Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 8/2019

16-06-2018 | Original Article

Task-set control, chunking, and hierarchical timing in rhythm production

Auteurs: Lars D. Hestermann, Johan Wagemans, Ralf T. Krampe

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We investigated task-set control processes and chunking in 16 novices and 16 amateur musicians, who produced unimanual rhythms in three experimental conditions: low-level timing tasks required isochronous tapping at constant target durations; sequencing tasks consisted of individual rhythmic patterns comprising multiple target durations; the task-set control condition required alternations between two rhythmic patterns. According to our hierarchical timing control model conditions differed in their task-set control demands necessary to provide rhythm programs for the sequencing of individual intervals. Transitions at predicted chunk boundaries were marked by increased frequencies of sequence errors, relative lengthening of intervals preceding the switch to a new rhythm chunk, and increased variabilities in intervals immediately following a switch. Amateur musicians showed superior timing (less variability) in complex rhythm tasks. Moreover, they made fewer sequence errors than novices at set-switch points with their error patterns suggesting that they relied on larger chunks compared with novices. Our findings elucidate the time course of task reconfiguration processes in rhythm production and the role of chunking in the context of musical skill.
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, DA, Styles AE, & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting attentional set—exploring the dynamic control of tasks. Attention and Performance XV, Vol. XV. Allport, DA, Styles AE, & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting attentional set—exploring the dynamic control of tasks. Attention and Performance XV, Vol. XV.
go back to reference Bailey, J. A., & Penhune, V. B. (2010). Rhythm synchronization performance and auditory working memory in early- and late-trained musicians. Experimental Brain Research, 204(1), 91–101.CrossRefPubMed Bailey, J. A., & Penhune, V. B. (2010). Rhythm synchronization performance and auditory working memory in early- and late-trained musicians. Experimental Brain Research, 204(1), 91–101.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Beek, P. J., Peper, C. E., & Daffertshofer, A. (2002). Modeling rhythmic interlimb coordination: Beyond the Haken–Kelso–Bunz model. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 149–165.CrossRefPubMed Beek, P. J., Peper, C. E., & Daffertshofer, A. (2002). Modeling rhythmic interlimb coordination: Beyond the Haken–Kelso–Bunz model. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 149–165.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Block, R. A., Hancock, P. A., & Zakay, D. (2010). How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 330–343.CrossRefPubMed Block, R. A., Hancock, P. A., & Zakay, D. (2010). How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 330–343.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Block, R. A., Zakay, D., & Hancock, P. A. (1998). Human aging and duration judgments: a meta-analytic review. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 584–596.CrossRefPubMed Block, R. A., Zakay, D., & Hancock, P. A. (1998). Human aging and duration judgments: a meta-analytic review. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 584–596.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Block, R. A., Zakay, D., & Hancock, P. A. (1999). Developmental changes in human duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 19(1), 183–211.CrossRef Block, R. A., Zakay, D., & Hancock, P. A. (1999). Developmental changes in human duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 19(1), 183–211.CrossRef
go back to reference Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. The American Journal of Psychology, 6(2), 145–238.CrossRef Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. The American Journal of Psychology, 6(2), 145–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Bouwer, F. L., Burgoyne, J. A., Odijk, D., Honing, H., & Grahn, J. A. (2018). What makes a rhythm complex? The influence of musical training and accent type on beat perception. PloS One, 13(1), e0190322. Bouwer, F. L., Burgoyne, J. A., Odijk, D., Honing, H., & Grahn, J. A. (2018). What makes a rhythm complex? The influence of musical training and accent type on beat perception. PloS One, 13(1), e0190322.
go back to reference Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), nrn1764.CrossRef Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), nrn1764.CrossRef
go back to reference Carrara-Augustenborg, C., & Schultz, B. G. (2017). The implicit learning of metrical and non-metrical rhythms in blind and sighted adults. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 1–17. Carrara-Augustenborg, C., & Schultz, B. G. (2017). The implicit learning of metrical and non-metrical rhythms in blind and sighted adults. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 1–17.
