Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 14-11-2022 | Original Article

The Association Between Persistence and Change in Behavioral Difficulties During Early to Middle Childhood and Cognitive Abilities at Age 8

Auteurs: Denise Neumann, Elizabeth R. Peterson, Lisa Underwood, Susan M.B. Morton, Karen E. Waldie

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 4/2024

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

We investigated the association between persistence and change in behavioral difficulties during early to middle childhood and several cognitive outcomes. We observed 3904 8-year-olds enrolled in the longitudinal study Growing Up in New Zealand (50% male/female; 23% Māori, 9% Pacific Peoples, 13% Asian, 2% Middle Eastern/Latin American/African, 9% Other, 43% European). The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery was used to assess cognitive functioning at 8 years and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire for behavioral difficulties at 4.5 and 8 years. Multivariate logistic regression analyses were conducted, controlling for well-known sociodemographic confounders. Children with persistent or later onset of behavioral difficulties were at higher risk for poorer vocabulary, reading, inhibitory control/attention, episodic memory, working memory and processing speed at age 8 compared to children with no or improved difficulties. Our study supports the importance of addressing both cognitive and behavioral aspects when planning educational programmes and interventions in early and middle childhood.
Opmerkingen

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abbreviations:
National Institute of Health (NIH)
Cognitive Battery (CB)
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)
Data Collection Wave (DCW), Aotearoa New Zealand (NZ).

Introduction

In recent decades, a considerable amount of research has emerged on the association of behavioral difficulties and the negative impact on a child’s ability to fulfil their educational and developmental potential [1]. Evidence exists that there is a generally a strong link between behavioral problems and cognitive performance in early and middle childhood, [2, 3], notwithstanding that the relationship is likely to be bidirectional in nature [4]. Gremillion and Martel [5] examined associations between language variation and disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) in preschool children. The authors found that children with DBD showed poorer receptive, expressive, and pragmatic language skills compared to children without DBD. Furthermore, preschoolers with increased parent-rated hyperactivity-impulsivity scores demonstrated poorer language skills [5]. Likewise, Sim et al. [6] found language difficulties in preschool children who scored in the abnormal range of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) total difficulties score. A further study compared 7-11-year-olds, who were identified as showing concerning behavior at school, to age- and sex-matched controls. The authors found that children with behavioral concerns were more likely to show structural language, word decoding and pragmatic language difficulties compared with the control group [7].
The link between behavioral difficulties and cognition extends to executive functioning in early and middle childhood. For instance, Raaijmakers and colleagues [72] observed that preschool children with aggressive behavior as indicated by scores at or above the 93rd percentile on the Aggressive Behavior Scale [8] showed inhibition deficits based on six neuropsychological tests [9]. Moreover, Jahromi and Stifter [10] found that preschool children with lower executive functioning on tasks involving inhibition of a prepotent response showed poorer emotion regulation and were less able to control their impulsive behaviors. A further study compared children aged 8–11 years who were identified as hyperactive according to teacher-ratings on the SDQ to matched controls on a battery of cognitive tasks. The authors found that children with hyperactivity performed more poorly on tasks of inhibition and executive function, as well as literacy measures, compared to the control group [11]. Furthermore, hyperactivity and conduct problems measured by the SDQ, have been found to be negatively correlated to reading and arithmetic performance assessed with standardized tests in children aged 3–6 years [12].
A range of longitudinal research has investigated the link between children’s behavior and cognitive functions. For instance, in the Christchurch Health and Development Study, Fergusson, Horwood and Lynsky [13] showed that early behavioral tendencies are associated with later developmental outcomes, i.e. conduct problems at age 7–9 years increased the risk of later juvenile offending while attention deficits were related to academic under-achievement in middle childhood. Furthermore, Helland and colleagues [4] looked at language ability in a group of adolescents (12–15 years) with externalising behavior problems compared to a typically developing comparison group. The group with behavioral problems showed poorer language ability and 70% showed language impairments in the clinical range. The authors further found that language and emotional and peer problems assessed with the SDQ in childhood (age 7–9 years), were strongly correlated with language impairments in adolescence (age 12–15 years). The authors concluded that the assessment of language, especially pragmatics, is crucial for follow-up and treatment of behavioral problems in children and adolescents (Helland, Lundervold, Heimann, & Posserud, [4]). McGee et al. [14] investigated to what extent hyperactivity during both the preschool years and at age of school entry leads to later academic and behavioral problems in adolescence by using data from two longitudinal studies, the Australian Temperament Project and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study. They found a strong linear relationship between early hyperactivity and continuing school difficulties, attention problems as well as poor reading at age 15.
Existing longitudinal research has mainly focused on how early cognitive difficulties relate to later behavioral problems, rather than vice versa, although the association between cognitive difficulties and behavioral problems is likely to be bidirectional in nature [4, 15, 16]. While some research indicates that behavior problems predispose the child to under-achievement, there is also evidence that language impairments can increase the occurrence of behavioral difficulties [1719].
In a recent longitudinal study in Aotearoa New Zealand, using data from the large birth cohort study Growing Up in New Zealand, D’Souza and colleagues [20] investigated whether persistence and change in serious behavioral problems from ages 2 to 4.5 years is linked to cognitive delay at preschool age in the areas of receptive language, early literacy and executive control. The total difficulties score of the SDQ was used at each time point and children were then categorised as showing no difficulties, improved behavior, concurrent difficulties, and persistent difficulties. The findings indicated that children with concurrent and persistent behavioral difficulties were more likely to show cognitive delays compared to children with no difficulties, after controlling for a range of sociodemographic covariates. They were also at greater risk of having comorbid delays across several cognitive domains. The authors noted that one limitation of the study was that some of the cognitive measures used at 4.5 years were more of an indicator of early cognitive ability and the lack of using a comprehensive neurocognitive battery. Furthermore, it would be of interest to investigate how persistence and change in behavioral difficulties from preschool to school age might have shaped the relationship between behavioral difficulties and cognitive abilities [20].
Transitioning from preschool to school age is an important period for cognitive development. Not only do developmental spurts of key executive functioning take place during this time [21], the transition is also often accompanied by major environmental changes in the children’s lives that may generate new cognitive demands, alongside potential socio-emotional and behavioral challenges [22]. Previous literature shows that while most behavioral difficulties in preschool children tend to be related to developmental phases of testing out boundaries and are no longer evident after transition to school, for some children behavioral difficulties, especially emotional problems, persist during early childhood as well as after school entry, [2326]. To what extend behavioral difficulties change from early to middle childhood, especially when transitioning into school and the impact this has on cognitive abilities requires further investigation.
To address this and to extend the existing literature, the current study aimed to explore the association between persistence and change in behavioral difficulties administered with the SDQ (when the Growing Up in New Zealand children were 4.5 and 8 years of age) and cognitive performance in the comprehensive NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery at 8 years. The cognitive outcomes of interest were comprised of language (reading and vocabulary), executive functioning (inhibitory control/attention and cognitive flexibility) episodic memory, working memory and processing speed.

Methods

Participants

Participants were members of the Growing Up in New Zealand study, a prospective cohort study with 6822 pregnant women recruited via three contiguous District Health Board regions in Aotearoa New Zealand, who had expected delivery dates between 25th April 2009 and 25th March 2010 [27]. The study’s cohort is broadly generalizable to current births statistics in Aotearoa New Zealand with respect to expected ethnicity, maternal age and parental socioeconomic status at birth [28]. A detailed description of the study’s design and recruitment can be found elsewhere (Morton et al., 2013, 2014). In brief, major data collection waves (DCWs) have included conducting computer assisted telephone and personal interviews to gather information longitudinally relating to six inter-connected domains of child development: health and wellbeing; cognitive and psychosocial; education; family and whānau (extended family); culture and identity; and neighbourhoods and societal context. Participants were included in the current study if complete information was obtained for the cognitive observations and child behavior data at the 4.5 and 8 year DCW (n = 3904).

