Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2009

01-09-2009 | Original Article

Strategic capacity sharing between two tasks: evidence from tasks with the same and with different task sets

Auteurs: Carola Lehle, Ronald Hübner

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2009

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The goal of the present study was to investigate the costs and benefits of different degrees of strategic parallel processing between two tasks. In a series of experiments with the dual-task flanker paradigm, participants were either instructed to process the tasks serially or in parallel, or—in a control condition—they received no specific instruction. Results showed that the participants were able to adjust the degree of parallel processing as instructed in a flexible manner. Parallel processing of the two tasks repeatedly led to large costs in performance and to high crosstalk effects compared to more serial processing. In spite of the costs, a moderate degree of parallel processing was preferred in the condition with no specific instruction. This pattern of results was observed if the same task set was used for the two tasks, but also if different ones were applied. Furthermore, a modified version of the central capacity sharing (CCS) model (Tombu and Jolicoeur in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:3–18, 2003) was proposed that accounts also for crosstalk effects in dual tasks. The modified CCS model was then evaluated by fitting it successfully to the present data.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
We define central capacity similar to the capacity of cognitive control by Lavie, Hirst, De Fockert, & Viding (2004): it encompasses postperceptual operations associated with higher cognitive functions which have been demonstrated to impose a large decline in performance when conducted concurrently with other operations. Besides response selection (Pashler, 1994a), these operations likely encompass also memory consolidation (e.g., Jolicoeur & Dell’Acqua, 1998), mental rotation (Band & Miller, 1997), suppression of response priming (Stürmer, Seiss, & Leuthold, 2005) and presumably also difficult conditions of stimulus selection (e.g., Jolicoeur et al., 2006; Magen & Cohen, 2005). There is definitively a need for further clarification, respectively, integration or dissociation of the different operations. However, this is not at the scope of the present study.
 
2
That we included the particular proportion of 50% Altering-flanker and 50% Fixed-flanker trials had the following reason: In a dual-task experiment where flanker stimuli changed on every trial, participants adopted a strategy of very serial processing (Hübner & Lehle, 2007; Experiment 4). In contrast, if the flankers did not change, a parallel strategy was preferred (Hübner & Lehle, 2007, Experiments 2A, B, C). Thus, by mixing Altering-flanker and Fixed-flanker trials equally, we intended to create a condition that itself neither induces an extreme parallel nor an extreme serial processing strategy.
 
3
To analyze practice effects, we conducted several ANOVAs for the two experiments and the different instruction conditions—containing block number as a factor. There, we revealed always a main effect of block number, i.e., the response times (RT1 and RT2) decreased with increasing practice. However, there was no significant interaction of block and congruency.
 
4
The variance across subjects was rather high in all Experiments reported in the present study, which indicates that there was some between-subjects variability in the degree of serial or parallel processing that was applied in the dual tasks. However, the mean SD in Experiment 1B was not increased, but even smaller compared to Experiment 1A (198 vs. 228 ms in RT1; 214 vs. 237 ms in RT2). This was also the case for Experiment 2B compared to Experiment 2A (185 vs. 232 ms in RT1; 199 vs. 269 ms in RT2).
 
