Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2015

01-03-2015 | Original Article

Task predictability influences the variable foreperiod effect: evidence of task-specific temporal preparation

Auteurs: Hannes Schröter, Teresa Birngruber, Daniel Bratzke, Jeff Miller, Rolf Ulrich

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Temporal preparation usually results in enhanced performance in choice reaction time tasks. The present study investigated to what extent temporal preparation involves increased readiness for task-specific processing requirements as opposed to increased task-independent readiness. Participants performed either a pitch, a letter, or a color discrimination task within a variable foreperiod paradigm and tasks alternated regularly between auditory and visual discriminations. In separate blocks of trials, the upcoming visual discrimination task was either predictable or unpredictable. We observed the standard variable foreperiod effect for both visual discrimination tasks irrespective of task predictability. Importantly, however, the variable foreperiod effect was larger when the visual discrimination task was predictable than when it was unpredictable. These results suggest that temporal preparation in choice reaction time tasks involves increased readiness for both task-independent and task-specific processing requirements.
Voetnoten
1
In order to test whether the sequential FP effect usually observed in standard choice RT tasks could be replicated within the present experimental design, a supplemental ANOVA with the additional factor of FP in the preceding trial (short vs. long) was performed on RT. Most importantly, this ANOVA revealed a significant interaction of FP in the preceding trial and FP, F(1,39) = 5.67, MSE = 1,076, p = 0.022,  η p 2 = 0.127, reflecting a typical asymmetric sequential FP effect. The sequential FP effect tended to be more pronounced in blocks with two than with three tasks, as was indicated by the marginally significant three-way interaction of FP in the preceding trial, FP, and block type, F(1,39) = 3.31, MSE = 1,174, p = 0.076,  η p 2 = 0.078.
 