Measures

Cognitive measures. To assess child cognitive functioning at 8 years, the NIH Toolbox® for Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function Cognition Battery (NIH Toolbox CB) was administered [29]. This instrument has been validated against existing gold-standard measures; normed in both English and Spanish languages with a sample of 4,859 participants (age ranges 3–85) (Weintraub et al., 30) and has been validated within the Aotearoa New Zealand context [31]. The NIH Toolbox CB version 7–17 years was delivered to our cohort using the NIH Toolbox iPad app. It comprised seven subtests: Picture Vocabulary Test; Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test; Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test; List Sorting Working Memory Test; Dimensional Change Card Sort Test; Picture Sequence Memory Test; Oral Reading Recognition Test. Raw scores were used to measure task performance in the present study. Outcomes were dichotomized into children who scored one standard deviation below the mean and all other children.
Measures of Behavioral Difficulties. The mother-reported Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [32] was used to measure behavioral difficulties when the cohort children were 4.5 and 8 years of age. This questionnaire measures four difficulties subscales (emotional symptoms, peer problems, hyperactivity-inattention and conduct problems) as well as a strengths-based prosocial behavior subscale. The standard parent-report SDQ was used at 4.5 and 8 years. Previous research with the Growing Up in New Zealand cohort has shown that the SDQ has good psychometric properties in preschool children [26, 33, 34].
In the current study, the total difficulties score was used, based on the emotional symptoms, peer problems, hyperactivity-inattention and conduct problems subscales with their 5 corresponding items accordingly. The total difficulties score (ranging from 0 to 40) was then converted into categorical variables by using the recommended four band categorisation based on population data, to divide the data into those who are close to average, slightly raised/slightly lowered scores, high/low and very high/very low scores [35]. It is important to note that the reference population data used to determine the categorisation is not based on an Aotearoa New Zealand population. However, the cut-offs used align with those used by the Ministry of Health when reporting on the New Zealand Health Survey [36]. In the current study, the SDQ total difficulties score was further dichotomized into ‘close to average and slightly raised difficulties’ and ‘high and very high’ difficulties [20, 32, 33, 37]. The choice to combine the close to average and slightly raised bands in the current study was due to our interest in the clinically significant high or ‘abnormal’ range, respectively, as per the original 3 band categorization. The high cut-off is typically used to screen for children with significant social and emotional challenges in the nationwide preschool health and development check in Aotearoa New Zealand, known as the B4 School Check [38].
Based on the dichotomized SDQ total difficulties score at 4.5 and 8 years, we derived a persistence/change variable with 4 possible categories: no difficulties (at either time point); improved (behavioral difficulties at 4.5 years but not at 8 years); later onset (no behavioral difficulties at 4.5 years but at 8 years) and persistent (behavioral difficulties at both time points).
Sociodemographic Covariates. We controlled for a range of variables that have been found to be related to both behavior and cognitive functioning in children [26, 3941]. Control variables specific to the child included birthweight, sex, and age in months when assessed at the 8-year DCW. Maternal variables that we controlled for were collected by self-report at the antenatal DCW: mother’s education (no secondary school, secondary school/diploma/trade certificate, Bachelor’s degree or higher); age (< 20 years, 20–29 years; ≥ 30 years); whether or not the pregnancy was planned. At the 4.5 year DCW, mothers reported on their children’s ethnicity by referring to a list of 32 possible answers as well as an open ended ‘Other, please specify’ category (multiple responses were collected). In the current study, Level 1 ethnicity categorisation was utilised, categorised into six categories by external prioritisation according to the Statistics New Zealand priorisation guidelines: Māori; Pacific Peoples; Asian; Middle Eastern, Latin American and African (MELAA); Other; European [42, 43]. We acknowledge that ethnicity is a complex multi-faceted construct which can be understood in different ways and may vary over time which takes careful considerations to capture in a longitudinal study [4446]. Further, area-level deprivation and rurality at 8 years was also controlled for in the analyses as these have been found to be linked to cognitive functioning in children [47, 48]. To determine SES, the NZDep2013 Index was used, which is an area-level measure using socioeconomic indicators from the 2013 NZ census [49]. Deprivation scores range from least deprived (decile 1) to most deprived (decile 10). In the current study, SES was categorised into high (deciles 8–10), medium (deciles 4–7) and low (deciles 1–3) deprivation. Rurality was defined using Statistics New Zealand’s classification for urban and rural areas [50]. We further controlled for time of testing (finish hour) of the cognitive assessment which might have varied across participants as it was part of a longer face-to-face interview procedure.

Data Analysis

Analyses were carried out using RStudio version 3.6.1 and IBM SPSS Statistics version 25.0. Statistical significance was given at an α level of p < .05.
To investigate how change or persistence from 4.5 to 8 years in behavioral difficulties total score is associated with cognitive performance at 8 years, a multivariate logistic regression analysis was conducted for each cognitive outcome (below average cognitive abilities vs. typical) while controlling for the aforementioned covariates.
Multicollinearity was assessed by calculating the generalised variance inflation factors (GVIF). Variables with VIF > 4, indicate a potential concern for multicollinearity [51].
Posthoc multiple comparisons between each level of the behavioral persistence/change variable were calculated using general linear hypothesis tests with a Tukey correction applied for multiple comparisons [52]. We also interpreted the effect sizes of significant odds ratios based on Cohen’s d effect sizes [53] and Chinn’s [54] method to convert odds ratios to effect size. Accordingly, effect size magnitudes as odds ratios are as follows: 1.47 = small effect; 2.47 = medium effect; and 4.25 = large effect.

Results

Descriptive statistics. When compared to the Growing Up in New Zealand baseline sample (n = 6853), cases with missing data for the current analysis were less likely to be European, more likely to live in a rural, highly deprived area and to have mothers of lower education while the pregnancy was less likely to be planned (p < .001). No differences were found with respect to children’s sex . The distribution of children within the SDQ 4 band categorisation at 4.5 and 8 years are presented in Table 1. The distribution of the total difficulties scores dichotomized into average/slightly raised vs. high/very high at 4.5 and 8 years can be found in Table 2. Table 3 presents the frequency distribution of behavioral difficulties (close to average/slightly raised and high/very high) and control variables across each cognitive measure (below average vs. typical) at age 8.
Table 1
Distribution of children within the SDQ 4 band categorisation at 8 years
  
4 band categorisation
 
Behavioral difficulties
 
Close to average
 
Slightly raised
 
High
 
Very high
  
n
%
 
n
%
 
n
%
 
n
%
Total difficulties 8 years
 
3423
87.7
 
235
6.0
 
143
3.7
 
103
2.6
Total difficulties 4.5 years
 
3183
81.5
 
401
10.3
 
187
4.8
 
133
3.4
Table 2
SDQ Total difficulties dichotomized at 8 years and 4.5 years
  
Total cohort
Behavioral difficulties
 
Close to average/
slightly raised
 
High/
Very high
  
  
n
%
 
n
%
  
Total difficulties 8 years
 
3658
93.7
 
246
6.3
  
Total difficulties 4.5 years
 
3584
91.8
 
320
8.2
  
Table 3
Frequency distribution of behavioral difficulties and control variables across cognitive measure at 8 years
 
Cognitive Outcome
 
Vocabulary
Inhibitory control
Working Memory
Cognitive Flexibility
Processing Speed
Episodic Memory
Reading
Variable
Average and above
1 SD below average
Average and above
1 SD below average
Average and above
1 SD below average
Average and above
1 SD below average
Average and above
1 SD below average
Average and above
1 SD below average
Average and above
1 SD below average
Total difficulties
              
Average/slightly raised
3238 (94.5)
420 (88.2)
3206 (94.6)
452 (87.9)
3246 (94.6)
412 (87.3)
3272 (94.1)
386 (90.2)
3133 (94.2)
525 (90.7)
3041 (94.5)
617 (89.9)
3200 (94.6)
458 (87.9)
High/very high
190 (5.5)
56 (11.8)
184 (5.4)
62 (12.1)
186 (5.4)
60 (12.7)
204 (5.9)
42 (9.8)
192 (5.8)
54 (9.3)
176 (5.5)
70 (10.2)
183 (5.4)
63 (12.1)
Emotional symptoms
              
Average/slightly raised
3172 (92.5)
428 (89.9)
3144 (92.7)
456 (88.7)
3182 (92.7)
418 (88.6)
3209 (92.3)
391 (91.4)
3074 (92.5)
526 (90.8)
2976 (92.5)
624 (90.8)
3133 (92.6)
467 (89.6)
High/very high
256 (7.5)
48 (10.1)
246 (7.3)
58 (11.3)
250 (7.3)
54 (11.4)
267 (7.7)
37 (8.6)
251 (7.5)
53 (9.2)
241 (7.5)
63 (9.2)
250 (7.4)
54 (10.4)
Peer problems
              
