Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2015

01-03-2015 | Original Article

The benefit of no choice: goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing

Auteurs: Markus Janczyk, Michael Dambacher, Maik Bieleke, Peter M. Gollwitzer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Choosing among different options is costly. Typically, response times are slower if participants can choose between several alternatives (free-choice) compared to when a stimulus determines a single correct response (forced-choice). This performance difference is commonly attributed to additional cognitive processing in free-choice tasks, which require time-consuming decisions between response options. Alternatively, the forced-choice advantage might result from facilitated perceptual processing, a prediction derived from the framework of implementation intentions. This hypothesis was tested in three experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 were PRP experiments and showed the expected underadditive interaction of the SOA manipulation and task type, pointing to a pre-central perceptual origin of the performance difference. Using the additive-factors logic, Experiment 3 further supported this view. We discuss the findings in the light of alternative accounts and offer potential mechanisms underlying performance differences in forced- and free-choice tasks.
Voetnoten
1
Note that Berlyne (1957a, b) has already speculated about why participants do respond at all in free-choice tasks and why the respective RTs are longer than in forced-choice tasks. Briefly, he alluded to the idea of enhanced response competition in the case of free-choice tasks where no clear stimulus-induced bias exists. We return to this interpretation in the General Discussion and relate it to the present findings.
 
2
Of course, individuals could spontaneously (i.e., without explicit instruction) conceive free choices as stimulus–response links like “if an X appears, then I press the left key about half of the times”. However, research indicates that most individuals substantially benefit from the explicit instruction to use such stimulus–response links (for review, see Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006), suggesting that spontaneous planning does not play a critical role. Further, implementation intentions are the more effective the more clearly defined the linked behavior in the “then”-part is (Gollwitzer, 1993; Gollwitzer, Wieber, Meyers, & McCrea, 2010). The definiteness of the if–then plan is necessarily higher for forced- than for free-choice tasks in setups like the present study. Thus, one might in fact speak of stimulus–response links that are provided by forced-choice instructions. We do not deny this possibility and return to this view in the General Discussion. For now, however, as our reasoning was derived from the framework of implementation intentions, we prefer speaking of if–then plans.
 
3
In Berlyne’s (1957a) experiments both (forced-choice) stimuli were presented in a free-choice trial.
 
4
Due to a programming error, participants 1–16 of Experiment 1b had only 85 instead of 90 trials per block.
 
5
At least in Experiment 3, Berlyne (1957a) doubled the free-choice trials for analyses to reach a comparable numbers of trials.
 