2
It is under debate whether this configuration process is switch specific (task-set reconfiguration) or only more time consuming for switches than for repetitions (task updating; for a review, see Kiesel et al., 2010). This distinction, however, is not important for the interpretation of the present data because both accounts predict reduced RT for task switches when advance task preparation is possible. For the remainder of the discussion, we use the term advance task-set configuration.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Alegria, J. (1975). Sequential effects of foreperiod duration: Some strategical factors in tasks involving time uncertainty. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance V (pp. 1–10). London: Academic Press. Alegria, J. (1975). Sequential effects of foreperiod duration: Some strategical factors in tasks involving time uncertainty. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance V (pp. 1–10). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Altmann, E. M., & Gray, W. D. (2008). An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. Psychological Review, 115(3), 602–639.CrossRefPubMed Altmann, E. M., & Gray, W. D. (2008). An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. Psychological Review, 115(3), 602–639.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Correa, Á., Lupiáñez, J., Milliken, B., & Tudela, P. (2004). Endogenous temporal orienting of attention in detection and discrimination tasks. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(2), 264–278.CrossRefPubMed Correa, Á., Lupiáñez, J., Milliken, B., & Tudela, P. (2004). Endogenous temporal orienting of attention in detection and discrimination tasks. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(2), 264–278.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7426–7435.PubMed Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7426–7435.PubMed
go back to reference Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1(1), 42–45. Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1(1), 42–45.
go back to reference Elithorn, A., & Lawrence, C. (1955). Central inhibition—some refractory observations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7(3), 116–127.CrossRef Elithorn, A., & Lawrence, C. (1955). Central inhibition—some refractory observations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7(3), 116–127.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, C. D., & Done, D. J. (1986). Routes to action in reaction time tasks. Psychological Research, 48(3), 169–177.CrossRefPubMed Frith, C. D., & Done, D. J. (1986). Routes to action in reaction time tasks. Psychological Research, 48(3), 169–177.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Karlin, L. (1959). Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(2), 185–191.CrossRefPubMed Karlin, L. (1959). Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(2), 185–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Karlin, L. (1966), Development of readiness to respond during short foreperiods. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(4), 505–509.CrossRefPubMed Karlin, L. (1966), Development of readiness to respond during short foreperiods. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(4), 505–509.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874.CrossRefPubMed Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klemmer, E. T. (1956). Time uncertainty in simple reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 179–184.CrossRefPubMed Klemmer, E. T. (1956). Time uncertainty in simple reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 179–184.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1189–1207.PubMed Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1189–1207.PubMed
go back to reference Los, S. A. (1996). On the origin of mixing costs: Exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials. Acta Psychologica, 94(2), 145–188.CrossRef Los, S. A. (1996). On the origin of mixing costs: Exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials. Acta Psychologica, 94(2), 145–188.CrossRef
go back to reference Los, S. A., & Agter, F. (2005). Reweighting sequential effects across different distributions of foreperiods: Segregating elementary contributions to nonspecific preparation. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(7), 1161–1170.CrossRefPubMed Los, S. A., & Agter, F. (2005). Reweighting sequential effects across different distributions of foreperiods: Segregating elementary contributions to nonspecific preparation. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(7), 1161–1170.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Los, S. A., Knol, D. L., & Boers, R. M. (2001). The foreperiod effect revisited: Conditioning as a basis for nonspecific preparation. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 121–145.CrossRefPubMed Los, S. A., Knol, D. L., & Boers, R. M. (2001). The foreperiod effect revisited: Conditioning as a basis for nonspecific preparation. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 121–145.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Los, S. A., & Schut, M. L. (2008). The effective time course of preparation. Cognitive Psychology, 57(1), 20–55.CrossRefPubMed Los, S. A., & Schut, M. L. (2008). The effective time course of preparation. Cognitive Psychology, 57(1), 20–55.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Los, S. A., & Van den Heuvel, C. E. (2001). Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 370–386.PubMed Los, S. A., & Van den Heuvel, C. E. (2001). Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 370–386.PubMed
go back to reference Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1997). Response force is sensitive to the temporal uncertainty of response stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(7), 1089–1097.CrossRefPubMed Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1997). Response force is sensitive to the temporal uncertainty of response stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(7), 1089–1097.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miniussi, C., Wilding, E., Coull, J., & Nobre, A. C. (1999). Orienting attention in time: modulation of brain potentials. Brain, 122(8), 1507–1518.CrossRefPubMed Miniussi, C., Wilding, E., Coull, J., & Nobre, A. C. (1999). Orienting attention in time: modulation of brain potentials. Brain, 122(8), 1507–1518.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Müller-Gethmann, H., Ulrich, R., & Rinkenauer, G. (2003). Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Psychophysiology, 40(4), 597–611.CrossRefPubMed Müller-Gethmann, H., Ulrich, R., & Rinkenauer, G. (2003). Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Psychophysiology, 40(4), 597–611.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Näätänen, R. (1970). The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods. Acta Psychologica, 34, 399–419.CrossRefPubMed Näätänen, R. (1970). The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods. Acta Psychologica, 34, 399–419.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Näätänen, R., Muranen, V., & Merisalo, A. (1974). Timing of expectancy peak in simple reaction time situation. Acta Psychologica, 38(6), 461–470.CrossRefPubMed Näätänen, R., Muranen, V., & Merisalo, A. (1974). Timing of expectancy peak in simple reaction time situation. Acta Psychologica, 38(6), 461–470.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Niemi, P., & Lehtonen, E. (1982). Foreperiod and visual stimulus intensity: A reappraisal. Acta Psychologica, 50(1), 73–82.CrossRefPubMed Niemi, P., & Lehtonen, E. (1982). Foreperiod and visual stimulus intensity: A reappraisal. Acta Psychologica, 50(1), 73–82.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Niemi, P., & Näätänen, R. (1981). Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychological Bulletin, 89(1), 133–162.CrossRef Niemi, P., & Näätänen, R. (1981). Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychological Bulletin, 89(1), 133–162.CrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.CrossRefPubMed Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Slaviero, D. P. (1975). Differential effects of a foreperiod countdown procedure on simple and choice reaction time. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7(1), 9–14.CrossRef Simon, J. R., & Slaviero, D. P. (1975). Differential effects of a foreperiod countdown procedure on simple and choice reaction time. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7(1), 9–14.CrossRef
go back to reference Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 129(2), 297–307.CrossRefPubMed Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 129(2), 297–307.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Dynamic adjustment of temporal preparation: Shifting warning signal modality attenuates the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 132(1), 40–47.CrossRefPubMed Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Dynamic adjustment of temporal preparation: Shifting warning signal modality attenuates the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 132(1), 40–47.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thomaschke, R., & Dreisbach, G. (2013). Temporal predictability facilitates action, not perception. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1335–1340.CrossRefPubMed Thomaschke, R., & Dreisbach, G. (2013). Temporal predictability facilitates action, not perception. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1335–1340.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thomaschke, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Response specific temporal expectancy: Evidence from a variable foreperiod paradigm. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(7), 2309–2322.CrossRef Thomaschke, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Response specific temporal expectancy: Evidence from a variable foreperiod paradigm. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(7), 2309–2322.CrossRef
go back to reference Thomaschke, R., Wagener, A., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). The scope and precision of specific temporal expectancy: Evidence from a variable foreperiod paradigm. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(3), 953–964.CrossRef Thomaschke, R., Wagener, A., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). The scope and precision of specific temporal expectancy: Evidence from a variable foreperiod paradigm. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(3), 953–964.CrossRef
go back to reference Vallesi, A., Lozano, V. N., & Correa, Á. (2013). Dissociating temporal preparation processes as a function of the inter-trial interval duration. Cognition, 127(1), 22–30.CrossRefPubMed Vallesi, A., Lozano, V. N., & Correa, Á. (2013). Dissociating temporal preparation processes as a function of the inter-trial interval duration. Cognition, 127(1), 22–30.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vallesi A., & Shallice T. (2007) Developmental dissociations of preparation over time: Deconstructing the variable foreperiod phenomena. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1377–1388.PubMed Vallesi A., & Shallice T. (2007) Developmental dissociations of preparation over time: Deconstructing the variable foreperiod phenomena. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1377–1388.PubMed
go back to reference Wagener, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2010). Temporal cueing of target-identity and target-location. Experimental Psychology, 57(6), 436–445.CrossRefPubMed Wagener, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2010). Temporal cueing of target-identity and target-location. Experimental Psychology, 57(6), 436–445.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Woodrow, H. (1914). The measurement of attention. Psychological Monographs, 17(5), 1–158.CrossRef Woodrow, H. (1914). The measurement of attention. Psychological Monographs, 17(5), 1–158.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Task predictability influences the variable foreperiod effect: evidence of task-specific temporal preparation
Auteurs
Hannes Schröter
Teresa Birngruber
Daniel Bratzke
Jeff Miller
Rolf Ulrich
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0550-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2015

Psychological Research 2/2015 Naar de uitgave