Average/slightly raised
3086 (90.0)
376 (79.0)
3026 (89.3)
436 (84.8)
3080 (89.7)
382 (80.9)
3108 (89.4)
354 (82.7)
2968 (89.3)
494 (85.3)
2880 (89.5)
582 (84.7)
3026 (89.4)
436 (83.7)
High/very high
342 (10.0)
100 (21.0)
364 (10.7)
78 (15.2)
352 (10.3)
90 (19.1)
368 (10.6)
74 (17.3)
357 (10.7)
85 (14.7)
337 (10.5)
105 (15.3)
357 (10.6)
85 (16.3)
Hyperactivity
              
Average/slightly raised
3271 (95.4)
448 (94.1)
3228 (95.2)
491 (95.5)
3287 (95.8)
432 (91.5)
3330 (95.8)
389 (90.9)
3172 (95.4)
547 (94.5)
3079 (95.7)
640 (93.2)
3246 (96.0)
473 (90.8)
High/very high
157 (4.6)
28 (5.9)
162 (4.8)
23 (4.5)
145 (4.2)
40 (8.5)
146 (4.2)
39 (9.1)
153 (4.6)
32 (5.5)
138 (4.3)
47 (6.8)
137 (4.0)
48 (9.2)
Conduct problems
              
Average/slightly raised
3168 (92.4)
409 (85.9)
3129 (92.3)
448 (87.2)
3169 (92.3)
408 (86.4)
3194 (91.9)
383 (89.5)
3057 (91.9)
520 (89.8)
2967 (92.2)
610 (88.8)
3124 (92.3)
453 (86.9)
High/very high
260 (7.6)
67 (14.1)
261 (7.7)
66 (12.8)
266 (7.7)
64 (13.6)
282 (8.1)
45 (10.5)
268 (8.1)
59 (10.2)
250 (7.8)
77 (11.2)
259 (7.7)
68 (13.1)
Ethnicity
              
European
1594 (46.5)
101 (21.2)
1475 (43.5)
220 (42.8)
1523 (44.4)
172 (36.4)
1533 (44.1)
162 (37.9)
1454 (43.7)
241 (41.6)
1391 (43.2)
304 (44.3)
1475 (43.6)
220 (42.2)
Māori
739 (21.6)
162 (34.0)
769 (22.7)
132 (25.7)
736 (22.2)
138 (29.2)
778 (22.4)
123 (28.7)
745 (22.4)
156 (29.6)
730 (22.7)
171 (24.9)
721 (21.3)
180 (34.5)
Pacific Peoples
267 (7.8)
99 (20.8)
317 (9.4)
49 (9.5)
308 (9.0)
58 (12.3)
317 (9.1)
49 (11.4)
308 (9.3)
58 (10.0)
293 (9.1)
73 (10.6)
317 (9.4)
49 (9.4)
Asian
418 (12.2)
86 (18.1)
440 (13.0)
64 (12.5)
428 (12.5)
76 (16.1)
443 (12.7)
61 (14.3)
442 (13.3)
62 (10.7)
430 (13.4)
74 (10.8)
469 (13.9)
35 (6.7)
Other incl. MELAA
410 (11.9)
28 (5.9)
389 (11.5)
49 (11.4)
410 (12)
28 (6.0)
405 (11.6)
33 (7.7)
376 (11.3)
62 (10.7)
373 (11.5)
65 (9.5)
401 (11.8)
37 (7.2)
Mother’s education
              
No secondary school
117 (3.5)
41 (8.8)
127 (3.8)
31 (6.2)
127 (3.8)
31 (6.8)
128 (3.8)
30 (7.2)
124 (3.8)
34 (6.0)
112 (3.6)
46 (6.9)
110 (3.3)
48 (9.5)
Secondary school/ Diploma/ Trade certificate
1588 (47.3)
298 (64.2)
1619 (48.7)
267 (53.6)
1609 (47.8)
277 (60.3)
1655 (48.6)
231 (55.3)
1579 (48.5)
307 (54.0)
1541 (48.9)
345 (51.5)
1595 (48.1)
291 (57.5)
Bachelor degree and higher
1653 (49.2)
125 (26.9)
1578 (47.5)
200 (40.2)
1627 (48.4)
151 (32.9)
1621 (47.6)
157 (37.6)
1551 (47.7)
227 (40.0)
1499 (47.6)
279 (41.6)
1611 (48.6)
167 (33.0)
Deprivation
              
Low
1310 (38.8)
102 (21.7)
1252 (37.4)
160 (32.1)
1275 (37.7)
137 (29.4)
1262 (36.8)
150 (35.7)
1230 (37.6)
182 (31.8)
1178 (37.1)
234 (34.8)
1302 (39.1)
110 (21.4)
Medium
1352 (40.0)
155 (32.9)
1317 (39.3)
190 (38.2)
1337 (39.5)
170 (36.5)
1369 (39.9)
138 (32.9)
1258 (38.4)
249 (43.5)
1268 (39.9)
239 (35.6)
1290 (38.7)
217 (42.3)
High
714 (21.1)
214 (45.4)
780 (23.3)
148 (29.7)
769 (22.7)
159 (34.1)
796 (23.3)
132 (31.4)
787 (24.0)
141 (24.7)
729 (23.0)
199 (29.6)
742 (22.3)
186 (36.3)
Rurality
              
Urban
2943 (87.2)
426 (90.4)
2926 (87.4)
443 (89.0)
2955 (87.4)
414 (88.8)
2993 (87.3)
376 (89.5)
2876 (87.8)
493 (86.2)
2783 (87.7)
586 (87.2)
2934 (88.0)
435 (84.8)
Rural
433 (12.8)
45 (9.6)
423 (12.5)
55 (11.0)
426 (12.6)
52 (11.2)
434 (12.4)
44 (10.5)
399 (12.2)
79 (13.8)
392 (12.3)
86 (12.8)
400 (12.0)
78 (15.2)
Mother’s age at birth
              
<20 years
88 (2.6)
30 (6.4)
104 (3.1)
14 (2.8)
97 (2.9)
21 (4.5)
100 (2.9)
18 (4.3)
105 (3.2)
13 (2.3)
95 (3.0)
23 (3.4)
98 (3.0)
20 (3.9)
…20–29 years
1086 (32.3)
217 (42.6)
1109 (33.3)
187 (37.5)
1120 (33.3)
176 (38.1)
1135 (33.3)
161 (38.3)
1088 (33.4)
208 (36.6)
1063 (33.7)
233 (34.6)
1079 (32.5)
217 (42.6)
>29 years
2187 (65.1)
228 (48.7)
2117 (63.6)
298 (59.7)
2150 (63.9)
265 (57.4)
2174 (63.8)
241 (57.4)
2067 (63.4)
348 (61.2)
1998 (63.3)
417 (62.0)
2143 (64.5)
272 (53.4)
Pregnancy planned
              
Yes
2336 (69.8)
253 (54.5)
2277 (68.7)
312 (63.0)
2288 (68.2)
301 (65.9)
2331 (68.6)
258 (62.3)
2203 (67.9)
386 (68.3)
2163 (68.8)
426 (64.0)
2301 (69.5)
288 (57.5)
No
1011 (30.2)
211 (45.5)
1039 (31.3)
183 (37.0)
1066 (31.8)
156 (34.1)
1066 (31.4)
156 (37.7)
1043 (32.1)
179 (31.7)
982 (31.2)
240 (36.0)
1009 (30.5)
213 (42.5)
Child’s sex
              
Boy
1679 (49.0)
284 (59.7)
1710 (50.5)
253 (49.2)
1723 (50.2)
240 (50.8)
1709 (49.2)
254 (59.3)
1639 (49.3)
324 (56.0)
1603 (49.9)
360 (52.4)
1675 (49.5)
288 (55.4)
Girl
1747 (51.0)
192 (40.3)
1678 (49.5)
261 (50.8)
1707 (49.8)
232 (49.2)
1765 (50.8)
174 (40.7)
1684 (50.7)
255 (44.0)
1612 (50.1)
327 (47.6)
1707 (50.5)
232 (44.63)
Over time, 3417 (87.5%) of children had no difficulties at any time point, 241 (6.2%) children improved, 167 (4.3%) had a later onset of behavioral difficulties at 8 years and 79 (2.0%) showed persistent difficulties at both time points.
Associations Between Persistence and Change in Behavioral Difficulties from 4.5 to 8 years and cognitive outcomes at 8 years. The results of the multivariate logistic regression analyses can be found in Table 4. Compared to children with no difficulties, children with a later onset of behavioral difficulties showed an increased likelihood of below average vocabulary (OR = 1.73, p < .05), inhibitory control/attention (OR = 2.36, p < 001), working memory (OR = 2.14, p < .001), processing speed (OR = 1.76, p < .05), episodic memory (OR = 1.79, p < .01) and reading (OR = 1.75, p < .05). Furthermore, compared to children with no difficulties, children who had persistent difficulties at 4.5 and 8 years had increased odds of below average inhibitory control/attention (OR = 2.44, p < .01), working memory (OR = 2.29, p < .05), episodic memory (OR = 1.91, p < .05) and reading (OR = 2.12, p < 05) at 8 years. Additionally, children who had a later onset of behavioral difficulties showed increased odds of below average working memory compared to children who had improved (OR = 2.05, p < 05). While the effects were generally small in magnitude, the effects were strongest for the association between persistent behavioral difficulties and cognitive outcomes. Multicollinearity was not of concern (GVIFs ≤ 1.41).
Table 4
Multivariate logistic regression evaluating the association between persistence and change in behavioral difficulties and cognitive outcomes at 8 years (post-hoc multiple comparison)
Cognitive Outcome (1SD below mean vs average and above)
Behavioral difficulties
Vocabulary
Inhibitory control
Working Memory
Cognitive Flexibility
Processing Speed
 
