Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2022

06-10-2021 | Original Article

Spatial cueing effects do not always index attentional capture: evidence for a priority accumulation framework

Auteurs: Maya Darnell, Dominique Lamy

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2022

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The spatial cueing paradigm is a popular tool to investigate under what conditions irrelevant objects capture attention against the observer’s intention. In this paradigm, finding better visual search performance when the target appears at the location of an irrelevant cue is taken to indicate that this cue summoned attention to its location, before the search display appeared. Here, we provide evidence challenging this canonical interpretation of spatial-cueing (or cue-validity) effects and supporting the priority accumulation framework (PAF). According to PAF, the cue can bias attention but such bias takes effect only when the relevant context for selection (the search display) appears: attentional priority accumulates over time at each location until the search context triggers selection of the location that has accumulated the highest priority. We used a spatial-cueing paradigm with abruptly onset cues and search displays varying in target–distractor similarity. We found that search performance on valid-cue trials deteriorates the more difficult the search (Experiment 1), and showed that this finding is explained by PAF but cannot be accommodated within the standard interpretation of spatial-cueing effects (Experiment 2). Finally, we assessed the priority accumulated at each location by using a combination of the spatial-cueing and dot-probe paradigms (Experiment 3). We showed that the similarity of the cued object to the target modulates probe detection performance, a finding that is at odds with the standard interpretation of cueing effects and supports PAF’s predictions. We discuss the implications of the findings in resolving existing controversies on the determinants of attentional priority.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
In both Gaspelin et al.’s study (2016, Exp.7) and our replications of their experiment (e.g., Exp.1 of the present study), similarity to the target and surface size were confounded, because the target was larger than difficult distractors, which were larger than easy distractors. In addition, when the search display was presented briefly (in Exp.1), some participants reported that in the context of the horizontal ellipses, the perfect circle appeared to be a vertical ellipse. This illusion may have rendered the instruction to look for a perfect circle difficult to follow. To address these issues, all objects were horizontal ellipses occupying the same surface and varying in elongation. Participants were instructed to search for the ellipse that was closest to a circle.
 
