parenting behaviors. The actor effects of these links, but not the partner effects, were supported by our results.
Specifically, consistent with the correlational results, Chinese parents with greater emotion regulation difficulties self-reported more parenting stress, and parents with higher levels of parenting stress self-reported more parental overprotection/control, less care/affection, and less optimal bonding with their children with ASD, even after controlling for the spouses’ emotion dysregulation and parenting stress. This finding provided empirical evidence to theoretical propositions regarding the relationships between parental emotional well-being and parenting behaviors in families of children with ASD (Dix
1991; Adam et al.
2004). Note that the direct effects of parental emotion dysregulation on self-reported parenting behaviors remained significant after controlling for spouses’ emotion dysregulation and both parents’ parenting stress. These results suggest that other possible mediators, such as marital relationships and co-parenting, may exist under these mechanisms (e.g., Katz and Gottman
1996).
We then tested our mediational hypotheses regarding the dyadic interactions among fathers’ and mothers’ emotion regulation, parenting stress, and parenting behaviors within the family system of children with ASD. The results mostly supported our hypotheses on actor, but not partner levels, suggesting the existence of a spillover rather than crossover effect within families of children with ASD. Specifically, parents who showed more difficulties with emotion regulation were more likely to perceive parenting stress, which in turn was associated with fewer optimal bonding behaviors, more overprotection, and less care toward their child. However, there was one exception: maternal parenting stress did not significantly mediate the links between maternal emotion dysregulation and maternal care.
Specifically, it seemed that for both the mothers and fathers of children with ASD in Chinese culture, difficulties coping with daily emotional challenges might be linked to their elevated levels of perceived stress when caring for their children with ASD. This heightened stress perception, in turn, seemed to relate to their use of optimal bonding behaviors with their children. Additionally, these pathways remained significant even after other factors, such as children’s daily living skills, household income, number of children in the family and spouses’ parenting stress and emotion dysregulation, were considered. Note that although we found only a marginal mediating effect of self-reported parenting stress on the link between Chinese parents’ emotion dysregulation and overprotection for mothers and on the link between emotion dysregulation and care for fathers, the pattern of results is still meaningful and consistent with our predictions.
These actor-level results were consistent with the theoretical proposition that the emotions of the individuals (e.g., parents’ emotions) within the family systems could directly influence those individuals’ relationship with their child (e.g., parent–child relationship; Cox and Paley
2003). This is one of few studies to use family systems approach to examine the family dynamics in families of children with developmental disabilities, despite the continuous call to adopt family systems approach as a guiding framework for family-focused ASD research (Cridland et al.
2014). These results indicate the importance of providing emotional support and training in emotion regulation strategies to both parents and to include both fathers and mothers as target primary caregiver in clinical support services. In China, mothers of children with ASD have long been the service targets in clinics and agencies that aim to provide interventions for children with ASD in home settings, and fathers have always been ignored (China Association of Persons with Psychiatric Disability and Their Relatives
2014). Systematic training on emotion regulation for both mothers and fathers should be provided in clinics and agencies to empower parents with emotion coping strategies. Furthermore, the provision of in-home family support programs for both parents by local government agencies in charge of affairs for children with autism (i.e., local association for people with disabilities) will help mothers and fathers respond to parenting pressure and challenges positively by teaching them how to recognize emotional pressure and strength, act as role models, promote resilience, and build competence (Wang and Hu
2014).
The partner-level pathways, however, were not significant for either positive or negative parenting behaviors. Although the crossover process of family systems theory (Erel and Burman
1995) suggests that parents’ emotion regulation might be both associated with their own parenting behaviors through its impact on perceived parenting stress and linked to their spouses’ parenting behaviors through their impact on the spouses’ stress perception, we did not find such crossover effects in our sample of Chinese families of children with ASD.
On the surface, these results imply that one partner’ emotion dysregulation and parenting stress may not contribute to the other partner’s parenting behaviors beyond the individual’s own emotion dysregulation and parenting stress in this specific group (i.e., Chinese parents of children with ASD). One possible explanation is that unlike in other groups of children (e.g., typically developing children), the distinct challenges associated with ASD might lead these Chinese parents to be overly occupied with dealing with their children’s core symptoms and mediating the children’s social interactions (e.g., Heiman and Berger
2008). This focus often means that parents must constantly cope with their children’s lack of spontaneity, sudden mood changes, and inflexible daily routines (Latefa and Ahmad
2015; Pakenham et al.