Episodic Memory
Reading
 
B (SE)
OR
(99% CI)
z value
B (SE)
OR
(99% CI)
z value
B (SE)
OR
(99% CI)
z value
B (SE)
OR
(99% CI)
z value
B (SE)
OR
(99% CI)
z value
B (SE)
OR
(99% CI)
z value
B (SE)
OR
(99% CI)
z value
 
Improved vs no difficulties
0.35 (0.18)
1.42
0.89–2.28)
1.93
0.33
(0.20)
1.39
(0.83–2.30)
1.65
0.04
(0.21)
1.04
(0.61–1.79)
0.21
0.16
(0.21)
1.17
(0.68–2.01)
0.75
0.30
(0.19)
1.35
(0.83–2.19)
1.61
0.054
(0.18)
1.05
(0.65–1.68)
0.25
0.37
(0.20)
1.45
(0.87–2.41)
1.89
 
Later onset vs no difficulties
0.55 (0.21)
1.73
(1.00–3.00)
2.56*
0.86
(0.19)
2.36
(1.44–3.88)
4.46***
0.76
(0.20)
2.14
(1.28–3.60)
3.80***
0.46
(0.22)
1.58
(0.89–2.78)
2.07
0.56
(0.19)
1.76
(1.07–2.90)
2.91*
0.58
(0.18)
1.79
(1.11–2.87)
3.16**
0.56
(0.20)
1.75
(1.03–2.97)
2.73*
 
Persistent vs no difficulties
0.52
(0.28)
1.69
(0.81–3.51)
1.85
0.89
(0.28)
2.44
(1.20–4.96)
3.24**
0.83
(0.28)
2.29
(1.12–4.69)
3.00*
0.22
(0.33)
1.25
(0.54–2.91)
0.68
0.17
(0.31)
1.19
(0.54–2.63)
0.56
0.65
(0.26)
1.91
(0.97–3.74)
2.45*
0.75
(0.28)
2.12
(1.04–4.35)
2.71*
 
Later onset vs improved
0.20
(0.27)
1.22
(0.61–2.05)
0.73
0.53
(0.26)
1.70
(0.87–3.35)
2.03
0.72
(0.28)
2.05
(1.00-4.20)
2.60*
0.30
(0.29)
1.35
(0.64–2.85)
1.02
0.26
(0.26)
1.30
(0.67–2.53)
1.01
0.53
(0.24)
1.71
(0.90–3.25)
2.14
0.19
(0.27)
1.21
(0.60–2.42)
0.69
 
Persistent vs improved
0.17
(0.32)
1.19
(0.52–2.73)
0.53
0.56
(0.32)
1.76
(0.76–4.05)
1.75
0.79
(0.33)
2.20
(0.93–5.17)
2.37
0.06
(0.38)
1.07
(0.41–2.80)
0.17
-0.13
(0.35)
0.88
(0.36–2.16)
-0.37
0.60
(0.31)
1.82
(0.83–4.02)
1.96
0.38
(0.33)
1.46
(0.63–3.38)
1.17
 
Persistent vs later onset
-0.02
(0.34)
0.98
(0.40–2.37)
-0.07
0.03
(0.32)
1.03
(0.45–2.38)
0.10
0.07
(0.33)
1.07
(0.46–2.50)
0.20
-0.23
(0.38)
0.79
(0.29–2.13)
-0.61
-0.39
(0.36)
0.68
(0.27–1.69)
-1.10
0.06
(0.31)
1.07
(0.48–2.37)
0.21
0.19
(0.33)
1.21
(0.51–2.87)
0.58
 
Note.*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. Behavioral difficulties: High/very high compared to average/slightly raised; Low/very low compared to average/slightly lowered. All analyses controlled for mother’s ethnicity, mother’s education, mother’s age when pregnant, area-level deprivation, rurality, child’s age, child’s gender, planned pregnancy, time of testing. OR: 1.47 = small effect; 2.47 = medium effect; 4.25 = large effect

