Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 8/2020

20-06-2019 | Original Article

The devil is in the details: investigating the influence of emotion on event memory using a simulated event

Auteurs: Adam R. Congleton, Dorthe Berntsen

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In our everyday lives, the negative events we experience sometimes include powerful, salient details which are ultimately responsible for us interpreting the events as negative (i.e., they drive our interpretation of the emotional valence of the event). Here we examined how the presence of such details within an event shapes our memory of that event. Research on the role of emotion in memory suggests that negative events are often remembered more accurately than positive ones, and this advantage is especially pronounced for the emotion-defining details of the events. However, research rarely separates retrieval effects from effects of attention and information processing during encoding. Here we used a simulated event to examine (1) whether negative events are remembered more accurately than positive events, (2) whether this effect is more pronounced for the emotion-defining detail of the event, (3) whether participants display enhanced accuracy for all aspects of an event (i.e., general memory enhancement) or for only certain aspects of the event (i.e., selective memory enhancement), and (4) whether any enhancement effects for central aspects of the event occur at the expense of contextual information (i.e., memory narrowing). Across three experiments, participants showed superior memory accuracy for the central details of the event in general, while those who interpreted the event as emotionally negative also displayed selective enhancement of the peripheral details. The results further suggested that it was the unexpectedness and/or salience introduced by the emotion-defining detail that was essential to enhancing memory accuracy, and not its goal relevance.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The choice of this story as part of our materials did not reflect any sympathy with burglars or thieves, or any other illegal activity. Rather, the story was chosen to create an emotional event that was not offensive (e.g., through the use of highly upsetting scenes or images). We followed the example of Anderson and Pichert (1978), who likewise asked the participants to view a story from the point of view of a potential burglar.
 
2
As there was never a significant difference across conditions in any of the experiments with regard to critical lures, we will not be discussing them further. Since the participants were demonstrating near-ceiling effects of accuracy regarding these questions, we can consider them as another manipulation check demonstrating that participants were indeed paying careful attention to the video during encoding.
 
3
To determine if our positive valence condition was positive with respect to the neutral rating on the scale used in this study to assess emotional valence, we conducted one-sample t tests for both the positive and negative valence conditions, where we compared the mean of participants’ self-reported emotional valence of each condition to the neutral rating of the scale. The results of these tests demonstrated that both conditions significantly differed from the neutral rating point (i.e., the midpoint of 4 on a 7 point Likert-type scale) in their respective directions on the rating scale (positive valence condition: M = 5.02, SD 1.48, t(99) = 6.87, p < 0.001; negative valence condition: M = 2.75, SD 1.70, t(99) = -7.37, p < 0.001).
 
4
To ensure that the only difference between the saliency conditions was participants’ accuracy regarding the critical final detail of the man/gate, we examined participants’ accuracy regarding the final detail (man vs. gate) compared to the mean of their accuracy for all of the other central detail questions combined (excluding the final detail question). The results revealed there were no differences between the conditions in terms of participants’ accuracy for all of the central details except for the man and gate (M_Positive = 0.66, M_Negative = 0.65). However, as previously reported, participants in the negative valence condition were significantly more accurate regarding the final detail of the man (M = 0.84) compared to those in the positive valence condition regarding the final detail of the garden gate (M = 0.35).
 
