Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2007

01-03-2007 | Original Article

Tau and Kappa effects in physical space: the case of audition

Auteurs: Jean-Christophe Sarrazin, Marie-Dominique Giraudo, John Bruce Pittenger

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The organization of spatio-temporal information in an auditory memory task was studied in two experiments. Stimuli consisted of four different configurations of eight sequentially presented beeps. In two configurations, the stimuli were space–time congruent, with (constant or variable) inter-stimulus distances corresponding to (constant or variable) inter-stimulus time intervals. In the other two configurations, the stimuli were space–time incongruent, with (constant or variable) inter-stimulus distances not corresponding to (variable or constant) inter-stimulus time intervals. After a learning phase consisting of 20 presentations of the target configuration, participants reproduced the spatial (Experiment 1) or temporal (Experiment 2) characteristics of the target 60 times in succession without re-examining the target configuration. Accuracy (with respect to the target) and variability (between responses) were found to evolve independently. In the incongruent space–time conditions, effects of variable inter-stimulus time intervals or distances on the reproduction of, respectively, constant distances (Tau effect) or constant time intervals (Kappa effect) were observed, while the reverse was not the case. Thus, dimensional interference occurred when the dimension to be ignored was variable. The results are discussed in the light of the distinction between properties of the stabilized mental image and the process of stabilization.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The conditions are labeled in the same way that the first experiment. The first letter corresponds to the spatial dimension and the second letter to the temporal dimension
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anderson, N.H. (1974). Algebraic models in perception. In E.C. Carterette & M.P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press. Anderson, N.H. (1974). Algebraic models in perception. In E.C. Carterette & M.P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Benussi, V. (1913). Versuche zur Analyse taktil erweckter Scheinbewegungen. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 36, 58–135. Benussi, V. (1913). Versuche zur Analyse taktil erweckter Scheinbewegungen. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 36, 58–135.
go back to reference Bill, J.C., & Teft, L.W. (1969). Space–time relations: Effects of time on perceived visual extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 196–199.PubMedCrossRef Bill, J.C., & Teft, L.W. (1969). Space–time relations: Effects of time on perceived visual extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 196–199.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bill, J.C., & Teft, L.W. (1972). Space–time relations: The effects of variations in stimulus and interstimulus interval duration on perceived visual extent. Acta Psychologica, 36, 358–369.CrossRefPubMed Bill, J.C., & Teft, L.W. (1972). Space–time relations: The effects of variations in stimulus and interstimulus interval duration on perceived visual extent. Acta Psychologica, 36, 358–369.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bremer, C.D., Pittenger, J.B., Warren, R., & Jenkins, J.J. (1977). An illusion of auditory saltation similar to the cutaneous “rabbit”. American Journal of Psychology, 90, 645–654.PubMedCrossRef Bremer, C.D., Pittenger, J.B., Warren, R., & Jenkins, J.J. (1977). An illusion of auditory saltation similar to the cutaneous “rabbit”. American Journal of Psychology, 90, 645–654.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Burnham, W.H. (1903). Retroactive amnesia: Illustrative cases and a tentative explanation. American Journal of Psychology, 14, 382–396.CrossRef Burnham, W.H. (1903). Retroactive amnesia: Illustrative cases and a tentative explanation. American Journal of Psychology, 14, 382–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Chatfield, C. (1975). The analysis of time series: Theories and practice. New York: Wiley. Chatfield, C. (1975). The analysis of time series: Theories and practice. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Chen, Y., Repp, B.H., & Patel, A.D. (2002). Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: Comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, 21, 515–532.CrossRefPubMed Chen, Y., Repp, B.H., & Patel, A.D. (2002). Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: Comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, 21, 515–532.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cohen, J., Hansel, C.E.M., & Sylvester, J.D. (1953). A new phenomenon in time judgement. Nature, London, 172, 901. Cohen, J., Hansel, C.E.M., & Sylvester, J.D. (1953). A new phenomenon in time judgement. Nature, London, 172, 901.
go back to reference Collyer, C.E. (1977). Discrimination of spatial and temporal intervals defined by three light flashes: Effects of spacing on temporal judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 357–364. Collyer, C.E. (1977). Discrimination of spatial and temporal intervals defined by three light flashes: Effects of spacing on temporal judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 357–364.
