Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2010

01-07-2010 | Original Article

Stimulus–response bindings contribute to item switch costs in working memory

Auteurs: Markus Janczyk, Wilfried Kunde

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In counter updating tasks, responses are typically faster when items repeat than when they change (item switch costs). The present study explored the contribution of stimulus–response bindings to these item switch costs. In two experiments, we orthogonally manipulated the repetition/switch of to-be-counted items and the repetition/switch of required manual responses. Item switch costs were considerably lower when item switches were accompanied by response switches than when accompanied by response repetitions. Experiment 2 showed that, although there was also a smaller contribution from stimulus–stimulus bindings (i.e., shape-location), the major part was due to stimulus-response bindings. These results show that in the widely used standard version of the counter updating task, a considerable portion of item switch costs is caused by the unbinding of stimulus–response bindings rather than by processes of switching items in working memory.
Literatuur
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). New York, NY: Academic Press. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). New York, NY: Academic Press.
go back to reference Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191.CrossRefPubMed Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26, 263–276. Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26, 263–276.
go back to reference Gehring, W. J., Bryck, R. L., Jonides, J., Albin, R. L., & Badre, D. (2003). The mind’s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in attention shifting. Psychophysiology, 40, 572–585.CrossRefPubMed Gehring, W. J., Bryck, R. L., Jonides, J., Albin, R. L., & Badre, D. (2003). The mind’s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in attention shifting. Psychophysiology, 40, 572–585.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2002). Responding to object files: automatic integration of spatial information revealed by stimulus–response compatibility effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 567–580. Hommel, B. (2002). Responding to object files: automatic integration of spatial information revealed by stimulus–response compatibility effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 567–580.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2005). How much attention does an event file need? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1067–1082.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (2005). How much attention does an event file need? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1067–1082.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2007). Feature integration across perception and action: event files affect response choice. Psychological Research, 71, 42–63.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (2007). Feature integration across perception and action: event files affect response choice. Psychological Research, 71, 42–63.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Wienrich, C., & Kunde, W. (2008). On the costs of refocusing items in working memory: a matter of inhibition or decay? Memory, 16, 374–385.CrossRefPubMed Janczyk, M., Wienrich, C., & Kunde, W. (2008). On the costs of refocusing items in working memory: a matter of inhibition or decay? Memory, 16, 374–385.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.CrossRefPubMed Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Li, Z., Bao, M., Chen, X., Zhang, D., Han, S., He, S., et al. (2006). Attention shift in human verbal working memory: priming contribution and dynamic brain activation. Brain Research, 1078, 131–142.CrossRefPubMed Li, Z., Bao, M., Chen, X., Zhang, D., Han, S., He, S., et al. (2006). Attention shift in human verbal working memory: priming contribution and dynamic brain activation. Brain Research, 1078, 131–142.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McElree, B. (2001). Working memory and focal attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 817–835.CrossRefPubMed McElree, B. (2001). Working memory and focal attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 817–835.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 411–421.CrossRefPubMed Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 411–421.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oberauer, K. (2003). Selective attention to elements in working memory. Experimental Psychology, 50, 257–269.PubMed Oberauer, K. (2003). Selective attention to elements in working memory. Experimental Psychology, 50, 257–269.PubMed
go back to reference Oberauer, K. (2006). Is the focus of attention in working memory expanded through practice? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 197–214.CrossRefPubMed Oberauer, K. (2006). Is the focus of attention in working memory expanded through practice? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 197–214.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oberauer, K., & Bialkova, S. (2009). Accessing information in working memory: can the focus of attention grasp two elements at the same time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 64–87.CrossRef Oberauer, K., & Bialkova, S. (2009). Accessing information in working memory: can the focus of attention grasp two elements at the same time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 64–87.CrossRef
go back to reference Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2008). Speed and accuracy of accessing information in working memory: an individual differences investigation of focus switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 616–630.CrossRefPubMed Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2008). Speed and accuracy of accessing information in working memory: an individual differences investigation of focus switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 616–630.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., & Basak, C. (2004). A working memory workout: how to expand the focus of serial attention from one to four items in 10 hours or less. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 1322–1337.CrossRefPubMed Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., & Basak, C. (2004). A working memory workout: how to expand the focus of serial attention from one to four items in 10 hours or less. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 1322–1337.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.CrossRefPubMed Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Stimulus–response bindings contribute to item switch costs in working memory
Auteurs
Markus Janczyk
Wilfried Kunde
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0259-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2010

Psychological Research 4/2010 Naar de uitgave