Discussion

In the current study, we investigated the association between persistence and change in behavioral difficulties and cognitive performance in 8-year-old children, enrolled in the Growing Up in New Zealand study. Our findings are novel and important as we looked at behavior stability over time when transitioning from preschool age (4.5 years) to school age (8 years) and cognitive outcomes were assessed using a comprehensive standardized cognitive battery including measures of language, executive functioning, episodic memory, working memory and processing speed.
Compared to children with no difficulties, children who had persistent difficulties (behavioral difficulties both at 4.5 and 8 years) or who had a later onset (showing behavioral difficulties at 8 years only) performed more poorly across a range of cognitive outcomes at 8 years. Specifically, children who had a later onset of behavioral difficulties compared to children with no difficulties were more likely to perform below average in tests of inhibitory control/attention, working memory, processing speed, episodic memory, vocabulary and reading. Children who had persistent behavioral difficulties were at higher risk for below average cognitive abilities in the areas of inhibitory control/attention, working memory, reading and episodic memory. Additionally, children who had a later onset of behavioral difficulties had greater odds of below average working memory performance compared to children who had improved. The effect of these associations appeared to be strongest for inhibitory control/attention, followed by working memory which also showed the most associations with behavioural difficulties. No significant associations between behavioral difficulties and cognitive outcomes were found for cognitive flexibility. There was also no significant difference in children whose behavioral difficulties improved compared to children with no difficulties.
Our study is an extension of an earlier longitudinal study with the same cohort studied in early childhood. Consistent with our findings, the earlier work demonstrated that only those children with later onset or persistent behavioral difficulties had an increased likelihood of showing preschool cognitive delay (i.e. executive control, receptive language and early literacy; [20]). Here, when transitioning from preschool to age 8, we observed that later onset and persistent behavioral difficulties were associated with the likelihood of underperformance in a broader range of cognitive abilities. No association was found for children whose behavioral problems improved from age 4.5 to age 8 years.
Interestingly, we found more statistically significant associations between cognitive outcomes and later onset of behavioral difficulties than between cognitive outcomes and persistent behavioral difficulties. One explanation might be that the incidence of later behavioral difficulties may have been experienced as unsettling and thus affecting performance in several cognitive areas. For instance, newly occurring behavioral problems might have been associated with major life events or disruptions in the children’s lives which may have affected a broader range of cognitive abilities directly or indirectly [55, 56]. Likewise, it is possible that current difficulties in academic success might have impacted on current behavioral patterns as this relationship is likely to be bidirectional. In this regard, it appears also plausible that the concurrent nature of the behavioral problems is more relevant as it has a more direct impact on the NIH Toolbox CB performance. Another possibility is that the group of children with later-onset of behavioral difficulties may be acting out because their cognitive difficulties, along with school becoming more demanding during middle childhood, manifesting behavioral problems in order to compensate for cognitive obstacles and poorer grades [57].
At the same time, we found that within the same cognitive outcome effect sizes were slightly greater for children who had persistent behavioral difficulties compared with children who showed later onset of difficulties. An earlier study using the Growing Up in New Zealand data investigated the persistence and change in clinically relevant behavioral problems during early childhood and observed that those whose difficulties persist are also more likely to experience risk factors for vulnerability [26]. This is in line with a secondary analysis of data from the UK Millennium Cohort Study and the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children with 2- to 3-year-olds, which showed that higher rates of behavioral difficulties among children with developmental delay may be partially due to a greater likelihood of exposure to adverse socio-economic conditions [58]. Hence, persistent behavioral difficulties after transition to school are likely to be accompanied by additional challenges in the children’s lives. This supports D’Souza and colleague’s [20] assumption that ongoing behavioral problems may have a greater impact on children’s cognitive performance.
It should be noted that we are not using clinical markers of cognitive impairment. We have simply identified the lowest end of the normal distribution of cognitive outcomes of the NIH Toolbox CB. This dichotomisation was chosen rather than the continuous cognitive outcomes as we were interested how behavioral difficulties relate to the likelihood of poorer cognitive functioning as in contrast to cognitive performance per se. The classification of neuropsychological impairment is dependent on the normative comparison applied and there is no normative data available for the Aotearoa New Zealand population [31]. In a study to develop demographically corrected normative standards for the English Version of the NIH Toolbox CB within a U.S. sample, cut-points were calculated one standard deviation below the mean as an operational definition of “impairment” across the fully corrected composites to increase clinical interpretation in a sample of children (3–17 years) [59]. Likewise, we used the one standard deviation below the mean threshold to indicate poorer cognitive functioning in our sample.
Our results provide a deeper insight into the association between persistence and change in behavioral difficulties when transitioning from preschool into school with specific cognitive abilities at age 8. Specifically, we found that that persistent and later onset of behavioral difficulties was associated with a range of cognitive abilities. Our results, alongside the recent findings of D’Souza and colleagues [20], extend the existing literature by demonstrating that both persistent and later onset of behavioral problems are associated with below average cognitive performance in contrast to children who showed no or improved difficulties. However, we cannot account for causation as the relationship between cognitive difficulties and behavioral problems is likely to be bidirectional in nature [4, 15, 16] and may be mediated by other underlying factors such as genetic or environmental factors which affect the manifestation of both cognitive and behavioral difficulties [6062].
Our findings have practical implications, as the SDQ is used in the New Zealand Health Survey and the B4 School Check and has been widely administered to screen for psychopathology in early childhood both internationally and in the Aotearoa New Zealand population [63, 64]. There has been increasing awareness regarding the social-emotional aspects of learning, indicating that young people must be socially and emotionally ready to learn in order to be able to benefit from the educational curriculum [65, 66]. Our study demonstrates that children with persistent and later onset behavioral difficulties are more likely to perform more poorly in several cognitive key outcomes which may potentially impact their future academic success. Given, we cannot account for any causal direction, there is a likely co-occurrence of challenges, with children who are struggling with the educational course also having to face behavioral difficulties. This highlights the importance of identifying and addressing behavioral problems alongside cognitive and educational difficulties to provide adequate support and resources for those requiring additional assistance [66].
Our study has several limitations. First, while one could infer that there may be some effect on behavioral difficulties when transitioning from pre-school to formal schooling, we did not directly measure this but captured this as a result of to the timing of measurement at preschool age and age 8 years. Consequently, many other factors could also have influenced the cognitive outcomes. In this regard, a methodological limitation is the lack of a baseline assessment of cognitive abilities due to the retrospective nature of this study. Thus, changes or stability in cognitive abilities over time as well as other factors not accounted for in this study such as the family environment and parenting factors may also partially accounted for associations between the children’s cognitive and behavioral outcomes [67, 68]. As the Growing Up in New Zealand study is an observational population-based study, some children may have naturally been in treatment for behavioral or cognitive problems which may have influenced the findings over time. Third, literature generally acknowledges that cognitive tests may be culturally biased with respect to their content and administration procedure [69, 70]. Fluid constructs, like attention/inhibitory control, might be less culturally biased than language tests but performance in these may also be dependent on the children’s ability to understand the instructions. In this regard, a proportion of the children in our sample are bilingual, with some having another language as English as their primary language which might have affected the cognitive test results. Additionally, the NIH Toolbox CB may not have been feasible for all children or suitable for those with special needs or requiring assistance. There may be some bias due to method effects, i.e. as the cognitive assessment was administered at the end of a longer interview after school, this may have caused an additional challenge for those with behavioral difficulties, especially for children with high hyperactivity. Furthermore, there may be bias when assessing behavior solely by mother-report as opposed to including father- and teacher-report or direct observation. [71] As cases with missing data showed a different socio-demographic distribution compared to the baseline sample, the overall generalizability is limited.
In conclusion, this study accounts for the longitudinal perspective of behavioral problems during the important stage of transitioning from preschool to school age and how persistence and change are linked to different cognitive areas at age 8 assessed with a comprehensive standardized cognitive battery. We found that children who showed persistent or later onset of behavioral difficulties were more likely to show below average performance across language, executive functioning, episodic memory, working memory and processing speed compared with children who had no difficulties across time. As cognitive performance is closely related to academic achievement, our study supports the importance of identifying and addressing needs in both cognitive skills and behavioral aspects when planning interventions and educational programmes in early and middle childhood.
Summary
The current study investigated the association between persistence and change in behavioral difficulties during early to middle childhood and a range of cognitive outcomes at age 8. Our sample comprised 3904 8-year-old children enrolled in the longitudinal GrowingUp in New Zealand study. The NIH Toolbox CB was used to assess cognitive outcomes at 8 years including vocabulary, language, inhibitory control/attention, processing speed, cognitive flexibility, working memory and episodic memory. The parent administered SDQ was used to assess behavioral difficulties at 4.5 and 8 years. Multivariate logistic regression analyses were conducted with cognitive measures as outcomes and persistence/change in the SDQ Total difficulties score as predictors while controlling for a range of sociodemographic confounders. Our findings show that children with persistent or later onset of behavioral difficulties were at higher risk for poorer vocabulary, reading, inhibitory control/attention, episodic memory, working memory and processing speed at age 8 compared to children with no difficulties or improved difficulties. Even though we cannot account for causation and other factors that might have influenced cognitive performance, there is a likely co-occurrence of behavioral and cognitive challenges. Thus, our study highlights the importance of identifying and addressing both behavioral problems as well as cognitive and educational difficulties to provide adequate support and resources when planning educational programmes and interventions in early and middle childhood.

Acknowledgements

Growing Up in New Zealand has been funded by the New Zealand Ministries of Social Development, Health, Education, Justice and the former Pacific Island Affairs (now the Ministry of Pacific Peoples); the former Ministry of Science Innovation and the former Department of Labour (now both part of the Ministry of Business, Innovation and Employment); the former Ministry of Women’s Affairs (now the Ministry for Women); the Department of Corrections; the former Families Commission (later known as the Social Policy Evaluation and Research Unit and now disestablished); Te Puni Kokiri; New Zealand Police; Sport New Zealand; the Housing New Zealand Corporation; and the former Mental Health Commission, The University of Auckland and Auckland UniServices Limited. Other support for the study has been provided by the NZ Health Research Council, Statistics New Zealand, the Office of the Children’s Commissioner and the Office of Ethnic Affairs. The study has been designed and conducted by the Growing Up in New Zealand study team, led by the University of Auckland. The authors acknowledge the contributions of the original study investigators: Susan M.B. Morton, Polly E. Atatoa Carr, Cameron C. Grant, Arier C. Lee, Dinusha K. Bandara, Jatender Mohal, Jennifer M. Kinloch, Johanna M. Schmidt, Mary R. Hedges, Vivienne C. Ivory, Te Kani R. Kingi, Renee Liang, Lana M. Perese, Elizabeth Peterson, Jan E. Pryor, Elaine Reese, Elizabeth M. Robinson, Karen E. Waldie, Clare R. Wall. The views reported in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Growing Up in New Zealand Investigators.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declared no potential conflict of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article. The author(s) have indicated that they have no financial relationships to disclose that are relevant to this article.