go back to reference Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 164–191.CrossRef Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 164–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Fitts, P.M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–391.PubMedCrossRef Fitts, P.M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–391.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Geldard, F.A., & Sherrick, C.E. (1972). The cutaneous “rabbit”: A perceptual illusion. Science, 178, 178–179.PubMedCrossRef Geldard, F.A., & Sherrick, C.E. (1972). The cutaneous “rabbit”: A perceptual illusion. Science, 178, 178–179.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gilden, D. L., Thornton, T., & Mallon, M. W. (1995). 1/f noise in human cognition. Science, 267, 1837–1839.PubMedCrossRef Gilden, D. L., Thornton, T., & Mallon, M. W. (1995). 1/f noise in human cognition. Science, 267, 1837–1839.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Giraudo, M.D., & Pailhous, J. (1999). Dynamic instability of visuo-spatial images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 6, 1495–1516.CrossRefPubMed Giraudo, M.D., & Pailhous, J. (1999). Dynamic instability of visuo-spatial images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 6, 1495–1516.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Glenberg, A.,& Jona, M. (1991). Temporal coding in rhythm tasks revealed by modality effects. Memory and Cognition, 19, 514–522. Glenberg, A.,& Jona, M. (1991). Temporal coding in rhythm tasks revealed by modality effects. Memory and Cognition, 19, 514–522.
go back to reference Golledge, R.G. (1987). Environmental cognition. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of Environmental Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 131–174). New-York: Wiley. Golledge, R.G. (1987). Environmental cognition. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of Environmental Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 131–174). New-York: Wiley.
go back to reference Grondin, S., & Rousseau, R. (1991). Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 245–256. Grondin, S., & Rousseau, R. (1991). Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 245–256.
go back to reference Helson, H. (1930). The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. Science, 71, 536–537.CrossRef Helson, H. (1930). The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. Science, 71, 536–537.CrossRef
go back to reference Helson, H., & King, S.M. (1931). The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 202–217.CrossRef Helson, H., & King, S.M. (1931). The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 202–217.CrossRef
go back to reference Jones, B., & Huang, Y.L. (1982). Space–time dependencies in psychological judgment of extent and duration: algebraic model of the Tau and Kappa effects. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 128–142.CrossRef Jones, B., & Huang, Y.L. (1982). Space–time dependencies in psychological judgment of extent and duration: algebraic model of the Tau and Kappa effects. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 128–142.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelso, J.A.S. (1981). On the oscillatory basis of movement. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 63. Kelso, J.A.S. (1981). On the oscillatory basis of movement. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 63.
go back to reference Lee, D. (2000). Learning of spatial and temporal patterns in sequential hand movements. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 35–39.CrossRefPubMed Lee, D. (2000). Learning of spatial and temporal patterns in sequential hand movements. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 35–39.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Monno, A., Chardenon, A., Temprado, J.J., Zanone, P.G., & Laurent, M. (2000). Effects of attention on phase transitions between bimanual coordination patterns: a behavioral and cost analysis in humans. Neuroscience Letters. 283: 93–96.CrossRefPubMed Monno, A., Chardenon, A., Temprado, J.J., Zanone, P.G., & Laurent, M. (2000). Effects of attention on phase transitions between bimanual coordination patterns: a behavioral and cost analysis in humans. Neuroscience Letters. 283: 93–96.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Newell, K.M., & Corcos, D.M. (1993). Issues in variability and motor control. In K.M. Newell & D.M. Corcos (Eds.), Variability and motor control (pp. 1–12). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Newell, K.M., & Corcos, D.M. (1993). Issues in variability and motor control. In K.M. Newell & D.M. Corcos (Eds.), Variability and motor control (pp. 1–12). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Newman, C.V., & Lee, S.G. (1972). The effect of real and imputed distance on judgments of time: Some experiments on the Kappa effect. Psychonomic Science, 29, 207–211. Newman, C.V., & Lee, S.G. (1972). The effect of real and imputed distance on judgments of time: Some experiments on the Kappa effect. Psychonomic Science, 29, 207–211.