6
This refers not only to the immediately preceding trial, but to the longer history of previous responses.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 971–979.CrossRef Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 971–979.CrossRef
go back to reference Achtziger, A., Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Committing to implementation intentions: attention and memory effects for selected situational cues. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 287–300.CrossRef Achtziger, A., Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Committing to implementation intentions: attention and memory effects for selected situational cues. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 287–300.CrossRef
go back to reference Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2009). Responding to subliminal cues: do if-then plans facilitate action preparation and initiation without conscious intent? Social Cognition, 27, 183–201.CrossRef Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2009). Responding to subliminal cues: do if-then plans facilitate action preparation and initiation without conscious intent? Social Cognition, 27, 183–201.CrossRef
go back to reference Berlyne, D. E. (1957b). Uncertainty and conflict: a point of contact between information-theory and behavior-theory concepts. Psychological Review, 64, 329–339.CrossRefPubMed Berlyne, D. E. (1957b). Uncertainty and conflict: a point of contact between information-theory and behavior-theory concepts. Psychological Review, 64, 329–339.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bieleke, M., Dambacher, M., Hübner, R., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (2013). A sequential sampling model account of implementation intention effects. Manuscript in preparation. Bieleke, M., Dambacher, M., Hübner, R., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (2013). A sequential sampling model account of implementation intention effects. Manuscript in preparation.
go back to reference Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.CrossRef Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.CrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The what, when, whether model of intentional action. The Neuroscientist, 14, 319–325.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The what, when, whether model of intentional action. The Neuroscientist, 14, 319–325.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.PubMed Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.PubMed
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: the role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.CrossRef Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: the role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.CrossRef
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.CrossRef Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.CrossRef
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186.CrossRef Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186.CrossRef
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.CrossRef Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.CrossRef
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M., Wieber, F., Meyers, A. L., & McCrea, S. M. (2010). How to maximize implementation intention effects. In C. R. Agnew, D. E. Carlston, W. G. Graziano, & J. R. Kelly (Eds.), Then a miracle occurs: focusing on behavior in social psychological theory and research (pp. 137–161). New York: Oxford Press. Gollwitzer, P. M., Wieber, F., Meyers, A. L., & McCrea, S. M. (2010). How to maximize implementation intention effects. In C. R. Agnew, D. E. Carlston, W. G. Graziano, & J. R. Kelly (Eds.), Then a miracle occurs: focusing on behavior in social psychological theory and research (pp. 137–161). New York: Oxford Press.
go back to reference Herwig, A., & Horstmann, G. (2011). Action–effect associations revealed by eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 531–537.CrossRefPubMed Herwig, A., & Horstmann, G. (2011). Action–effect associations revealed by eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 531–537.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(11), 1540–1554.CrossRef Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(11), 1540–1554.CrossRef
go back to reference Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2009). Intention and attention in ideomotor learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 219–227.CrossRefPubMed Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2009). Intention and attention in ideomotor learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 219–227.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2012). Action–effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 444.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2012). Action–effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 444.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMed Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M. (2013). Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1878–1887.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M. (2013). Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1878–1887.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Heinemann, A., & Pfister, R. (2012a). Instant attraction: immediate action–effect bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 446.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Janczyk, M., Heinemann, A., & Pfister, R. (2012a). Instant attraction: immediate action–effect bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 446.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Nolden, S., & Jolicoeur, P. (2014). No difference in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks. Manuscript in revision. Janczyk, M., Nolden, S., & Jolicoeur, P. (2014). No difference in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks. Manuscript in revision.
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Crognale, M., & Kunde, W. (2012b). Effective rotations: action effects determine the interplay of mental and manual rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 489–501.CrossRef Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Crognale, M., & Kunde, W. (2012b). Effective rotations: action effects determine the interplay of mental and manual rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 489–501.CrossRef
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Skirde, S., Weigelt, M., & Kunde, W. (2009). Visual and tactile action effects determine bimanual coordination performance. Human Movement Science, 28, 437–449.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Skirde, S., Weigelt, M., & Kunde, W. (2009). Visual and tactile action effects determine bimanual coordination performance. Human Movement Science, 28, 437–449.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: evidence for nonaction–effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 542–549.CrossRefPubMed Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: evidence for nonaction–effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 542–549.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.PubMed Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.PubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). The locus of tool-transformation costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 703–714.PubMed Kunde, W., Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). The locus of tool-transformation costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 703–714.PubMed
go back to reference Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100.CrossRefPubMed Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238.CrossRef Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Mattler, U., & Palmer, S. (2012). Time course of free-choice priming effects explained by a simple accumulator model. Cognition, 123, 360–437.CrossRef Mattler, U., & Palmer, S. (2012). Time course of free-choice priming effects explained by a simple accumulator model. Cognition, 123, 360–437.CrossRef
go back to reference Metzker, M., & Dreisbach, G. (2009). Bidirectional priming processes in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1770–1783.PubMed Metzker, M., & Dreisbach, G. (2009). Bidirectional priming processes in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1770–1783.PubMed
