Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2013

01-03-2013 | Original Article

The effect of distraction on face and voice recognition

Auteurs: Sarah V. Stevenage, Greg J. Neil, Jess Barlow, Amy Dyson, Catherine Eaton-Brown, Beth Parsons

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The results of two experiments are presented which explore the effect of distractor items on face and voice recognition. Following from the suggestion that voice processing is relatively weak compared to face processing, it was anticipated that voice recognition would be more affected by the presentation of distractor items between study and test compared to face recognition. Using a sequential matching task with a fixed interval between study and test that either incorporated distractor items or did not, the results supported our prediction. Face recognition remained strong irrespective of the number of distractor items between study and test. In contrast, voice recognition was significantly impaired by the presence of distractor items regardless of their number (Experiment 1). This pattern remained whether distractor items were highly similar to the targets or not (Experiment 2). These results offer support for the proposal that voice processing is a relatively vulnerable method of identification.
Voetnoten
1
Analysis in signal detection terms enables scrutiny of both sensitivity of discrimination (d’) and bias (C). Analysis of bias here revealed no effects either of the number of intervening stimuli (F (2, 76) < 1, ns), stimulus type (F (1, 38) < 1, ns) or their interaction (F (2, 76) < 1, ns).
 
2
As in Experiment 1, analysis of bias is included here to explore the effect of stimulus type, distraction and distractor strength on responding. Only one interaction emerged as a weak effect (distraction x stimulus type: F (2, 122) = 3.23, p < .05, ή2 = .05). However, there was no effect of distraction on bias for faces (F (2, 64) = 1.17, ns) or voices (F (2, 62) = 2.53, ns), and there was no significant difference in levels of bias between faces and voices at either zero distractors (t (63) = −1.53, ns), two distractors (t (63) = 1.20, ns) or four distractors (t (63) = −1.61, ns). This interaction instead seemed to capture a reversal of small levels of bias between faces and voices when there were two distractors. No other main effects of interactions reached significance (Fs < 1.38, ns).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, O. O., & Wittlenger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 104, 54–75.CrossRef Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, O. O., & Wittlenger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 104, 54–75.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsics, C., & Brédart, S. (2011). Recalling episodic information about personally known faces and voices. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 303–308.PubMedCrossRef Barsics, C., & Brédart, S. (2011). Recalling episodic information about personally known faces and voices. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 303–308.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barsics, C., & Brédart, S. (2012). Recalling semantic information about newly learned faces and voices. Memory, 20(5), 527–534.PubMedCrossRef Barsics, C., & Brédart, S. (2012). Recalling semantic information about newly learned faces and voices. Memory, 20(5), 527–534.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brédart, S., Barsics, C., & Hanley, R. (2009). Recalling semantic information about personally known faces and voices. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1013–1021.CrossRef Brédart, S., Barsics, C., & Hanley, R. (2009). Recalling semantic information about personally known faces and voices. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1013–1021.CrossRef
go back to reference Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Johnston, R. A. (1990). Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 361–380.PubMedCrossRef Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Johnston, R. A. (1990). Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 361–380.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clifford, B. R. (1980). Voice identification by human listeners: On earwitness reliability. Law and Human Behavior, 4(4), 373–394.CrossRef Clifford, B. R. (1980). Voice identification by human listeners: On earwitness reliability. Law and Human Behavior, 4(4), 373–394.CrossRef
go back to reference Cook, S., & Wilding, J. (1997). Earwitness Testimony 2: Voices, Faces and Context. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 527–541.CrossRef Cook, S., & Wilding, J. (1997). Earwitness Testimony 2: Voices, Faces and Context. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 527–541.CrossRef
go back to reference Damjanovic, L. (2011). The face advantage in recalling episodic information: Implications for modelling human memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 309–311.PubMedCrossRef Damjanovic, L. (2011). The face advantage in recalling episodic information: Implications for modelling human memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 309–311.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Damjanovic, L., & Hanley, J. R. (2007). Recalling episodic and semantic information about famous faces and voices. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1205–1210.CrossRef Damjanovic, L., & Hanley, J. R. (2007). Recalling episodic and semantic information about famous faces and voices. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1205–1210.CrossRef