2004). Such heightened demands might deplete parents’ opportunities to become highly involved in interactions with their co-parents. Another possible explanation is that mothers and fathers have clear-cut, distinct roles in daily family life, especially within the traditional Chinese cultural background. Specifically, fathers may be responsible for supporting, providing for, and protecting his family by paying for all family expenses and the costs of interventions and therapy for the child with ASD, while mothers usually stay home to take care of the child with ASD (China Association of Persons with Psychiatric Disability and Their Relatives
2014). Additionally, compared with families within the Western culture, emotional interactions between Chinese mothers and fathers are introverted and implicit (Ma and Lai
2016). Such family values and role divisions might impede spousal communication, bonding, and closeness.
Although no partner effects were found in our models, other potential partner-level pathways could still existing. For example, emotion dysregulation and parenting stress were correlated within couples, so it is possible that one parent’s emotion dysregulation and parenting stress influence his/her partner’s parenting behaviors through the bidirectional exchanges between fathers and mothers on emotion dysregulation and parenting stress. Previous work has supported the interconnectedness of daily emotions in marital relationships (Schoebi
2008) and the correlations between couples’ self-reported parenting stress in families of children with ASD (Pisula and Porębowicz-Dörsmann
2017); thus, it is possible that the participants’ parenting stress and behaviors were influenced by their partners’ emotion dysregulation through the coregulation processes within couples. Of course, the cross-sectional design of our study limited our ability to make any statement regarding time order or causality.
Although this study expands the parenting literature on ASD from a dyadic perspective, multiple limitations exist and warrant caution for interpretation. First, the present study was limited by its reliance on paternal and maternal self-reports. At present, the findings should be interpreted with caution as the study was subject to potential biases, such as social desirability bias and response bias. For example, the major study variables, such as parenting emotion regulation and parenting behaviors, and the covariates, such as child academic performance and daily living skills, were assessed only by the parents and may have been biased by parents’ tendency to give socially acceptable answers, even if they are not true. Future studies should consider assessing these variables through multiple approaches (e.g., behavioral observation, psychophysiological assessment, teachers’ reports) to overcome the issues with social desirability and response bias and create a more comprehensive picture.
Second, other covariates, such as child intellectual abilities, should be considered. Unfortunately, we were not able to obtain the children’s IQ information. Although we included child academic performance, which is strongly correlated with intellectual abilities in children with ASD (e.g., Venter et al.
1992), other studies have shown significant discrepancies between intellectual abilities and academic performance (Estes et al.
2011). Thus, future studies that include other important covariates (e.g., child IQ) and assess covariates using more sophisticated instruments and multiple perspectives (e.g., teachers’ report, behavioral observation) are warranted. Third, the cross-sectional design of the current study limited our abilities to test causal relations or determine the directionality of the associations among examined variables. Future longitudinal studies are strongly encouraged to investigate the causality and relationships among these parenting constructs and to measure changes in these constructs over time.
Additionally, the optimal parenting subscale created in the current study was consistent with studies with typically developing children and children with other types of psychopathology (e.g., ADHD, anxiety disorder); however, whether overprotection can always be considered a negative parenting characteristic should be considered with caution. Future studies are strongly encouraged to improve our understanding of this parenting behavior in families of children with ASD. Lastly, although our study was not designed in a cross-cultural manner, this emerging evidence suggests that future studies should include measures of culturally relevant constructs (e.g., parents’ emotion regulation) during their investigations.
In summary, our results support the existence of spillover effects (but not crossover effects) in the relationships among parental emotion regulation, parenting stress and parenting behaviors. The present study contributes to the literature in several important ways. First, our study is among the first to investigate the mechanisms of the links between emotion regulation and parenting behaviors by including both parents’ parenting stress as mediators. Second, it examined these relationships using dyadic analysis, enabling an examination of whether potential family processes (i.e., spillover and crossover processes) exist under stricter control. Third, our findings further indicate the need to provide family-centered support to both mothers and fathers to enhance their roles in parenting children with ASD in China (Hu et al.
2015). Support for parents might include, but should not be limited to, empowerment strategies, parent–child relationship-building skills, coping skills for working with challenging behaviors, and spousal emotional assistance strategies. In addition to providing and tailoring support and services to parents, education programs for addressing and enhancing daily living skills of school-aged children with ASD should be prioritized because daily living skills were significantly correlated with parenting stress. Daily living skills programs should be integrated into current teaching programs at special education schools for children with ASD.