go back to reference Miller, J., & Reynolds, A. (2003). The locus of redundant-targets and non-targets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1126–1142.PubMed Miller, J., & Reynolds, A. (2003). The locus of redundant-targets and non-targets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1126–1142.PubMed
go back to reference Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.PubMed Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.PubMed
go back to reference Nachev, P., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2008). Functional role of supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 856–869.CrossRefPubMed Nachev, P., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2008). Functional role of supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 856–869.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2000). Motivational and volitional processes in action initiation: a field study of the role of implementation intentions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 780–797.CrossRef Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2000). Motivational and volitional processes in action initiation: a field study of the role of implementation intentions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 780–797.CrossRef
go back to reference Paelecke, M., & Kunde, W. (2007). Action–effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 627–644.PubMed Paelecke, M., & Kunde, W. (2007). Action–effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 627–644.PubMed
go back to reference Parks-Stamm, E. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Action control by implementation intentions: effective cue detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, 25, 248–266.CrossRef Parks-Stamm, E. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Action control by implementation intentions: effective cue detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, 25, 248–266.CrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377.PubMed Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377.PubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45.CrossRef Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45.CrossRef
go back to reference Passingham, R. E., Bengtsson, S. L., & Lau, H. C. (2010). Medial frontal cortex: from self-generated action to reflection on one’s own performance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 16–21.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Passingham, R. E., Bengtsson, S. L., & Lau, H. C. (2010). Medial frontal cortex: from self-generated action to reflection on one’s own performance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 16–21.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., Heinemann, A., Kiesel, A., Thomaschke, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Do endogenous and exogenous action control compete for perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 279–284.PubMed Pfister, R., Heinemann, A., Kiesel, A., Thomaschke, R., & Janczyk, M. (2012). Do endogenous and exogenous action control compete for perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 279–284.PubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research, 75, 61–65.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research, 75, 61–65.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Melcher, T. (2010). Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations. Acta Psychologica, 135(3), 316–322.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Melcher, T. (2010). Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations. Acta Psychologica, 135(3), 316–322.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2013). Dissecting the response in response-effect compatibility. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 647–655.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2013). Dissecting the response in response-effect compatibility. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 647–655.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.CrossRefPubMed Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109, 160–174.CrossRefPubMed Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109, 160–174.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1998). Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 10–20. Prinz, W. (1998). Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung. Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 10–20.
go back to reference Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85(2), 59–108.CrossRef Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85(2), 59–108.CrossRef
go back to reference Schüür, F., & Haggard, P. (2011). What are self-generated actions? Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1697–1704.CrossRefPubMed Schüür, F., & Haggard, P. (2011). What are self-generated actions? Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1697–1704.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schweickert, R. (1978). A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: analysis of a Stroop task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 18, 105–139.CrossRef Schweickert, R. (1978). A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: analysis of a Stroop task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 18, 105–139.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.CrossRef Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.CrossRef
go back to reference Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.PubMed Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.PubMed
go back to reference Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1997). Decision and response in dual-task interference. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 266–307.CrossRefPubMed Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1997). Decision and response in dual-task interference. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 266–307.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2007). How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 295–302.CrossRef Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2007). How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 295–302.CrossRef
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1951). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance: a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19. Welford, A. T. (1951). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance: a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19.
go back to reference Wieber, F., & Sassenberg, K. (2006). I can’t take my eyes off it: attention attraction effects of implementation intentions. Social Cognition, 24, 723–752.CrossRef Wieber, F., & Sassenberg, K. (2006). I can’t take my eyes off it: attention attraction effects of implementation intentions. Social Cognition, 24, 723–752.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolfensteller, U., & Ruge, H. (2011). On the timescale of stimulus-based action–effect learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1273–1289.CrossRef Wolfensteller, U., & Ruge, H. (2011). On the timescale of stimulus-based action–effect learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1273–1289.CrossRef
go back to reference Wykowska, A., & Schubö, A. (2012). Action intentions modulate allocation of visual attention: electrophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 379.PubMedCentralPubMed Wykowska, A., & Schubö, A. (2012). Action intentions modulate allocation of visual attention: electrophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 379.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Wykowska, A., Schubö, A., & Hommel, B. (2009). How you move is what you see: action planning biases selection in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1755–1769.PubMed Wykowska, A., Schubö, A., & Hommel, B. (2009). How you move is what you see: action planning biases selection in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1755–1769.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
The benefit of no choice: goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing
Auteurs
Markus Janczyk
Michael Dambacher
Maik Bieleke
Peter M. Gollwitzer
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0549-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2015

Psychological Research 2/2015 Naar de uitgave