Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 8/2019

23-04-2018 | Original Article

Starting or finishing sooner? Sequencing preferences in object transfer tasks

Auteurs: Lisa R. Fournier, Alexandra M. Stubblefield, Brian P. Dyre, David A. Rosenbaum

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

When tasks are performed, other tasks are postponed, at least implicitly. Little is known about how task sequencing is determined. We examined task sequencing in object transfer tasks for which either task could easily or logically come before the other. The task was to transfer ping pong balls from two buckets into a bowl. To perform the task, participants walked down a corridor, picked up one of two buckets (their choice), carried it to the end of the corridor, transferred the balls from the bucket into a bowl, carried the bucket back to the start position, and then did the same with the other remaining bucket. As in an earlier study where just one of two buckets had to be carried to the end of a corridor (Rosenbaum et al. Psychol Sci 25(7):1487–1496, 2014), participants showed a marked tendency to start with the near bucket. The near-bucket preference was modulated only to a small extent by the number of balls that could be emptied into the bowl. The relative lack of importance of the number of balls to be transferred (to finish the first task more quickly or to get closer to the end goal of transferring all balls into the bowl) was further demonstrated by the fact that the effect of the number of balls to be transferred did not depend on how the emptying was supposed to occur (by pouring the balls or placing the balls one at a time into the bowl), or by whether the instruction focused on filling the bowl or emptying the buckets. The results suggest that the near-bucket preference reflects a strong inclination to start the task (sub-goal) as soon as possible rather than complete the task (sub-goal) as soon as possible. Starting the task as soon as possible may be related to the affordance triggered by the sight of the near object or by the freedom to perform without having to inhibit a reach for a bucket when the performer is empty-handed. Starting a task sooner may free up cognitive resources for subsequent decision-making.
Voetnoten
1
The possible dynamics of ordering decisions have been modeled in detail and with considerable success (Botvinick & Plaut, 2004; Cooper & Shallice, 2000; Norman & Shallice, 1980, 1986), but with presupposed (normative) orders whose violations are odd or even pathological (Shallice, 1982; Schwartz et al., 1998). The focus here is on optional ordering, a topic that has been studied since Panini proposed his Law of Syllable Number (also known as Panini’s Law). For a review of and evidence for the psychological reality of this rule and related ones, see Pinker and Birdsong (1979) and MacDonald (2013). The latter author has proposed an “easy first” principle of language production which is not restricted to frozen idioms. Studies that have examined task ordering in the context of experiments on the psychological refractory period have made analogous conclusions; see Leonhard et al. (2011) and Ruiz Fernández et al. (2011; 2013).
 
2
There was a non-significant trend suggesting that the framing of the instructions may influence the interaction of right bucket relative distance and ball ratio in the pour group only. However, this trend did not compromise any of the interpretations of the effects above, and suggested that the probability of picking up the right bucket first trended toward an increase for the “fill bowl” instruction when the right bucket contained a larger number of balls relative to when the right and left bucket contained an equivalent number of balls. Importantly, we found no evidence that the “fill bowl” or “empty buckets” instructions differentially affected the parameter estimates for the one-by-one group. For this group, the instruction to “empty buckets” did not lead to a preference in selecting the bucket with the smaller (vs. larger) number of balls first, as the bucket with the smaller number of balls could more quickly satisfy the means of the goal to “empty the buckets”. Thus, the framing of the instructions did not change the outcome of the study.
 