5
Since our hypothesis corresponded to a one-tailed statistical test, and the F test is two-tailed, the result can be considered as significant.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Band, G. P. H., & Miller, J. (1997). Mental rotation interferes with response preparation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 319–338.PubMedCrossRef Band, G. P. H., & Miller, J. (1997). Mental rotation interferes with response preparation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 319–338.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brisson, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2007). A psychological refractory period in access to visual short-term memory and the deployment of visual-spatial attention: Multitasking processing deficits revealed by event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 44, 323–333.PubMedCrossRef Brisson, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2007). A psychological refractory period in access to visual short-term memory and the deployment of visual-spatial attention: Multitasking processing deficits revealed by event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 44, 323–333.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, A., & Shoup, R. (1997). Perceptual dimensional constraints in response selection processes. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 128–181.PubMedCrossRef Cohen, A., & Shoup, R. (1997). Perceptual dimensional constraints in response selection processes. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 128–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ellenbogen, R., & Meiran, N. (2008). Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect. Memory & Cognition, 36, 968–978.CrossRef Ellenbogen, R., & Meiran, N. (2008). Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect. Memory & Cognition, 36, 968–978.CrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, R., Miller, J., & Schubert, T. (2007). Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1685–1699. Fischer, R., Miller, J., & Schubert, T. (2007). Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1685–1699.
go back to reference Gopher, D. (1986). In defense of resources: On structures, energies, pools and the allocation of attention. In G. R. J. Hockey, A. W. K. Gaillard & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Energetics and human information processing (pp. 352–371). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff Publishers. Gopher, D. (1986). In defense of resources: On structures, energies, pools and the allocation of attention. In G. R. J. Hockey, A. W. K. Gaillard & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Energetics and human information processing (pp. 352–371). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff Publishers.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2006). Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? Psychological Research, 70, 245–261.PubMedCrossRef Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2006). Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? Psychological Research, 70, 245–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hübner, R., & Lehle, C. (2007). Strategies of flanker coprocessing in single and dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 103–123.PubMedCrossRef Hübner, R., & Lehle, C. (2007). Strategies of flanker coprocessing in single and dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 103–123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term memory consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 138–202.PubMedCrossRef Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term memory consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 138–202.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., Sessa, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Robitaille, N. (2006). On the control of visual spatial attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychological Research, 70, 414–424.PubMedCrossRef Jolicoeur, P., Sessa, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Robitaille, N. (2006). On the control of visual spatial attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychological Research, 70, 414–424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.CrossRef Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Lehle, C., & Hübner, R. (2008). On-the-fly adaptation of selectivity in the flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 814–818.CrossRef Lehle, C., & Hübner, R. (2008). On-the-fly adaptation of selectivity in the flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 814–818.CrossRef
go back to reference Lehle, C., Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2008). Serial or parallel processing in dual tasks: What is more effortful? Psychophysiology (in press). Lehle, C., Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2008). Serial or parallel processing in dual tasks: What is more effortful? Psychophysiology (in press).
go back to reference Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238. Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 212–238.
go back to reference Lien, M. C., Schweickert, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 692–712.PubMedCrossRef Lien, M. C., Schweickert, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 692–712.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1072–1090.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1072–1090.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Magen, H., & Cohen, A. (2005). Location specificity in response selection processes for visual stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 541–548. Magen, H., & Cohen, A. (2005). Location specificity in response selection processes for visual stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 541–548.
go back to reference McLeod, P. (1977). Parallel processing and the psychological refractory period. Acta Psychologica, 41, 381–396.CrossRef McLeod, P. (1977). Parallel processing and the psychological refractory period. Acta Psychologica, 41, 381–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65.PubMedCrossRef Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research, 70, 483–493. Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research, 70, 483–493.
go back to reference Miller, J., & Alderton, M. (2006). Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 149–165.PubMedCrossRef Miller, J., & Alderton, M. (2006). Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 149–165.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Rolke, B. (2008). Parallel and serial processing in dual-tasking: An optimization account. Cognitive Psychology (in press). Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Rolke, B. (2008). Parallel and serial processing in dual-tasking: An optimization account. Cognitive Psychology (in press).
go back to reference Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 435–448.PubMedCrossRef Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 435–448.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Navon, D., & Miller, J. (2002). Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 193–251.PubMedCrossRef Navon, D., & Miller, J. (2002). Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 193–251.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oriet, C., Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Symbolic distance affects two processing loci in the number comparison task. Memory & Cognition, 33, 913–926. Oriet, C., Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Symbolic distance affects two processing loci in the number comparison task. Memory & Cognition, 33, 913–926.
go back to reference Pashler, H. E. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377.PubMedCrossRef Pashler, H. E. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. E. (1994a). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.PubMedCrossRef Pashler, H. E. (1994a). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. E. (1994b). Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 330–342.PubMedCrossRef Pashler, H. E. (1994b). Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 330–342.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. E., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 41, 19–45. Pashler, H. E., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 41, 19–45.
go back to reference Pashler, H. E., Johnston, J. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 629–651.PubMedCrossRef Pashler, H. E., Johnston, J. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 629–651.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (Vol. 32, pp. 531–556). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (Vol. 32, pp. 531–556). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Pashler, H. E., & Klaassen, A. (2001). Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: Structural limitation or strategic postponement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 73–80. Ruthruff, E., Pashler, H. E., & Klaassen, A. (2001). Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: Structural limitation or strategic postponement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 73–80.
go back to reference Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2007). Automatic activation of task-related representations in task shifting. Memory & Cognition, 35, 138–155. Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2007). Automatic activation of task-related representations in task shifting. Memory & Cognition, 35, 138–155.
go back to reference Strayer, D. L., & Kramer, A. F. (1994). Strategies and automaticity: II Dynamic aspects of strategy adjustment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 342–365.CrossRef Strayer, D. L., & Kramer, A. F. (1994). Strategies and automaticity: II Dynamic aspects of strategy adjustment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 342–365.CrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Seiss, E., & Leuthold, H. (2005). Executive control in the Simon task: A dual-task examination of response priming and its suppression. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 590–618.CrossRef Stürmer, B., Seiss, E., & Leuthold, H. (2005). Executive control in the Simon task: A dual-task examination of response priming and its suppression. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 590–618.CrossRef
go back to reference Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.CrossRef Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590. Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 591–611. Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 591–611.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Jerreat, L. M., & Burak, A. L. (1994). Object-based and environment-based inhibition of return of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 478–499.PubMedCrossRef Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Jerreat, L. M., & Burak, A. L. (1994). Object-based and environment-based inhibition of return of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 478–499.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.PubMedCrossRef Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.PubMedCrossRef Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watter, S., & Logan, G. D. (2006). Parallel response selection in dual-task situations. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 254–277. Watter, S., & Logan, G. D. (2006). Parallel response selection in dual-task situations. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 254–277.
Metagegevens
Titel
Strategic capacity sharing between two tasks: evidence from tasks with the same and with different task sets
Auteurs
Carola Lehle
Ronald Hübner
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2009
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2009
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0162-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2009

Psychological Research 5/2009 Naar de uitgave