Ethical Approval

The Growing Up in New Zealand study had ethical approval of the Ministry of Health Northern Y Regional Ethics Committee in New Zealand (NTY/08/06/055) and each data collection wave has been approved by the Health and Disability Ethics Committee. All procedures using human subjects were conducted in accordance with the standards of the University of Auckland, the Regional District Health Board and the Health and Disability Ethics Committee. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Written informed consent was obtained all individual participants included in this study.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Rutter M (2002) Development and psychopathology. In: Rutter M, Taylor E (eds) Child and adolescent psychiatry. Osney Mead. Blackwell Science, Oxford Rutter M (2002) Development and psychopathology. In: Rutter M, Taylor E (eds) Child and adolescent psychiatry. Osney Mead. Blackwell Science, Oxford
2.
go back to reference Beitchman JH, Wilson B, Johnson CJ, Atkinson L, Young A, Adlaf E et al (2001) Fourteen-year follow-up of speech/language-impaired and control children: Psychiatric outcome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 40(1):75–82PubMedCrossRef Beitchman JH, Wilson B, Johnson CJ, Atkinson L, Young A, Adlaf E et al (2001) Fourteen-year follow-up of speech/language-impaired and control children: Psychiatric outcome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 40(1):75–82PubMedCrossRef
3.
go back to reference Benasich AA, Curtiss S, Tallal P (1993) Language, learning, and behavioral disturbances in childhood: a longitudinal perspective. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 32(3):585–594PubMedCrossRef Benasich AA, Curtiss S, Tallal P (1993) Language, learning, and behavioral disturbances in childhood: a longitudinal perspective. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 32(3):585–594PubMedCrossRef
4.
go back to reference Helland WA, Lundervold AJ, Heimann M, Posserud M-B (2014) Stable associations between behavioral problems and language impairments across childhood–The importance of pragmatic language problems. Res Dev Disabil 35(5):943–951PubMedCrossRef Helland WA, Lundervold AJ, Heimann M, Posserud M-B (2014) Stable associations between behavioral problems and language impairments across childhood–The importance of pragmatic language problems. Res Dev Disabil 35(5):943–951PubMedCrossRef
5.
go back to reference Gremillion ML, Martel MM (2014) Merely misunderstood? Receptive, expressive, and pragmatic language in young children with disruptive behavior disorders. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 43(5):765–776PubMedCrossRef Gremillion ML, Martel MM (2014) Merely misunderstood? Receptive, expressive, and pragmatic language in young children with disruptive behavior disorders. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 43(5):765–776PubMedCrossRef
6.
go back to reference Sim F, O’Dowd J, Thompson L, Law J, Macmillan S, Affleck M et al (2013) Language and social/emotional problems identified at a universal developmental assessment at 30 months. BMC Pediatr 13(1):206PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sim F, O’Dowd J, Thompson L, Law J, Macmillan S, Affleck M et al (2013) Language and social/emotional problems identified at a universal developmental assessment at 30 months. BMC Pediatr 13(1):206PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
7.
go back to reference Mackie L, Law J (2010) Pragmatic language and the child with emotional/behavioural difficulties (EBD): a pilot study exploring the interaction between behaviour and communication disability. Int J Lang communication disorders 45(4):397–410CrossRef Mackie L, Law J (2010) Pragmatic language and the child with emotional/behavioural difficulties (EBD): a pilot study exploring the interaction between behaviour and communication disability. Int J Lang communication disorders 45(4):397–410CrossRef
8.
go back to reference Achenbach TM, Rescorla L (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment Aseba., Burlington, VT Achenbach TM, Rescorla L (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment Aseba., Burlington, VT
9.
go back to reference Raaijmakers MA, Smidts DP, Sergeant JA, Maassen GH, Posthumus JA, Van Engeland H et al (2008) Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36(7):1097PubMedCrossRef Raaijmakers MA, Smidts DP, Sergeant JA, Maassen GH, Posthumus JA, Van Engeland H et al (2008) Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36(7):1097PubMedCrossRef
10.
go back to reference Jahromi LB, Stifter CA(2008) Individual differences in preschoolers’ self-regulation and theory of mind.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly:125–150 Jahromi LB, Stifter CA(2008) Individual differences in preschoolers’ self-regulation and theory of mind.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly:125–150
11.
go back to reference Adams JW, Snowling MJ (2001) Executive function and reading impairments in children reported by their teachers as ‘hyperactive’. Br J Dev Psychol 19(2):293–306CrossRef Adams JW, Snowling MJ (2001) Executive function and reading impairments in children reported by their teachers as ‘hyperactive’. Br J Dev Psychol 19(2):293–306CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Adams JW, Snowling MJ, Hennessy SM, Kind P (1999) Problems of behaviour, reading and arithmetic: Assessments of comorbidity using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Br J Educ Psychol 69(4):571–585PubMedCrossRef Adams JW, Snowling MJ, Hennessy SM, Kind P (1999) Problems of behaviour, reading and arithmetic: Assessments of comorbidity using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Br J Educ Psychol 69(4):571–585PubMedCrossRef
13.
go back to reference Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lynskey MT (1993) The effects of conduct disorder and attention deficit in middle childhood on offending and scholastic ability at age 13. J Child Psychol 34(6):899–916CrossRef Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lynskey MT (1993) The effects of conduct disorder and attention deficit in middle childhood on offending and scholastic ability at age 13. J Child Psychol 34(6):899–916CrossRef
14.
go back to reference McGee R, Prior M, Williams S, Smart D, Sanson A (2002) The long-term significance of teacher‐rated hyperactivity and reading ability in childhood: Findings from two longitudinal studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43(8):1004–1017PubMedCrossRef McGee R, Prior M, Williams S, Smart D, Sanson A (2002) The long-term significance of teacher‐rated hyperactivity and reading ability in childhood: Findings from two longitudinal studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43(8):1004–1017PubMedCrossRef
15.
go back to reference Gallagher TM (1999) Interrelationships among children’s language, behavior, and emotional problems. Top Lang disorders 19(2):1–15CrossRef Gallagher TM (1999) Interrelationships among children’s language, behavior, and emotional problems. Top Lang disorders 19(2):1–15CrossRef
16.
go back to reference Redmond SM, Rice ML (1998) The socioemotional behaviors of children with SLI: Social adaptation or social deviance? J Speech Lang Hear Res 41(3):688–700PubMedCrossRef Redmond SM, Rice ML (1998) The socioemotional behaviors of children with SLI: Social adaptation or social deviance? J Speech Lang Hear Res 41(3):688–700PubMedCrossRef
17.
go back to reference DeBaryshe BD, Patterson GR, Capaldi DM (1993) A performance model for academic achievement in early adolescent boys. Dev Psychol 29(5):795CrossRef DeBaryshe BD, Patterson GR, Capaldi DM (1993) A performance model for academic achievement in early adolescent boys. Dev Psychol 29(5):795CrossRef
18.
go back to reference Ketelaars MP, Cuperus J, Jansonius K, Verhoeven L (2010) Pragmatic language impairment and associated behavioural problems. Int J Lang Commun Disord 45(2):204–214PubMedCrossRef Ketelaars MP, Cuperus J, Jansonius K, Verhoeven L (2010) Pragmatic language impairment and associated behavioural problems. Int J Lang Commun Disord 45(2):204–214PubMedCrossRef
19.
go back to reference Williams S, McGee R (1994) Reading attainment and juvenile delinquency. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 35(3):441–459PubMedCrossRef Williams S, McGee R (1994) Reading attainment and juvenile delinquency. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 35(3):441–459PubMedCrossRef
20.
go back to reference D’Souza S, Underwood L, Peterson ER, Morton SM, Waldie KE(2020) The Association Between Persistence and Change in Early Childhood Behavioural Problems and Preschool Cognitive Outcomes.Child Psychiatry Human Development:1–11 D’Souza S, Underwood L, Peterson ER, Morton SM, Waldie KE(2020) The Association Between Persistence and Change in Early Childhood Behavioural Problems and Preschool Cognitive Outcomes.Child Psychiatry Human Development:1–11
21.
go back to reference Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J, Wallner-Allen K, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S (2013) II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 78(4):16–33PubMedCrossRef Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J, Wallner-Allen K, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S (2013) II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 78(4):16–33PubMedCrossRef
22.
go back to reference Vandenbroucke L, Verschueren K, Baeyens D (2017) The development of executive functioning across the transition to first grade and its predictive value for academic achievement. Learn Instruction 49:103–112CrossRef Vandenbroucke L, Verschueren K, Baeyens D (2017) The development of executive functioning across the transition to first grade and its predictive value for academic achievement. Learn Instruction 49:103–112CrossRef