go back to reference Ellis, H. D., Jones, D. M., & Mosdell, N. (1997). Intra- and inter-modal repetition priming of familiar faces and voices. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 143–156.PubMedCrossRef Ellis, H. D., Jones, D. M., & Mosdell, N. (1997). Intra- and inter-modal repetition priming of familiar faces and voices. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 143–156.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hanley, J. R., & Damjanovic, L. (2009). It is more difficult to retrieve a familiar person’s name and occupation from their voice than from their blurred face. Memory, 17, 830–839.PubMedCrossRef Hanley, J. R., & Damjanovic, L. (2009). It is more difficult to retrieve a familiar person’s name and occupation from their voice than from their blurred face. Memory, 17, 830–839.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hanley, J. R., Smith, S. T., & Hadfield, J. (1998). I recognise you but can’t place you. An investigation of familiar-only experiences during tests of voice and face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A(1), 179–195. Hanley, J. R., Smith, S. T., & Hadfield, J. (1998). I recognise you but can’t place you. An investigation of familiar-only experiences during tests of voice and face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A(1), 179–195.
go back to reference Hanley, J. R., & Turner, J. M. (2000). Why are familiar-only experiences more frequent for voices than for faces? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 1105–1116. Hanley, J. R., & Turner, J. M. (2000). Why are familiar-only experiences more frequent for voices than for faces? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 1105–1116.
go back to reference Hinz, T., & Pezdek, K. (2001). The effect of exposure to multiple lineups on face identification accuracy. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 185–198.PubMedCrossRef Hinz, T., & Pezdek, K. (2001). The effect of exposure to multiple lineups on face identification accuracy. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 185–198.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kerstholt, J. H., Jansen, N. H. M., van Amelsvoort, A. G., & Broeders, A. P. A. (2006). Earwitnesses: Effects of accent, retention and telephone. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 187–197.CrossRef Kerstholt, J. H., Jansen, N. H. M., van Amelsvoort, A. G., & Broeders, A. P. A. (2006). Earwitnesses: Effects of accent, retention and telephone. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 187–197.CrossRef
go back to reference McAllister, H. A., Dale, R. H. I., Bregman, N. J., McCabe, A., & Cotton, C. R. (1993). When eyewitnesses are also earwitnesses: Effects on visual and voice identifications. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 14(2), 161–170.CrossRef McAllister, H. A., Dale, R. H. I., Bregman, N. J., McCabe, A., & Cotton, C. R. (1993). When eyewitnesses are also earwitnesses: Effects on visual and voice identifications. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 14(2), 161–170.CrossRef
go back to reference McGehee, F. (1937). The reliability of the identification of the human voice. Journal of General Psychology, 17, 249–271.CrossRef McGehee, F. (1937). The reliability of the identification of the human voice. Journal of General Psychology, 17, 249–271.CrossRef
go back to reference Saslove, H., & Yarmey, A. D. (1980). Long-term auditory memory: Speaker identification. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 111–116.PubMedCrossRef Saslove, H., & Yarmey, A. D. (1980). Long-term auditory memory: Speaker identification. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 111–116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schweinberger, S. R., Herholz, A., & Stief, V. (1997). Auditory long-term memory: Repetition priming of voice recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A(3), 498–517. Schweinberger, S. R., Herholz, A., & Stief, V. (1997). Auditory long-term memory: Repetition priming of voice recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A(3), 498–517.
go back to reference Stevenage, S. V., Howland, A., & Tippelt, A. (2011). Interference in eyewitness and earwitness recognition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 112–118.CrossRef Stevenage, S. V., Howland, A., & Tippelt, A. (2011). Interference in eyewitness and earwitness recognition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 112–118.CrossRef
go back to reference Stevenage, S. V., Hugill, A. R., & Lewis, H. G. (2012). Integrating voice recognition into models of person perception. Journal of Cognitive Psychology (in press). Stevenage, S. V., Hugill, A. R., & Lewis, H. G. (2012). Integrating voice recognition into models of person perception. Journal of Cognitive Psychology (in press).
go back to reference Yarmey, A. D. (1995). Earwitness speaker identification. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 1(4), 792–816.CrossRef Yarmey, A. D. (1995). Earwitness speaker identification. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 1(4), 792–816.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The effect of distraction on face and voice recognition
Auteurs
Sarah V. Stevenage
Greg J. Neil
Jess Barlow
Amy Dyson
Catherine Eaton-Brown
Beth Parsons
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2013
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0450-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2013

Psychological Research 2/2013 Naar de uitgave