go back to reference Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Moving on time: Brain network for auditory-motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(2), 226–239.CrossRefPubMed Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Moving on time: Brain network for auditory-motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(2), 226–239.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Drake, C. (1993). Reproduction of musical rhythms by children, adult musicians, and adult nonmusicians. Perception & Psychophysics, 53(1), 25–33.CrossRef Drake, C. (1993). Reproduction of musical rhythms by children, adult musicians, and adult nonmusicians. Perception & Psychophysics, 53(1), 25–33.CrossRef
go back to reference Engbert, R., Scheffczyk, C., Krampe, R. T., Rosenblum, M., Kurths, J., & Kliegl, R. (1997). Tempo-induced transitions in polyrhythmic hand movements. Physical Review E, 56(5), 5823–5833.CrossRef Engbert, R., Scheffczyk, C., Krampe, R. T., Rosenblum, M., Kurths, J., & Kliegl, R. (1997). Tempo-induced transitions in polyrhythmic hand movements. Physical Review E, 56(5), 5823–5833.CrossRef
go back to reference Ericcson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science (New York, N.Y.), 208(4448), 1181–1182.CrossRef Ericcson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science (New York, N.Y.), 208(4448), 1181–1182.CrossRef
go back to reference Ericsson, K. A., & Chase, W. G. (1982). Exceptional memory. American Scientist, 70(6), 607–615.PubMed Ericsson, K. A., & Chase, W. G. (1982). Exceptional memory. American Scientist, 70(6), 607–615.PubMed
go back to reference Essens, P. J., & Povel, D.-J. (1985). Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 37(1), 1–7.CrossRef Essens, P. J., & Povel, D.-J. (1985). Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 37(1), 1–7.CrossRef
go back to reference Fraisse, P. (1946). II. - Mouvements rythmiques et arythmiques. L’Année psychologique, 47(1), 11–27.CrossRef Fraisse, P. (1946). II. - Mouvements rythmiques et arythmiques. L’Année psychologique, 47(1), 11–27.CrossRef
go back to reference Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1968). The perception and learning of temporal patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(2), 97–109.CrossRefPubMed Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1968). The perception and learning of temporal patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(2), 97–109.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grahn, J. A., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(23), 7540–7548.CrossRef Grahn, J. A., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(23), 7540–7548.CrossRef
go back to reference Grange, J. A., & Houghton, G. (2010). Task preparation and task inhibition: a comment on Koch, Gade, Schuch, & Philipp (2010). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 211–216.CrossRef Grange, J. A., & Houghton, G. (2010). Task preparation and task inhibition: a comment on Koch, Gade, Schuch, & Philipp (2010). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 211–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561–582.CrossRef Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561–582.CrossRef
go back to reference Honing, H. (2012). Without it no music: Beat induction as a fundamental musical trait. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 85–91.CrossRefPubMed Honing, H. (2012). Without it no music: Beat induction as a fundamental musical trait. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 85–91.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., & Ohgushi, K. (2008). Perception of rhythmic grouping depends on auditory experience. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(4), 2263–2271.CrossRefPubMed Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., & Ohgushi, K. (2008). Perception of rhythmic grouping depends on auditory experience. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(4), 2263–2271.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, R. E. (1995). Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 3–18.CrossRefPubMed Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, R. E. (1995). Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 3–18.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ivry, R. B., & Richardson, T. C. (2002). Temporal control and coordination: The multiple timer model. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 117–132.CrossRefPubMed Ivry, R. B., & Richardson, T. C. (2002). Temporal control and coordination: The multiple timer model. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 117–132.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83(5), 323–355.CrossRefPubMed Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83(5), 323–355.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jones, M. R. (2009). Musical time. In S. Hallam, I. Cross & M. Thaut (Eds.), The handbook of musical psychology (pp. 81–92). New York: Oxford University Press. Jones, M. R. (2009). Musical time. In S. Hallam, I. Cross & M. Thaut (Eds.), The handbook of musical psychology (pp. 81–92). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96(3), 459–491.CrossRefPubMed Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96(3), 459–491.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance., 70, 387–403. Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance., 70, 387–403.
