Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2013

01-03-2013 | Original Article

The effects of an unexpected spider stimulus on skin conductance responses and eye movements: an inattentional blindness study

Auteurs: Julian Wiemer, Antje B. M. Gerdes, Paul Pauli

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Are spiders evolutionarily relevant threat cues that capture attention automatically and preattentively—also in non-fearful persons? Previous studies concerning this question did not examine responses to unexpected spider stimuli, although this is an ecologically valid situation. Therefore, we conducted an inattentional blindness (IB) experiment. While unselected participants (N = 120) were focusing their attention on a visual discrimination task, suddenly a spider or a flower image appeared. The rates of reported detection were not enhanced for spider images, and fear of spiders had no moderating influence on IB rates. However, spider pictures in general elicited greater skin conductance responses than flower pictures, and this difference was also observed for pictures that were not reported afterward. Moreover, eye movement data revealed that the spider stimulus was initially fixated more often than the flower stimulus. In sum, our findings imply that enhanced reactions to unexpected spider images are independent of fear level and include early engagement processes. Awareness of a spider picture does not seem to be a prerequisite for elevated arousal.
Voetnoten
1
In exploratory analyses, we also defined high and low fearful groups by means of a median split or by comparing the 25% with the lowest and the highest scores, respectively. These analyses, as with a continuous variable, did not reveal significant differences between high and low fearful participants in detection rates, skin conductance responses and eye movements.
 
2
In a different analysis with a time window of 1–5 s after stimulus onset and a minimum response criterion of 0.01 µS, all significant results were maintained.
 