5
Of course, one cannot rule out the possibility that the central details included in this study were remembered better because of potentially greater inherent saliency of (some of) these details. However, many of the details identified as “central” would most likely not have been considered salient if they were not relevant to the goal of being a thief (e.g., the flat-screen TV in the living room, which was not particularly unique, but could be considered a desirable object for a thief to steal). Of course, distinguishing between the effects of the inherent saliency of details and their increased salience as a result of being goal relevant is beyond the scope of this study.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anderson, R. C., & Pichert, J. W. (1978). Recall of previously unrecallable information following a shift in perspective. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 1–12.CrossRef Anderson, R. C., & Pichert, J. W. (1978). Recall of previously unrecallable information following a shift in perspective. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 1–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D. (2002). Tunnel memories for autobiographical events: Central details are remembered more frequently from shocking than from happy experiences. Memory and Cognition, 30, 1010–1020.PubMedCrossRef Berntsen, D. (2002). Tunnel memories for autobiographical events: Central details are remembered more frequently from shocking than from happy experiences. Memory and Cognition, 30, 1010–1020.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 352–372.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 352–372.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2007). Pleasantness bias in flashbulb memories: Positive and negative flashbulb memories of the fall of the Berlin Wall among East and West Germans. Memory and Cognition, 35, 565–577.PubMedCrossRef Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2007). Pleasantness bias in flashbulb memories: Positive and negative flashbulb memories of the fall of the Berlin Wall among East and West Germans. Memory and Cognition, 35, 565–577.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Breslin, C. W., & Safer, M. A. (2011). Effects of event valence on long-term memory for two baseball championship games. Psychological Science, 22, 1408–1412.PubMedCrossRef Breslin, C. W., & Safer, M. A. (2011). Effects of event valence on long-term memory for two baseball championship games. Psychological Science, 22, 1408–1412.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buhrmester, M. D., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2018). An evaluation of Amazon’s mechanical turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 149–154.PubMedCrossRef Buhrmester, M. D., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2018). An evaluation of Amazon’s mechanical turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 149–154.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Burt, C. D. B., Watt, S. C., Mitchell, D. A., & Conway, M. A. (1998). Retrieving the sequence of autobiographical event components. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 321–338.CrossRef Burt, C. D. B., Watt, S. C., Mitchell, D. A., & Conway, M. A. (1998). Retrieving the sequence of autobiographical event components. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 321–338.CrossRef
go back to reference Chipchase, S. Y., & Chapman, P. (2013). Trade-offs in visual attention and the enhancement of memory specificity for positive and negative emotional stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 277–298.PubMedCrossRef Chipchase, S. Y., & Chapman, P. (2013). Trade-offs in visual attention and the enhancement of memory specificity for positive and negative emotional stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 277–298.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Compton, R. J. (2003). The interface between emotion and attention: A review of evidence from psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2, 115–129.PubMedCrossRef Compton, R. J. (2003). The interface between emotion and attention: A review of evidence from psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2, 115–129.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frey, U., & Morris, R. G. M. (1997). Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature, 385, 533–536.CrossRef Frey, U., & Morris, R. G. M. (1997). Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature, 385, 533–536.CrossRef
go back to reference Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorization. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 322–337.CrossRef Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorization. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 322–337.CrossRef
go back to reference Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). Attentional consequences of pregoal and postgoal positive affects. Emotion, 11, 1358–1367.PubMedCrossRef Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). Attentional consequences of pregoal and postgoal positive affects. Emotion, 11, 1358–1367.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grysman, A. (2014). The roles of gender and temporal distance in the recall of dissonant self- related memories. Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 10–22.PubMedCrossRef Grysman, A. (2014). The roles of gender and temporal distance in the recall of dissonant self- related memories. Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 10–22.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grysman, A., Prabhakar, J., Anglin, S. M., & Hudson, J. A. (2014). Self-enhancement and the life script in future thinking across the lifespan. Memory, 23, 22. Grysman, A., Prabhakar, J., Anglin, S. M., & Hudson, J. A. (2014). Self-enhancement and the life script in future thinking across the lifespan. Memory, 23, 22.