3
Both of the women who discovered these effects, Bluma Zeigarnik and Maria Ovsiankina, were students of the Gestalt psychologist, Kurt Lewin, whose field theory they sought to test (or validate). According to field theory (Lewin, 1935), actors (people or animals) are drawn toward teleologically relevant attractors in the environment and are drawn away from teleologically relevant repellers in the environment. The findings from the present experiment, and the results of Rosenbaum et al (2014), can be viewed as consistent with Lewin’s field theory in that participants in both studies were apparently drawn to the nearest goal-relevant object.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Atwood, M. E., & Polson, P. G. (1976). A process model for water jug problems. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 191–216.CrossRef Atwood, M. E., & Polson, P. G. (1976). A process model for water jug problems. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 191–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., Pook, P. K., & Rao, R. P. (1997). Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(04), 723–742.CrossRef Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., Pook, P. K., & Rao, R. P. (1997). Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(04), 723–742.CrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111, 395–429.CrossRef Botvinick, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111, 395–429.CrossRef
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment., 48, 306–307.CrossRef Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment., 48, 306–307.CrossRef
go back to reference Cooper, R., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297–338.CrossRef Cooper, R., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297–338.CrossRef
go back to reference Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Umiltà, C. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Evidence for visuomotor priming effect. Neuroreport, 8(1), 347–349.CrossRef Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Umiltà, C. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Evidence for visuomotor priming effect. Neuroreport, 8(1), 347–349.CrossRef
go back to reference Dunlosky, J., & Ariel, R. (2011). The influence of agenda-based and habitual processes on item selection during study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 899–912.PubMed Dunlosky, J., & Ariel, R. (2011). The influence of agenda-based and habitual processes on item selection during study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 899–912.PubMed
go back to reference Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Harcourt Brace and World. Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Harcourt Brace and World.
go back to reference Lewin, K. (1926). Will and needs. Psychological Forces, 7, 294–385. Lewin, K. (1926). Will and needs. Psychological Forces, 7, 294–385.
go back to reference Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In CHIP Report 99. San Diego, CA: University of California. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In CHIP Report 99. San Diego, CA: University of California.
go back to reference Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum.
go back to reference Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302–379.CrossRef Ovsiankina, M. (1928). Die Wiederaufnahme unterbrochener Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 11, 302–379.CrossRef
go back to reference Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768–774. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1.CrossRef Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768–774. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1.CrossRef
go back to reference Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1988). Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 534–552. Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1988). Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 534–552.
go back to reference Pinker, S., & Birdsong, D. (1979). Speakers’ sensitivity to rules of frozen word order. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 497–508.CrossRef Pinker, S., & Birdsong, D. (1979). Speakers’ sensitivity to rules of frozen word order. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 497–508.CrossRef
go back to reference Rasmussen, J. (1987). Mental models and the control of action in complex environments. In D. Ackermann & M. J. Tanber (Eds.) Mental models and human–computer interaction (pp. 41–69), North-Holland: Elsevier. Rasmussen, J. (1987). Mental models and the control of action in complex environments. In D. Ackermann & M. J. Tanber (Eds.) Mental models and human–computer interaction (pp. 41–69), North-Holland: Elsevier.
go back to reference Schwartz, M. F., Montgomery, M. W., Buxbaum, L. J., Less, S. S., Carew, T. G., Coslett, H. B., Ferraro, M., Fitzpatrick-De Salme, E. J., Hart, T., & Mayer, N. H. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12, 13–28.CrossRef Schwartz, M. F., Montgomery, M. W., Buxbaum, L. J., Less, S. S., Carew, T. G., Coslett, H. B., Ferraro, M., Fitzpatrick-De Salme, E. J., Hart, T., & Mayer, N. H. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12, 13–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Shah, A. K., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Heuristics made easy: An effort-reduction framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 207–222.CrossRef Shah, A. K., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Heuristics made easy: An effort-reduction framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 207–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Paul, M., & Hayes, A. (2006). Vision-for action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 493–498.CrossRef Tipper, S. P., Paul, M., & Hayes, A. (2006). Vision-for action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 493–498.CrossRef
go back to reference Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.CrossRef Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.CrossRef
go back to reference Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85.CrossRef Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Starting or finishing sooner? Sequencing preferences in object transfer tasks
Auteurs
Lisa R. Fournier
Alexandra M. Stubblefield
Brian P. Dyre
David A. Rosenbaum
Publicatiedatum
23-04-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1022-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2019

Psychological Research 8/2019 Naar de uitgave