go back to reference Keele, S. W. (1986). Motor control. In (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (Vol. 2). New York, NY: Wiley. K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas. Keele, S. W. (1986). Motor control. In (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (Vol. 2). New York, NY: Wiley. K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas.
go back to reference Kincaid, A. E., Duncan, S., & Scott, S. A. (2002). Assessment of fine motor skill in musicians and nonmusicians: Differences in timing versus sequence accuracy in a bimanual fingering task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95(1), 245–257.CrossRefPubMed Kincaid, A. E., Duncan, S., & Scott, S. A. (2002). Assessment of fine motor skill in musicians and nonmusicians: Differences in timing versus sequence accuracy in a bimanual fingering task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95(1), 245–257.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2002). Representational models and nonlinear dynamics: Irreconcilable approaches to human movement timing and coordination or two sides of the same coin? Introduction to the special issue on movement timing and coordination. Introduction. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 1–6.CrossRefPubMed Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2002). Representational models and nonlinear dynamics: Irreconcilable approaches to human movement timing and coordination or two sides of the same coin? Introduction to the special issue on movement timing and coordination. Introduction. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 1–6.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krampe, R. T, Kliegl, R, Mayr, U, Engbert, R, & Vorberg, D (2000). The fast and the slow of skilled bimanual rhythm production: Parallel versus integrated timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 26(1), 206–233.CrossRefPubMed Krampe, R. T, Kliegl, R, Mayr, U, Engbert, R, & Vorberg, D (2000). The fast and the slow of skilled bimanual rhythm production: Parallel versus integrated timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 26(1), 206–233.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Timing, sequencing, and executive control in repetitive movement production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 379–397.CrossRefPubMed Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Timing, sequencing, and executive control in repetitive movement production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 379–397.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Large, E., & Riess Jones, M. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.CrossRef Large, E., & Riess Jones, M. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., Herrera, J. A., & Velasco, M. J. (2015). Neural networks for beat perception in musical rhythm. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9. Large, E. W., Herrera, J. A., & Velasco, M. J. (2015). Neural networks for beat perception in musical rhythm. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9.
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26(1), 1–37.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26(1), 1–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. New York: Wiley. Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Lustig, C., & Meck, W. H. (2001). Paying attention to time as one gets older. Psychological Science, 12(6), 478–484.CrossRefPubMed Lustig, C., & Meck, W. H. (2001). Paying attention to time as one gets older. Psychological Science, 12(6), 478–484.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Matthews, T. E., Thibodeau, J. N. L., Gunther, B. P., & Penhune, V. B. (2016). The impact of instrument-specific musical training on rhythm perception and production. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 69.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Matthews, T. E., Thibodeau, J. N. L., Gunther, B. P., & Penhune, V. B. (2016). The impact of instrument-specific musical training on rhythm perception and production. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 69.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), pp. 4–26.CrossRef Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), pp. 4–26.CrossRef
go back to reference Meinz, E. J., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). Deliberate practice is necessary but not sufficient to explain individual differences in piano sight-reading skill: The role of working memory capacity. Psychological Science, 21(7), 914–919.CrossRefPubMed Meinz, E. J., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). Deliberate practice is necessary but not sufficient to explain individual differences in piano sight-reading skill: The role of working memory capacity. Psychological Science, 21(7), 914–919.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Monsell, S., Sumner, P., & Waters, H. (2003). Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches. Memory & Cognition, 31(3), 327–342.CrossRef Monsell, S., Sumner, P., & Waters, H. (2003). Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches. Memory & Cognition, 31(3), 327–342.CrossRef
go back to reference Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1425–1433.CrossRefPubMed Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1425–1433.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (2003). Human memory: An introduction to research, data, and theory. Thomson/Wadsworth, Belmont Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (2003). Human memory: An introduction to research, data, and theory. Thomson/Wadsworth, Belmont
go back to reference Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the Beat: movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science; Washington, 308(5727), 1430.CrossRef Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the Beat: movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science; Washington, 308(5727), 1430.CrossRef