23.
go back to reference Hartas D (2011) Children’s language and behavioural, social and emotional difficulties and prosocial behaviour during the toddler years and at school entry. Br J Special Educ 38(2):83–91CrossRef Hartas D (2011) Children’s language and behavioural, social and emotional difficulties and prosocial behaviour during the toddler years and at school entry. Br J Special Educ 38(2):83–91CrossRef
24.
go back to reference Rose SL, Rose SA, Feldman JF (1989) Stability of behavior problems in very young children. Dev Psychopathol 1(1):5–19CrossRef Rose SL, Rose SA, Feldman JF (1989) Stability of behavior problems in very young children. Dev Psychopathol 1(1):5–19CrossRef
25.
go back to reference Keenan K, Wakschlag L (2000) More than the terrible twos: The nature and severity of behavior problems in clinic-referred preschool children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 28(1):33–46PubMedCrossRef Keenan K, Wakschlag L (2000) More than the terrible twos: The nature and severity of behavior problems in clinic-referred preschool children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 28(1):33–46PubMedCrossRef
26.
go back to reference D’Souza S, Underwood L, Peterson ER, Morton SM, Waldie KE (2019) Persistence and change in behavioural problems during early childhood. BMC Pediatr 19(1):259PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef D’Souza S, Underwood L, Peterson ER, Morton SM, Waldie KE (2019) Persistence and change in behavioural problems during early childhood. BMC Pediatr 19(1):259PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
go back to reference Morton SM, Grant CC, Atatoa Carr PE, Robinson EM, Kinloch JM, Fleming CJ et al (2014) How do you recruit and retain a prebirth cohort? Lessons learnt from Growing Up in New Zealand. Eval Health Prof 37(4):411–433PubMedCrossRef Morton SM, Grant CC, Atatoa Carr PE, Robinson EM, Kinloch JM, Fleming CJ et al (2014) How do you recruit and retain a prebirth cohort? Lessons learnt from Growing Up in New Zealand. Eval Health Prof 37(4):411–433PubMedCrossRef
28.
go back to reference Morton SM, Ramke J, Kinloch J, Grant CC, Atatoa Carr PE, Leeson H et al (2015) Growing Up in New Zealand cohort alignment with all New Zealand births. Aust N Z J Public Health 39(1):82–87PubMedCrossRef Morton SM, Ramke J, Kinloch J, Grant CC, Atatoa Carr PE, Leeson H et al (2015) Growing Up in New Zealand cohort alignment with all New Zealand births. Aust N Z J Public Health 39(1):82–87PubMedCrossRef
29.
go back to reference Gershon RC, Cella D, Fox NA, Havlik RJ, Hendrie HC, Wagster MV (2010) Assessment of neurological and behavioural function: the NIH Toolbox. Lancet Neurol 9(2):138–139PubMedCrossRef Gershon RC, Cella D, Fox NA, Havlik RJ, Hendrie HC, Wagster MV (2010) Assessment of neurological and behavioural function: the NIH Toolbox. Lancet Neurol 9(2):138–139PubMedCrossRef
30.
go back to reference Weintraub S, Bauer PJ, Zelazo PD, Wallner-Allen K, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK et al (2013) I. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): introduction and pediatric data. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 78(4):1–15PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Weintraub S, Bauer PJ, Zelazo PD, Wallner-Allen K, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK et al (2013) I. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): introduction and pediatric data. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 78(4):1–15PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
31.
go back to reference Neumann D, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SMB, Waldie KE(2021) Exploring the factor structure of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery in a large sample of 8-year old children in Aotearoa New Zealand.J Int Neuropsychol Soc:1–10 Neumann D, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SMB, Waldie KE(2021) Exploring the factor structure of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery in a large sample of 8-year old children in Aotearoa New Zealand.J Int Neuropsychol Soc:1–10
32.
go back to reference Goodman R (1997) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38(5):581–586PubMedCrossRef Goodman R (1997) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38(5):581–586PubMedCrossRef
33.
go back to reference D’Souza S, Waldie KE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM (2017) Psychometric properties and normative data for the preschool strengths and difficulties questionnaire in two-year-old children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 45(2):345–357PubMedCrossRef D’Souza S, Waldie KE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM (2017) Psychometric properties and normative data for the preschool strengths and difficulties questionnaire in two-year-old children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 45(2):345–357PubMedCrossRef
34.
go back to reference D’Souza S, Waldie KE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM (2019) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: factor structure of the father-report and parent agreement in 2-year-old children. Assessment 26(6):1059–1069PubMedCrossRef D’Souza S, Waldie KE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM (2019) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: factor structure of the father-report and parent agreement in 2-year-old children. Assessment 26(6):1059–1069PubMedCrossRef
35.
36.
go back to reference Ministry of Health (2018) Social, Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties in New Zealand Children: Technical Report. Wellington: Ministry of Health Ministry of Health (2018) Social, Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties in New Zealand Children: Technical Report. Wellington: Ministry of Health
37.
go back to reference D’Souza S, Crawford CN, Buckley J, Underwood L, Peterson ER, Bird A et al (2019) Antenatal determinants of early childhood talking delay and behavioural difficulties. Infant Behav Dev 57:101388PubMedCrossRef D’Souza S, Crawford CN, Buckley J, Underwood L, Peterson ER, Bird A et al (2019) Antenatal determinants of early childhood talking delay and behavioural difficulties. Infant Behav Dev 57:101388PubMedCrossRef
39.
go back to reference D’Souza S, Waldie KE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM (2019) Antenatal and postnatal determinants of behavioural difficulties in early childhood: Evidence from growing up in New Zealand. Child Psychiatry Human Development 50(1):45–60PubMedCrossRef D’Souza S, Waldie KE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM (2019) Antenatal and postnatal determinants of behavioural difficulties in early childhood: Evidence from growing up in New Zealand. Child Psychiatry Human Development 50(1):45–60PubMedCrossRef
40.
go back to reference Buckley J, Peterson ER, Underwood L, D’Souza S, Morton S, Waldie KEJL et al(2020) Socio-demographic and maternal health indicators of inhibitory control in preschool age children: evidence from Growing Up in New Zealand. 11(2):181–201 Buckley J, Peterson ER, Underwood L, D’Souza S, Morton S, Waldie KEJL et al(2020) Socio-demographic and maternal health indicators of inhibitory control in preschool age children: evidence from Growing Up in New Zealand. 11(2):181–201
41.
go back to reference Neumann D, Herbert SE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM, Waldie KE (2019) A longitudinal study of antenatal and perinatal risk factors in early childhood cognition: Evidence from Growing Up in New Zealand. Early Hum Dev 132:45–51PubMedCrossRef Neumann D, Herbert SE, Peterson ER, Underwood L, Morton SM, Waldie KE (2019) A longitudinal study of antenatal and perinatal risk factors in early childhood cognition: Evidence from Growing Up in New Zealand. Early Hum Dev 132:45–51PubMedCrossRef
42.
go back to reference Statistics New Zealand (2004) Report of the Review of the Measurement of Ethnicity Statistics New Zealand, Wellington Statistics New Zealand (2004) Report of the Review of the Measurement of Ethnicity Statistics New Zealand, Wellington
43.
go back to reference Statistics New Zealand (2005) Statistical Standard for Ethnicity Statistics New Zealand, Wellington Statistics New Zealand (2005) Statistical Standard for Ethnicity Statistics New Zealand, Wellington
44.
go back to reference Atatoa Carr P, Langridge F, Neumann D, Paine S-J, Liang R, Taufa S et al(2022) ‘Seeing’our tamariki in longitudinal studies: exploring the complexity of ethnic identification trajectories within Growing Up in New Zealand.Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand:1–17 Atatoa Carr P, Langridge F, Neumann D, Paine S-J, Liang R, Taufa S et al(2022) ‘Seeing’our tamariki in longitudinal studies: exploring the complexity of ethnic identification trajectories within Growing Up in New Zealand.Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand:1–17
45.
go back to reference Yao ES, Meissel K, Bullen P, Carr PA, Clark TC, Morton SM (2021) Classifying multiple ethnic identifications. Demographic Res 44:481–512CrossRef Yao ES, Meissel K, Bullen P, Carr PA, Clark TC, Morton SM (2021) Classifying multiple ethnic identifications. Demographic Res 44:481–512CrossRef
46.