3
The leaves of the flower used in the experiment share features with the leg of a spider. However, we avoided a flower stem without leaves because of shared features with the discrimination task’s cross; shared features are known to cause reduced IB effects (Mack & Rock, 1998).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Alpers, G. W., Gerdes, A. B. M., Lagarie, B., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2009). Attention and amygdala activity: An fMRI study with spider pictures in spider phobia. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116, 747–757.PubMedCrossRef Alpers, G. W., Gerdes, A. B. M., Lagarie, B., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2009). Attention and amygdala activity: An fMRI study with spider pictures in spider phobia. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116, 747–757.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24.PubMedCrossRef Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Batty, M. J., Cave, K. R., & Pauli, P. (2005). Abstract stimuli associated with threat through conditioning cannot be detected preattentively. Emotion, 5, 418–430.PubMedCrossRef Batty, M. J., Cave, K. R., & Pauli, P. (2005). Abstract stimuli associated with threat through conditioning cannot be detected preattentively. Emotion, 5, 418–430.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beanland, V., & Pammer, K. (2010). Looking without seeing or seeing without looking? Eye movements in sustained inattentional blindness. Vision Research, 50, 977–988.PubMedCrossRef Beanland, V., & Pammer, K. (2010). Looking without seeing or seeing without looking? Eye movements in sustained inattentional blindness. Vision Research, 50, 977–988.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115.PubMedCrossRef Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Breska, A., Maoz, K., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2011). Interstimulus intervals for skin conductance response measurement. Psychophysiology, 48, 437–440.PubMedCrossRef Breska, A., Maoz, K., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2011). Interstimulus intervals for skin conductance response measurement. Psychophysiology, 48, 437–440.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Büchel, C., Morris, J., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (1998). Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 20, 947–957.PubMedCrossRef Büchel, C., Morris, J., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (1998). Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 20, 947–957.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carlsson, K., Petersson, K. M., Lundqvist, D., Karlsson, A., Ingvar, M., & Öhman, A. (2004). Fear and the amygdala: Manipulation of awareness generates differential cerebral responses to phobic and fear-relevant (but non-feared) stimuli. Emotion, 4, 340–353.PubMedCrossRef Carlsson, K., Petersson, K. M., Lundqvist, D., Karlsson, A., Ingvar, M., & Öhman, A. (2004). Fear and the amygdala: Manipulation of awareness generates differential cerebral responses to phobic and fear-relevant (but non-feared) stimuli. Emotion, 4, 340–353.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cavanagh, K., & Davey, G. C. L. (2000). UCS expectancy biases in spider phobics: Underestimation of aversive consequences following fear-irrelevant stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 641–651.PubMedCrossRef Cavanagh, K., & Davey, G. C. L. (2000). UCS expectancy biases in spider phobics: Underestimation of aversive consequences following fear-irrelevant stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 641–651.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700.CrossRef Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerdes, A. B. M., Alpers, G. W., & Pauli, P. (2008). When spiders appear suddenly: Spider-phobic patients are distracted by task-irrelevant spiders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 174–187.PubMedCrossRef Gerdes, A. B. M., Alpers, G. W., & Pauli, P. (2008). When spiders appear suddenly: Spider-phobic patients are distracted by task-irrelevant spiders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 174–187.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gerdes, A. B. M., Pauli, P., & Alpers, G. W. (2009). Toward and away from spiders: Eye-movements in spider-fearful participants. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116, 725–733.PubMedCrossRef Gerdes, A. B. M., Pauli, P., & Alpers, G. W. (2009). Toward and away from spiders: Eye-movements in spider-fearful participants. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116, 725–733.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hamm, A. O., & Weike, A. I. (2005). The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 5–14.PubMedCrossRef Hamm, A. O., & Weike, A. I. (2005). The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 5–14.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 284–291.PubMedCrossRef Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 284–291.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2006). Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: Visual processing speed revisited. Vision Research, 46, 1762–1776.PubMedCrossRef Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2006). Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: Visual processing speed revisited. Vision Research, 46, 1762–1776.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Schweckendiek, J., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2009). Neural, electrodermal and behavioral response patterns in contingency aware and unaware subjects during a picture–picture conditioning paradigm. Neuroscience, 158, 721–731.PubMedCrossRef Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Schweckendiek, J., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2009). Neural, electrodermal and behavioral response patterns in contingency aware and unaware subjects during a picture–picture conditioning paradigm. Neuroscience, 158, 721–731.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kolassa, I.-T., Buchmann, A., Lauche, R., Kolassa, S., Partchev, I., Miltner, W., et al. (2007). Spider phobics more easily see a spider in morphed schematic pictures. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3, 59.PubMedCrossRef Kolassa, I.-T., Buchmann, A., Lauche, R., Kolassa, S., Partchev, I., Miltner, W., et al. (2007). Spider phobics more easily see a spider in morphed schematic pictures. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3, 59.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004a). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4, 312–317.PubMedCrossRef Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004a). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4, 312–317.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004b). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1183–1192.PubMedCrossRef Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004b). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1183–1192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster. LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.