go back to reference Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (1990). Vivid memories of emotional events: The accuracy of remembered minutiae. Memory and Cognition, 18, 496–506.PubMedCrossRef Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (1990). Vivid memories of emotional events: The accuracy of remembered minutiae. Memory and Cognition, 18, 496–506.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Holmes, E. A., & Bourne, C. (2008). Inducing and modulating intrusive emotional memories: A review of the trauma film paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 127, 553–566.PubMedCrossRef Holmes, E. A., & Bourne, C. (2008). Inducing and modulating intrusive emotional memories: A review of the trauma film paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 127, 553–566.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Memory for specific visual details can be enhanced by negative arousing content. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 99–112.CrossRef Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Memory for specific visual details can be enhanced by negative arousing content. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 99–112.CrossRef
go back to reference Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006a). When the Red Sox shocked the Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 757–763.PubMedCrossRef Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006a). When the Red Sox shocked the Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 757–763.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006b). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 2564–2570.PubMedCrossRef Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006b). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 2564–2570.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laney, C., Campbell, H. V., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (2004). Memory for thematically arousing events. Memory and Cognition, 32, 1149–1159.PubMedCrossRef Laney, C., Campbell, H. V., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (2004). Memory for thematically arousing events. Memory and Cognition, 32, 1149–1159.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laney, C., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (2003). Thematically induced arousal in naturally occurring emotional memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 995–1004.CrossRef Laney, C., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (2003). Thematically induced arousal in naturally occurring emotional memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 995–1004.CrossRef
go back to reference Levine, L. J., & Bluck, S. (2004). Painting with broad strokes: Happiness and the malleability of event memory. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 559–574.CrossRef Levine, L. J., & Bluck, S. (2004). Painting with broad strokes: Happiness and the malleability of event memory. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 559–574.CrossRef
go back to reference Levine, L. J., & Burgess, S. L. (1997). Beyond general arousal: Effects of specific emotions on memory. Social Cognition, 15, 157–181.CrossRef Levine, L. J., & Burgess, S. L. (1997). Beyond general arousal: Effects of specific emotions on memory. Social Cognition, 15, 157–181.CrossRef
go back to reference Levine, L. J., & Edelstein, R. S. (2009). Emotion and memory narrowing: A review and goal-relevance approach. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 833–875.CrossRef Levine, L. J., & Edelstein, R. S. (2009). Emotion and memory narrowing: A review and goal-relevance approach. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 833–875.CrossRef
go back to reference Levine, L. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2004). Emotion and memory research: A grumpy overview. Social Cognition, 22, 530–554.CrossRef Levine, L. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2004). Emotion and memory research: A grumpy overview. Social Cognition, 22, 530–554.CrossRef
go back to reference Loftus, E. F., & Burns, T. E. (1982). Mental shock can produce retrograde amnesia. Memory and Cognition, 10, 318–323.PubMedCrossRef Loftus, E. F., & Burns, T. E. (1982). Mental shock can produce retrograde amnesia. Memory and Cognition, 10, 318–323.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Luminet, O., & Curci, A. (2009). Flashbulb memories: New issues and new perspectives. Hove: Psychology Press. Luminet, O., & Curci, A. (2009). Flashbulb memories: New issues and new perspectives. Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2012). Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavioral Research Methods, 44, 1–23.CrossRef Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2012). Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavioral Research Methods, 44, 1–23.CrossRef
go back to reference Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 33–52.PubMedCrossRef Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 33–52.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ochsner, K. N. (2000). Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 129, 242–261.PubMedCrossRef Ochsner, K. N. (2000). Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 129, 242–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 3–41). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 3–41). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Rubin, D. C., & Umanath, S. (2015). Event memory: A theory of memory for laboratory, autobiographical and fictional events. Psychological Review, 122, 1–23.PubMedCrossRef Rubin, D. C., & Umanath, S. (2015). Event memory: A theory of memory for laboratory, autobiographical and fictional events. Psychological Review, 122, 1–23.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRef Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRef
go back to reference Talarico, J. M., Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2009). Positive emotions enhance recall of peripheral details. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 380–398.PubMedCrossRef Talarico, J. M., Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2009). Positive emotions enhance recall of peripheral details. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 380–398.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Bergen, P., & Salmon, K. (2011). Attachment and emotion knowledge in development. In J. Low & P. Jose (Eds.), Lifespan development: New Zealand perspectives. Pearson: Auckland. Van Bergen, P., & Salmon, K. (2011). Attachment and emotion knowledge in development. In J. Low & P. Jose (Eds.), Lifespan development: New Zealand perspectives. Pearson: Auckland.
Metagegevens
Titel
The devil is in the details: investigating the influence of emotion on event memory using a simulated event
Auteurs
Adam R. Congleton
Dorthe Berntsen
Publicatiedatum
20-06-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01215-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2020

Psychological Research 8/2020 Naar de uitgave