Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2008

01-07-2008 | Original Article

The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks

Auteurs: Mike Wendt, Andrea Kiesel

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In task switching experiments participants have to respond to the same set of stimuli while task instructions vary (e.g., digit stimuli are assigned to left- or right-sided key presses by means of magnitude vs. parity classification). Response congruency effects denote worse performance for a stimulus, which is associated with different responses in the two tasks as compared to a stimulus, which is associated with the same response. Previous research suggests that such effects reflect direct links between stimuli and responses acquired in the course of experimental practice. In the current study we investigated the impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instruction by reversing the S–R mapping of one task (Experiment 1) or replacing one task with a new one (Experiment 2) in the second half of an experimental session. Consistent with the direct link account, S–R links practiced during the first half of the experiment largely determined congruency effects despite altered task instructions. Furthermore, the results suggest that previously practiced S–R links (a) can be relatively quickly overwritten by practicing a novel S–R mapping, and (b) are subject to passive decay when no longer in use.
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (2000). Task switching, stimulus–response bindings, and negative priming. In: S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge: MIT Press. Allport, A., & Wylie, G. (2000). Task switching, stimulus–response bindings, and negative priming. In: S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umiltà, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT Press. Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umiltà, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Fagot, C. (1994). Chronometric investigations of task switching. Ph. D. Dissertation, University of California, San Diego. Fagot, C. (1994). Chronometric investigations of task switching. Ph. D. Dissertation, University of California, San Diego.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Kiesel, A., & Sebald, A. (2003). Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15, 101–128.CrossRef Hoffmann, J., Kiesel, A., & Sebald, A. (2003). Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15, 101–128.CrossRef
go back to reference Hübner, R. (1997). The effect of spatial frequency on global precedence and hemispheric differences. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 187–201. Hübner, R. (1997). The effect of spatial frequency on global precedence and hemispheric differences. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 187–201.
go back to reference Hübner, M., Kluwe, R. H, Luna-Rodriguez, A., & Peters, A. (2004). Response selection difficulty and asymmetrical costs of switching between tasks and stimuli: no evidence for an exogenous component of task-set reconfiguration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 1043–1063.PubMedCrossRef Hübner, M., Kluwe, R. H, Luna-Rodriguez, A., & Peters, A. (2004). Response selection difficulty and asymmetrical costs of switching between tasks and stimuli: no evidence for an exogenous component of task-set reconfiguration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 1043–1063.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2007). Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects. Psychological Research, 71, 117–125.PubMedCrossRef Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2007). Task switching: on the origin of response congruency effects. Psychological Research, 71, 117–125.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koch, I., & Allport, A. (2006). Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching. Memory & Cognition, 34, 433–444. Koch, I., & Allport, A. (2006). Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching. Memory & Cognition, 34, 433–444.
go back to reference Lamb, M. R., & Yund, E. W. (1993). The role of spatial frequency in the processing of hierarchically organized stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 773–784. Lamb, M. R., & Yund, E. W. (1993). The role of spatial frequency in the processing of hierarchically organized stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 773–784.
go back to reference Lien, M. -C., Ruthruff, E., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (2005). On the limits of advance preparation for a task switch: do people prepare all the task some of the time or some of the task all the time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 299–315.PubMedCrossRef Lien, M. -C., Ruthruff, E., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (2005). On the limits of advance preparation for a task switch: do people prepare all the task some of the time or some of the task all the time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 299–315.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.CrossRef Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.CrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.PubMedCrossRef Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning: 1. Locus of practice effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 20–32.CrossRef Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning: 1. Locus of practice effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 20–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, R., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 763–797.PubMedCrossRef Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 763–797.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime User’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime User’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: role of episodic S–R bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.PubMedCrossRef Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: role of episodic S–R bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2007). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, 71, 92–106.PubMedCrossRef Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2007). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, 71, 92–106.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks
Auteurs
Mike Wendt
Andrea Kiesel
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-007-0117-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2008

Psychological Research 4/2008 Naar de uitgave