Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 7/2019

10-04-2018 | Original Article

Spatial transformation abilities and their relation to later mathematics performance

Auteur: Andrea Frick

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 7/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Using a longitudinal approach, this study investigated the relational structure of different spatial transformation skills at kindergarten age, and how these spatial skills relate to children’s later mathematics performance. Children were tested at three time points, in kindergarten, first grade, and second grade (N = 119). Exploratory factor analyses revealed two subcomponents of spatial transformation skills: one representing egocentric transformations (mental rotation and spatial scaling), and one representing allocentric transformations (e.g., cross-sectioning, perspective taking). Structural equation modeling suggested that egocentric transformation skills showed their strongest relation to the part of the mathematics test tapping arithmetic operations, whereas allocentric transformations were strongly related to Numeric-Logical and Spatial Functions as well as geometry. The present findings point to a tight connection between early mental transformation skills, particularly the ones requiring a high level of spatial flexibility and a strong sense for spatial magnitudes, and children’s mathematics performance at the beginning of their school career.
Voetnoten
1
Test–retest reliability measures are reported where available; however, for some of the tasks, such measures were unobtainable. Measures based on inter-item correlations are misleading in these tasks, as items differ considerably in difficulty. For example, accuracy on items requiring mental rotation by a large angle is significantly lower than items requiring only a small mental transformation (Frick et al., 2013). Nevertheless, for the sake of completeness, Guttman’s Lambda 2 was calculated in these cases and was as follows: Ghost Rotation, 0.70; Perspective Taking, 0.70; Cross-sectioning, 0.56; Children’s Mental Transformation Task, 0.75; Diagrammatic Representations, 0.69; Spatial Scaling, .70; Proportional Reasoning, 0.88; Number-line Task, 0.87. For span tasks (both working memory tasks) and timed tasks (Card Rotation, Geometry task), inter-item correlations are meaningless as children solved different numbers of items.
 