go back to reference Yao ES, Meissel K, Bullen P, Clark TC, Carr PA, Tiatia-Seath J et al (2022) Demographic discrepancies between administrative-prioritisation and self-prioritisation of multiple ethnic identifications. Soc Sci Res 103:102648PubMedCrossRef Yao ES, Meissel K, Bullen P, Clark TC, Carr PA, Tiatia-Seath J et al (2022) Demographic discrepancies between administrative-prioritisation and self-prioritisation of multiple ethnic identifications. Soc Sci Res 103:102648PubMedCrossRef
47.
go back to reference Willingham DT (2012) Ask the Cognitive Scientist: Why Does Family Wealth Affect Learning? Am Educ 36(1):33–39 Willingham DT (2012) Ask the Cognitive Scientist: Why Does Family Wealth Affect Learning? Am Educ 36(1):33–39
48.
go back to reference Bradley RH, Corwyn RF (2002) Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol 53(1):371–399PubMedCrossRef Bradley RH, Corwyn RF (2002) Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol 53(1):371–399PubMedCrossRef
49.
go back to reference Atkinson J, Salmond C, Crampton P (2014) NZDep2013 index of deprivation. Department of Public Health, University of Otago, Wellington Atkinson J, Salmond C, Crampton P (2014) NZDep2013 index of deprivation. Department of Public Health, University of Otago, Wellington
50.
go back to reference Statistics New Zealand (2004) An urban/rural profile Statistics New Zealand, Wellington Statistics New Zealand (2004) An urban/rural profile Statistics New Zealand, Wellington
51.
go back to reference Glantz SA, Slinker BK, Neilands TB (1990) Primer of applied regression and analysis of variance McGraw-Hill. Health Profession Division, New York Glantz SA, Slinker BK, Neilands TB (1990) Primer of applied regression and analysis of variance McGraw-Hill. Health Profession Division, New York
52.
go back to reference Bretz F, Westfall P, Hothorn T(2016)Multiple comparisons using R CRC Press, London Bretz F, Westfall P, Hothorn T(2016)Multiple comparisons using R CRC Press, London
53.
go back to reference Cohen J (2013) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Elsevier Science, BurlingtonCrossRef Cohen J (2013) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Elsevier Science, BurlingtonCrossRef
54.
go back to reference Chinn S (2000) A simple method for converting an odds ratio to effect size for use in meta-analysis. Stat Med 19(22):3127–3131PubMedCrossRef Chinn S (2000) A simple method for converting an odds ratio to effect size for use in meta-analysis. Stat Med 19(22):3127–3131PubMedCrossRef
55.
go back to reference Berden GF, Althaus M, Verhulst FC, Psychiatry (1990) Major life events and changes in the behavioural functioning of children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 31(6):949–959PubMedCrossRef Berden GF, Althaus M, Verhulst FC, Psychiatry (1990) Major life events and changes in the behavioural functioning of children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 31(6):949–959PubMedCrossRef
56.
go back to reference Flouri E, Mavroveli S, Panourgia C (2013) The role of general cognitive ability in moderating the relation of adverse life events to emotional and behavioural problems. Br J Psychol 104(1):130–139PubMedCrossRef Flouri E, Mavroveli S, Panourgia C (2013) The role of general cognitive ability in moderating the relation of adverse life events to emotional and behavioural problems. Br J Psychol 104(1):130–139PubMedCrossRef
57.
go back to reference Waldie K, Spreen O (1993) The relationship between learning disabilities and persisting delinquency. J Learn Disabil 26(6):417–423PubMedCrossRef Waldie K, Spreen O (1993) The relationship between learning disabilities and persisting delinquency. J Learn Disabil 26(6):417–423PubMedCrossRef
58.
go back to reference Emerson E, Einfeld S (2010) Emotional and behavioural difficulties in young children with and without developmental delay: a bi-national perspective. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51(5):583–593PubMedCrossRef Emerson E, Einfeld S (2010) Emotional and behavioural difficulties in young children with and without developmental delay: a bi-national perspective. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51(5):583–593PubMedCrossRef
59.
go back to reference Casaletto KB, Umlauf A, Beaumont J, Gershon R, Slotkin J, Akshoomoff N et al (2015) Demographically corrected normative standards for the English version of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 21(5):378–391PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Casaletto KB, Umlauf A, Beaumont J, Gershon R, Slotkin J, Akshoomoff N et al (2015) Demographically corrected normative standards for the English version of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 21(5):378–391PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
60.
go back to reference Savitz J, Solms M, Ramesar R The molecular genetics of cognition: dopamine, COMT and BDNF. Genes(2006)Brain and Behavior5(4):311–328 Savitz J, Solms M, Ramesar R The molecular genetics of cognition: dopamine, COMT and BDNF. Genes(2006)Brain and Behavior5(4):311–328
61.
go back to reference Van den Bergh BR, Mulder EJ, Mennes M, Glover V (2005) Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29(2):237–258PubMedCrossRef Van den Bergh BR, Mulder EJ, Mennes M, Glover V (2005) Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29(2):237–258PubMedCrossRef
63.
go back to reference Slykerman RF, Thompson J, Waldie KE, Murphy R, Wall C, Mitchell EA (2017) Antibiotics in the first year of life and subsequent neurocognitive outcomes. Acta Paediatr 106(1):87–94PubMedCrossRef Slykerman RF, Thompson J, Waldie KE, Murphy R, Wall C, Mitchell EA (2017) Antibiotics in the first year of life and subsequent neurocognitive outcomes. Acta Paediatr 106(1):87–94PubMedCrossRef
64.
go back to reference Thompson JM, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Morgan AR, Cornforth CM, Turic D, Ferguson LR et al (2012) The catechol‐O‐methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism moderates the effect of antenatal stress on childhood behavioural problems: longitudinal evidence across multiple ages. Dev Med Child Neurol 54(2):148–154PubMedCrossRef Thompson JM, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Morgan AR, Cornforth CM, Turic D, Ferguson LR et al (2012) The catechol‐O‐methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism moderates the effect of antenatal stress on childhood behavioural problems: longitudinal evidence across multiple ages. Dev Med Child Neurol 54(2):148–154PubMedCrossRef
65.
go back to reference Turkstra LS, Williams WH, Tonks J, Frampton I (2008) Measuring social cognition in adolescents: Implications for students with TBI returning to school. NeuroRehabilitation 23(6):501–509PubMedCrossRef Turkstra LS, Williams WH, Tonks J, Frampton I (2008) Measuring social cognition in adolescents: Implications for students with TBI returning to school. NeuroRehabilitation 23(6):501–509PubMedCrossRef
66.
go back to reference Cortina MA, Fazel M (2015) The Art Room: An evaluation of a targeted school-based group intervention for students with emotional and behavioural difficulties. The Arts in Psychotherapy 42:35–40CrossRef Cortina MA, Fazel M (2015) The Art Room: An evaluation of a targeted school-based group intervention for students with emotional and behavioural difficulties. The Arts in Psychotherapy 42:35–40CrossRef
67.
go back to reference Raviv T, Kessenich M, Morrison FJ (2004) A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: The role of parenting factors. Early Child Res Q 19(4):528–547CrossRef Raviv T, Kessenich M, Morrison FJ (2004) A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: The role of parenting factors. Early Child Res Q 19(4):528–547CrossRef
68.
go back to reference Schroeder VM, Kelley ML (2010) Family environment and parent-child relationships as related to executive functioning in children. Early child development care 180(10):1285–1298CrossRef Schroeder VM, Kelley ML (2010) Family environment and parent-child relationships as related to executive functioning in children. Early child development care 180(10):1285–1298CrossRef
69.
go back to reference Ogden JA, McFarlane-Nathan G (1997) Cultural bias in the neuropsychological assessment of young Māori men. NZ J Psychol 26:2–12 Ogden JA, McFarlane-Nathan G (1997) Cultural bias in the neuropsychological assessment of young Māori men. NZ J Psychol 26:2–12
70.
go back to reference Haitana T, Pitama S, Rucklidge JJ (2010) Cultural biases in the peabody picture vocabulary test-III: testing tamariki in a New Zealand sample. NZ J Psychol 39(3):24–34 Haitana T, Pitama S, Rucklidge JJ (2010) Cultural biases in the peabody picture vocabulary test-III: testing tamariki in a New Zealand sample. NZ J Psychol 39(3):24–34
71.
go back to reference Mieloo CL, Bevaart F, Donker MC, van Oort FV, Raat H, Jansen W (2014) Validation of the SDQ in a multi-ethnic population of young children. Eur J Public Health 24(1):26–32PubMedCrossRef Mieloo CL, Bevaart F, Donker MC, van Oort FV, Raat H, Jansen W (2014) Validation of the SDQ in a multi-ethnic population of young children. Eur J Public Health 24(1):26–32PubMedCrossRef
72.
go back to reference Raaijmakers MA, Smidts DP, Sergeant JA, Maassen GH, Posthumus JA, Van Engeland H, et al. (2008) Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36(7):1097 Raaijmakers MA, Smidts DP, Sergeant JA, Maassen GH, Posthumus JA, Van Engeland H, et al. (2008) Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36(7):1097
Metagegevens
Titel
The Association Between Persistence and Change in Behavioral Difficulties During Early to Middle Childhood and Cognitive Abilities at Age 8
Auteurs
Denise Neumann
Elizabeth R. Peterson
Lisa Underwood
Susan M.B. Morton
Karen E. Waldie
Publicatiedatum
14-11-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 4/2024
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-022-01453-1