go back to reference Pailhous, J., & Bonnard, M. (1992). Steady state variability of human gait: Role of optical flow. Behavior Brain Research, 47, 181–190.CrossRef Pailhous, J., & Bonnard, M. (1992). Steady state variability of human gait: Role of optical flow. Behavior Brain Research, 47, 181–190.CrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (1980). The development of attentional mechanisms. In P.M.A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and Performance (Vol. V). New York: Academic Press. Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (1980). The development of attentional mechanisms. In P.M.A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and Performance (Vol. V). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Repp, B.H., & Penel, A. (2002). Auditory dominance in temporal processing: New evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1085–1099.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B.H., & Penel, A. (2002). Auditory dominance in temporal processing: New evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1085–1099.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B.H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68(4), 252–270CrossRefPubMed Repp, B.H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68(4), 252–270CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sarrazin, J.C., Giraudo, M.D.,& Pailhous, J. (2001). Dynamics space and time dimensions in the memorizing process. In G. Burton and R. Schmidt (Eds.). Studies in perception and action VI (pp. 209–212). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Sarrazin, J.C., Giraudo, M.D.,& Pailhous, J. (2001). Dynamics space and time dimensions in the memorizing process. In G. Burton and R. Schmidt (Eds.). Studies in perception and action VI (pp. 209–212). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Sarrazin, J.C., Giraudo, M.D. Pailhous, J., & Bootsma, R.J. (2004). Tau and Kappa effects revisited: Dynamics of balancing space and time in Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30,3, 411–430.CrossRefPubMed Sarrazin, J.C., Giraudo, M.D. Pailhous, J., & Bootsma, R.J. (2004). Tau and Kappa effects revisited: Dynamics of balancing space and time in Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30,3, 411–430.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmidt, R.A., (1988). Motor control and learning. A behavioral emphasis. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publishers. Schmidt, R.A., (1988). Motor control and learning. A behavioral emphasis. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publishers.
go back to reference Sherrick, C. E. (1974). Sensory processes. In L. L. Elliott& J. A. Swets (Eds.), Psychology and the handicapped child (pp.13–39). Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office. Sherrick, C. E. (1974). Sensory processes. In L. L. Elliott& J. A. Swets (Eds.), Psychology and the handicapped child (pp.13–39). Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office.
go back to reference Sokolov, E.N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Pergamon. Sokolov, E.N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Pergamon.
go back to reference Stoffregen, T.A., & Bardy, B.G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 24, 195–261.CrossRef Stoffregen, T.A., & Bardy, B.G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 24, 195–261.CrossRef
go back to reference Suzuki, S., & Cavanagh, P. (1997). Focused attention distorts visual space: An attentional repulsion effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 443–463.CrossRef Suzuki, S., & Cavanagh, P. (1997). Focused attention distorts visual space: An attentional repulsion effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 443–463.CrossRef
go back to reference Temprado, J.J., Chardenon, A., & Laurent, M. (2001). Interplay of biomechanical and neuromuscular constraints on pattern stability and attentional demands in a bimanual coordination task. Neuroscience Letters, 303, 127–131.CrossRefPubMed Temprado, J.J., Chardenon, A., & Laurent, M. (2001). Interplay of biomechanical and neuromuscular constraints on pattern stability and attentional demands in a bimanual coordination task. Neuroscience Letters, 303, 127–131.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wade, N.J., & Heller, D. (1997). Scopes of perception: the experimental manipulation of space and time. Psychological Research, 60(4), 227–237.CrossRefPubMed Wade, N.J., & Heller, D. (1997). Scopes of perception: the experimental manipulation of space and time. Psychological Research, 60(4), 227–237.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Willingham, D.B., Greenberg, A.R., & Thomas, R.C. (1997). Response-to-stimulus interval does not affect implicit motor sequence learning, but does affect performance. Memory and Cognition, 25, 534–542. Willingham, D.B., Greenberg, A.R., & Thomas, R.C. (1997). Response-to-stimulus interval does not affect implicit motor sequence learning, but does affect performance. Memory and Cognition, 25, 534–542.
Metagegevens
Titel
Tau and Kappa effects in physical space: the case of audition
Auteurs
Jean-Christophe Sarrazin
Marie-Dominique Giraudo
John Bruce Pittenger
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0019-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2007

Psychological Research 2/2007 Naar de uitgave