2
In the case of map reading, changes in scale typically go hand in hand with changes in representational format, as maps typically are scaled two-dimensional representations of a three-dimensional space. To disentangle these two transformations, the Spatial Scaling Task used in the present study did not involve any changes in format—conversely, the Diagrammatic Reasoning Task did not involve changes in scale.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Arbuckle, J. L. (2013). IBM SPSS Amos 22 user’s guide. Crawfordville: Amos Development Corporation. Arbuckle, J. L. (2013). IBM SPSS Amos 22 user’s guide. Crawfordville: Amos Development Corporation.
go back to reference Cohen, C. A., & Hegarty, M. (2007). Sources of difficulty in imagining cross sections of 3D objects. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 179–184). Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX. Cohen, C. A., & Hegarty, M. (2007). Sources of difficulty in imagining cross sections of 3D objects. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 179–184). Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX.
go back to reference Committeri, G., Galati, G., Paradis, A. L., Pizzamiglio, L., Berthoz, A., & LeBihan, D. (2004). Reference frames for spatial cognition: Different brain areas are involved in viewer-, object-, and landmark-centered judgments about object location. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1517–1535. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042568550.CrossRefPubMed Committeri, G., Galati, G., Paradis, A. L., Pizzamiglio, L., Berthoz, A., & LeBihan, D. (2004). Reference frames for spatial cognition: Different brain areas are involved in viewer-, object-, and landmark-centered judgments about object location. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1517–1535. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1162/​0898929042568550​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition , 1, 83–120. Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition , 1, 83–120.
go back to reference Haffner, J., Baro, K., Parzer, P., & Resch, F. (2005). Heidelberger Rechentest (HRT 1–4). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Haffner, J., Baro, K., Parzer, P., & Resch, F. (2005). Heidelberger Rechentest (HRT 1–4). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
go back to reference International Labour Office (Ed.). (1990). International standard classification of occupations. ISCO-1988. Geneva: International Labour Office. International Labour Office (Ed.). (1990). International standard classification of occupations. ISCO-1988. Geneva: International Labour Office.
go back to reference Klatzky, R. L. (1998). Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: Definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In C. Freksa, C. Habel & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition I—An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 1–17). Berlin: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-69342-4_1.CrossRef Klatzky, R. L. (1998). Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: Definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In C. Freksa, C. Habel & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition I—An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 1–17). Berlin: Springer. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​3-540-69342-4_​1.CrossRef
go back to reference Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind bring mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books. Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind bring mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Lennon, P. A. (2000). Improving students’ flexibility of closure while presenting biology content. American Biology Teacher, 62, 177–180. https://doi.org/10.1662/0002-7685(2000)062[0177:isfocw]2.0.co;2.CrossRef Lennon, P. A. (2000). Improving students’ flexibility of closure while presenting biology content. American Biology Teacher, 62, 177–180. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1662/​0002-7685(2000)062[0177:isfocw]2.0.co;2.CrossRef
go back to reference Lohman, D. F. (1979). Spatial ability: A review and reanalysis of the correlational literature. Aptitude Research Project (Technical Report No. 8). Stanford, CA: Stanford University, School of Education. Lohman, D. F. (1979). Spatial ability: A review and reanalysis of the correlational literature. Aptitude Research Project (Technical Report No. 8). Stanford, CA: Stanford University, School of Education. 
go back to reference Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2011). Origins and development of generalized magnitude representation. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, time, and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought (pp. 225–244). London: Elsevier.CrossRef Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2011). Origins and development of generalized magnitude representation. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, time, and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought (pp. 225–244). London: Elsevier.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Salzburger Lese-Screening für die Klassenstufen 1–4 (SLS 1–4). Bern: Hans Huber. Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Salzburger Lese-Screening für die Klassenstufen 1–4 (SLS 1–4). Bern: Hans Huber.
go back to reference Mix, K. S., Levine, S. C., Cheng, Y.-L., Young, C., Hambrick, D. Z., Ping, R., & Konstantopoulos, S. (2016). Separate but correlated: The latent structure of space and mathematics across development. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 1206–1227. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000182.CrossRef Mix, K. S., Levine, S. C., Cheng, Y.-L., Young, C., Hambrick, D. Z., Ping, R., & Konstantopoulos, S. (2016). Separate but correlated: The latent structure of space and mathematics across development. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 1206–1227. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​xge0000182.CrossRef
go back to reference National Research Council (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Committee on a Conceptual Framework for New K-12 Science Education Standards. Board on Science Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. National Research Council (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Committee on a Conceptual Framework for New K-12 Science Education Standards. Board on Science Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
go back to reference Newcombe, N. S. (2010). Picture this: Increasing math and science learning by improving spatial thinking. American Educator, 34, 29–43. Newcombe, N. S. (2010). Picture this: Increasing math and science learning by improving spatial thinking. American Educator, 34, 29–43.
go back to reference Newcombe, N. S., Möhring, W., & Frick, A. (in press). How big is many? Development of spatial and numerical magnitude understanding. In A. Henik & W. Fias (Eds.), Heterogeneity of function in numerical cognition. Newcombe, N. S., Möhring, W., & Frick, A. (in press). How big is many? Development of spatial and numerical magnitude understanding. In A. Henik & W. Fias (Eds.), Heterogeneity of function in numerical cognition.
go back to reference Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1948). Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1948).
go back to reference Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1971). Mental imagery in the child; a study of the development of imaginal representation (P. A. Chilton, Trans.). New York: Basic. (Original work published 1966). Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1971). Mental imagery in the child; a study of the development of imaginal representation (P. A. Chilton, Trans.). New York: Basic. (Original work published 1966).
go back to reference Ratliff, K. R., McGinnis, C. R., & Levine, S. C. (2010). The development and assessment of cross-sectioning ability in young children. Paper submitted to the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Portland, OR. Ratliff, K. R., McGinnis, C. R., & Levine, S. C. (2010). The development and assessment of cross-sectioning ability in young children. Paper submitted to the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Portland, OR.
go back to reference Ricken, G., Fritz, A., Schuck, K. D., & Preuss, U. (2007). HAWIVA-III: Hannover-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für das Vorschulalter – III. Bern: Huber. Ricken, G., Fritz, A., Schuck, K. D., & Preuss, U. (2007). HAWIVA-III: Hannover-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für das Vorschulalter – III. Bern: Huber.
go back to reference Schaie, K. W. (1985). Schaie-Thurstone adult mental abilities test. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Schaie, K. W. (1985). Schaie-Thurstone adult mental abilities test. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
go back to reference Thurstone, L. L. (1950). Some primary abilities in visual thinking (Rep. no. 59). Chicago, IL: Psychometric Laboratory, University of Chicago. Thurstone, L. L. (1950). Some primary abilities in visual thinking (Rep. no. 59). Chicago, IL: Psychometric Laboratory, University of Chicago.
Metagegevens
Titel
Spatial transformation abilities and their relation to later mathematics performance
Auteur
Andrea Frick
Publicatiedatum
10-04-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 7/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1008-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2019

Psychological Research 7/2019 Naar de uitgave