go back to reference Lee, H., & Telch, M. J. (2008). Attentional biases in social anxiety: An investigation using the inattentional blindness paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 819–835.PubMedCrossRef Lee, H., & Telch, M. J. (2008). Attentional biases in social anxiety: An investigation using the inattentional blindness paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 819–835.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear-relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5, 365–369.PubMedCrossRef Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear-relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5, 365–369.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: Attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7, 192–200.PubMedCrossRef Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: Attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7, 192–200.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.PubMedCrossRef MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 563–569.PubMedCrossRef Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 563–569.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mayer, B., Muris, P., Vogel, L., Nojoredjo, I., & Merckelbach, H. (2006). Fear-relevant change detection in spider-fearful and non-fearful participants. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 510–519.PubMedCrossRef Mayer, B., Muris, P., Vogel, L., Nojoredjo, I., & Merckelbach, H. (2006). Fear-relevant change detection in spider-fearful and non-fearful participants. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 510–519.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miltner, W. H. R., Krieschel, S., Hecht, H., Trippe, R., & Weiss, T. (2004). Eye movements and behavioral responses to threatening and nonthreatening stimuli during visual search in phobic and nonphobic subjects. Emotion, 4, 323–339.PubMedCrossRef Miltner, W. H. R., Krieschel, S., Hecht, H., Trippe, R., & Weiss, T. (2004). Eye movements and behavioral responses to threatening and nonthreatening stimuli during visual search in phobic and nonphobic subjects. Emotion, 4, 323–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6, 257–268.PubMedCrossRef Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6, 257–268.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A., Carlsson, K., Lundqvist, D., & Ingvar, M. (2007). On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiology & Behavior, 92, 180–185.CrossRef Öhman, A., Carlsson, K., Lundqvist, D., & Ingvar, M. (2007). On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiology & Behavior, 92, 180–185.CrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130, 466–478.PubMedCrossRef Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130, 466–478.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231–240.PubMedCrossRef Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231–240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pessoa, L., Japee, S., Sturman, D., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2006). Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 366–375.PubMedCrossRef Pessoa, L., Japee, S., Sturman, D., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2006). Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 366–375.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., & Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage, 28, 249–255.PubMedCrossRef Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., & Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage, 28, 249–255.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Phaf, R. H., & Kan, K. J. (2007). The automaticity of emotional Stroop: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(2), 184–199.PubMedCrossRef Phaf, R. H., & Kan, K. J. (2007). The automaticity of emotional Stroop: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(2), 184–199.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pischek-Simpson, L. K., Boschen, M. J., Neumann, D. L., & Waters, A. M. (2009). The development of an attentional bias for angry faces following Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 322–330.PubMedCrossRef Pischek-Simpson, L. K., Boschen, M. J., Neumann, D. L., & Waters, A. M. (2009). The development of an attentional bias for angry faces following Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 322–330.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.PubMedCrossRef Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Recio, G., Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009). Effects of inter-stimulus interval on skin conductance responses and event-related potentials in a Go/NoGo task. Biological Psychology, 80, 246–250.PubMedCrossRef Recio, G., Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009). Effects of inter-stimulus interval on skin conductance responses and event-related potentials in a Go/NoGo task. Biological Psychology, 80, 246–250.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rinck, M., Bundschuh, S., Englert, S., Müller, A., Wissmann, J., Ellwart, T., et al. (2002). Reliabilität und Validität dreier Instrumente zur Messung von Angst vor Spinnen. Diagnostica, 48, 141–149.CrossRef Rinck, M., Bundschuh, S., Englert, S., Müller, A., Wissmann, J., Ellwart, T., et al. (2002). Reliabilität und Validität dreier Instrumente zur Messung von Angst vor Spinnen. Diagnostica, 48, 141–149.CrossRef
go back to reference Rinck, M., Reinecke, A., Ellwart, T., Heuer, K., & Becker, E. S. (2005). Speeded detection and increased distraction in fear of spiders: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 235–248.PubMedCrossRef Rinck, M., Reinecke, A., Ellwart, T., Heuer, K., & Becker, E. S. (2005). Speeded detection and increased distraction in fear of spiders: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 235–248.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.PubMedCrossRef Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Straube, T., Mentzel, H. J., & Miltner, W. H. (2006). Neural mechanisms of automatic and direct processing of phobogenic stimuli in specific phobia. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 162–170.PubMedCrossRef Straube, T., Mentzel, H. J., & Miltner, W. H. (2006). Neural mechanisms of automatic and direct processing of phobogenic stimuli in specific phobia. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 162–170.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Szczepanowski, R., & Pessoa, L. (2007). Fear perception: Can objective and subjective awareness measures be dissociated? Journal of Vision, 7, 10.PubMedCrossRef Szczepanowski, R., & Pessoa, L. (2007). Fear perception: Can objective and subjective awareness measures be dissociated? Journal of Vision, 7, 10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Usunoff, K. G., Schmitt, O., Itzev, D. E., Rolfs, A., & Wree, A. (2007). Efferent connections of the parabigeminal nucleus to the amygdala and the superior colliculus in the rat: A double-labeling fluorescent retrograde tracing study. Brain Research, 1133, 87–91.PubMedCrossRef Usunoff, K. G., Schmitt, O., Itzev, D. E., Rolfs, A., & Wree, A. (2007). Efferent connections of the parabigeminal nucleus to the amygdala and the superior colliculus in the rat: A double-labeling fluorescent retrograde tracing study. Brain Research, 1133, 87–91.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Colour naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 97–108.PubMedCrossRef Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Colour naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 97–108.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wieser, M. J., Pauli, P., & Mühlberger, A. (2009). Probing the attentional control theory in social anxiety: An emotional saccade task. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 314–322.CrossRef Wieser, M. J., Pauli, P., & Mühlberger, A. (2009). Probing the attentional control theory in social anxiety: An emotional saccade task. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 314–322.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting memories (pp. 71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting memories (pp. 71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Wurtz, R. H., & Goldberg, M. E. (1971). Superior colliculus cell responses related to eye movements in awake monkeys. Science, 171, 82–84.PubMedCrossRef Wurtz, R. H., & Goldberg, M. E. (1971). Superior colliculus cell responses related to eye movements in awake monkeys. Science, 171, 82–84.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 665–681. Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 665–681.
Metagegevens
Titel
The effects of an unexpected spider stimulus on skin conductance responses and eye movements: an inattentional blindness study
Auteurs
Julian Wiemer
Antje B. M. Gerdes
Paul Pauli
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2013
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0407-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2013

Psychological Research 2/2013 Naar de uitgave