go back to reference Povel, D.-J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(4), 411–440.CrossRef Povel, D.-J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(4), 411–440.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Bruttomesso, M. (2010). A filled duration illusion in music: Effects of metrical subdivision on the perception and production of beat tempo. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 5, 114–134.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Repp, B. H., & Bruttomesso, M. (2010). A filled duration illusion in music: Effects of metrical subdivision on the perception and production of beat tempo. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 5, 114–134.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General; Washington, 124(2), 207.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General; Washington, 124(2), 207.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Kenny, S. B., & Derr, M. A. (1983). Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 9(1), 86–102.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Kenny, S. B., & Derr, M. A. (1983). Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 9(1), 86–102.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmitz, F., & Voss, A. (2014). Components of task switching: a closer look at task switching and cue switching. Acta Psychologica, 151, 184–196.CrossRefPubMed Schmitz, F., & Voss, A. (2014). Components of task switching: a closer look at task switching and cue switching. Acta Psychologica, 151, 184–196.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schultz, B. G., Stevens, C. J., Keller, P. E., & Tillmann, B. (2013). The implicit learning of metrical and nonmetrical temporal patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 360–380.CrossRefPubMed Schultz, B. G., Stevens, C. J., Keller, P. E., & Tillmann, B. (2013). The implicit learning of metrical and nonmetrical temporal patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 360–380.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Strobach, T., Liepelt, R., Schubert, T., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 76(1), 74–83.CrossRefPubMed Strobach, T., Liepelt, R., Schubert, T., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 76(1), 74–83.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thaut, M. H., Trimarchi, P. D., & Parsons, L. M. (2014). Human brain basis of musical rhythm perception: Common and distinct neural substrates for meter, tempo, and pattern. Brain Sciences, 4(2), 428–452.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Thaut, M. H., Trimarchi, P. D., & Parsons, L. M. (2014). Human brain basis of musical rhythm perception: Common and distinct neural substrates for meter, tempo, and pattern. Brain Sciences, 4(2), 428–452.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Tulving, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.). (2005). The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Tulving, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.). (2005). The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference van der Weij, B., Pearce, M. T., & Honing, H. (2017). A probabilistic model of meter perception: Simulating enculturation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. van der Weij, B., Pearce, M. T., & Honing, H. (2017). A probabilistic model of meter perception: Simulating enculturation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8.
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1978). On the temporal control of rhythmic performance. In Attention and performance (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1978). On the temporal control of rhythmic performance. In Attention and performance (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1984). Timing of two-handed rhythmic performance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 390–406.CrossRefPubMed Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1984). Timing of two-handed rhythmic performance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 390–406.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. M. (1996). Modelling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action (Vol. 3, pp. 181–261). London: Academic Press. Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. M. (1996). Modelling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action (Vol. 3, pp. 181–261). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Wing, A. M., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(1), 5–12.CrossRef Wing, A. M., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(1), 5–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Wing, A. M., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1973). The timing of interresponse intervals., 13, 455–460. Wing, A. M., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1973). The timing of interresponse intervals., 13, 455–460.
go back to reference Yu, M., Getz, L., & Kubovy, M. (2015). Perceiving the initial note: Quantitative models of how listeners parse cyclical auditory patterns. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(8), 2728–2739.CrossRef Yu, M., Getz, L., & Kubovy, M. (2015). Perceiving the initial note: Quantitative models of how listeners parse cyclical auditory patterns. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(8), 2728–2739.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Task-set control, chunking, and hierarchical timing in rhythm production
Auteurs
Lars D. Hestermann
Johan Wagemans
Ralf T. Krampe
Publicatiedatum
16-06-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1038-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2019

Psychological Research 8/2019 Naar de uitgave