2
Hilchey and Pratt (2019) also claimed to expose latent capture by onset cues through statistical learning. Relying on the notion of learned predictiveness (LePelley, Mitchell, Beesley, George & Wills, 2016), they reasoned that participants should pick up on the statistical regularities that exist between an onset cue and the target location only if this onset cue reliably captures attention. However, the learned predictiveness principle stipulates that “attention is biased toward stimuli that predict their consequences reliably” (LePelley et al., 2016) and not that associative learning is possible only for attended stimuli. Thus, it is unclear whether there was “latent capture” by the onset cue before learning occurred.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2012). Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(6), 1183–1198.CrossRef Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2012). Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(6), 1183–1198.CrossRef
go back to reference Barras, C., & Kerzel, D. (2017). Salient-but-irrelevant stimuli cause attentional capture in difficult, but attentional suppression in easy visual search. Psychophysiology, 54(12), 1826–1838.PubMedCrossRef Barras, C., & Kerzel, D. (2017). Salient-but-irrelevant stimuli cause attentional capture in difficult, but attentional suppression in easy visual search. Psychophysiology, 54(12), 1826–1838.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bundesen, C., Habekost, T., & Kyllingsbæk, S. (2005). A neural theory of visual attention: Bridging cognition and neurophysiology. Psychological Review, 112(2), 291–328.PubMedCrossRef Bundesen, C., Habekost, T., & Kyllingsbæk, S. (2005). A neural theory of visual attention: Bridging cognition and neurophysiology. Psychological Review, 112(2), 291–328.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Büsel, C., Voracek, M., & Ansorge, U. (2018). A meta-analysis of contingent-capture effects. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 1, 1–26. Büsel, C., Voracek, M., & Ansorge, U. (2018). A meta-analysis of contingent-capture effects. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 1, 1–26.
go back to reference Chen, P., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2007). Contingent capture at a very short SOA: Evidence against rapid disengagement. Visual Cognition, 15(6), 637–646.CrossRef Chen, P., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2007). Contingent capture at a very short SOA: Evidence against rapid disengagement. Visual Cognition, 15(6), 637–646.CrossRef
go back to reference Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: The neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7426–7435.PubMedCrossRef Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: The neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7426–7435.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193–222.PubMedCrossRef Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193–222.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Duncan, J., Ward, R., & Shapiro, K. (1994). Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision. Nature, 369(6478), 313–315.PubMedCrossRef Duncan, J., Ward, R., & Shapiro, K. (1994). Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision. Nature, 369(6478), 313–315.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48(1), 269–297.PubMedCrossRef Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48(1), 269–297.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Grubert, A. (2014). Spatial attention can be allocated rapidly and in parallel to new visual objects. Current Biology, 24(2), 193–198.PubMedCrossRef Eimer, M., & Grubert, A. (2014). Spatial attention can be allocated rapidly and in parallel to new visual objects. Current Biology, 24(2), 193–198.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1974). Selective attention: Noise suppression or signal enhancement? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4(6), 587–589.CrossRef Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1974). Selective attention: Noise suppression or signal enhancement? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4(6), 587–589.CrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, C. W., & Yeh, Y. Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(5), 583.PubMed Eriksen, C. W., & Yeh, Y. Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(5), 583.PubMed
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030.PubMed Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030.PubMed
go back to reference Gabbay, C., Zivony, A., & Lamy, D. (2019). Splitting the attentional spotlight? Evidence from attentional capture by successive events. Visual Cognition, 27(5–8), 518–536.CrossRef Gabbay, C., Zivony, A., & Lamy, D. (2019). Splitting the attentional spotlight? Evidence from attentional capture by successive events. Visual Cognition, 27(5–8), 518–536.CrossRef
go back to reference Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2015). Direct evidence for active suppression of salient-but-irrelevant sensory inputs. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1740–1750.PubMedCrossRef Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2015). Direct evidence for active suppression of salient-but-irrelevant sensory inputs. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1740–1750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., & Lien, M. C. (2016). The problem of latent attentional capture: Easy visual search conceals capture by task-irrelevant abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1104–1120.PubMed Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., & Lien, M. C. (2016). The problem of latent attentional capture: Easy visual search conceals capture by task-irrelevant abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1104–1120.PubMed
go back to reference Hilchey, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2019). Hidden from view: Statistical learning exposes latent attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 1633–1640.CrossRef Hilchey, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2019). Hidden from view: Statistical learning exposes latent attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 1633–1640.CrossRef
go back to reference Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43(4), 346–354.CrossRef Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43(4), 346–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, M. S., & Cave, K. R. (1995). Spatial attention in visual search for features and feature conjunctions. Psychological Science, 6(6), 376–380.CrossRef Kim, M. S., & Cave, K. R. (1995). Spatial attention in visual search for features and feature conjunctions. Psychological Science, 6(6), 376–380.CrossRef
go back to reference Lamy, D. (2021). The attentional capture debate: The long-lasting consequences of a misnomer. Visual Cognition, 29, 1–4.CrossRef Lamy, D. (2021). The attentional capture debate: The long-lasting consequences of a misnomer. Visual Cognition, 29, 1–4.CrossRef
go back to reference Lamy, D. (2005). Temporal expectations modulate attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 1112–1119.CrossRef Lamy, D. (2005). Temporal expectations modulate attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 1112–1119.CrossRef
go back to reference Lamy, D., Darnell, M., Levi, A., & Bublil, C. (2018). Testing the attentional dwelling hypothesis of attentional capture. Journal of Cognition, 1, 1.CrossRef Lamy, D., Darnell, M., Levi, A., & Bublil, C. (2018). Testing the attentional dwelling hypothesis of attentional capture. Journal of Cognition, 1, 1.CrossRef
go back to reference Lamy, D., Tsal, Y., & Egeth, H. E. (2003). Does a salient distractor capture attention early in processing? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 621–629.CrossRef Lamy, D., Tsal, Y., & Egeth, H. E. (2003). Does a salient distractor capture attention early in processing? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 621–629.CrossRef
go back to reference Luck, S. J., Girelli, M., McDermott, M. T., & Ford, M. A. (1997). Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: An ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 64–87.PubMedCrossRef Luck, S. J., Girelli, M., McDermott, M. T., & Ford, M. A. (1997). Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: An ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 64–87.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Luck, S. J., Gaspelin, N., Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Theeuwes, J. (2021). Progress toward resolving the attentional capture debate. Visual Cognition, 29(1), 1–21.PubMedCrossRef Luck, S. J., Gaspelin, N., Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Theeuwes, J. (2021). Progress toward resolving the attentional capture debate. Visual Cognition, 29(1), 1–21.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moore, C. M., Egeth, H., Berglan, L. R., & Luck, S. J. (1996). Are attentional dwell times inconsistent with serial visual search? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(3), 360–365.CrossRef Moore, C. M., Egeth, H., Berglan, L. R., & Luck, S. J. (1996). Are attentional dwell times inconsistent with serial visual search? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(3), 360–365.CrossRef
go back to reference Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Reason, 4(2), 61–64. Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Reason, 4(2), 61–64.
go back to reference Nobre, A. C. (2010). How can temporal expectations bias perception and action? Attention and Time, 2010, 371–392.CrossRef Nobre, A. C. (2010). How can temporal expectations bias perception and action? Attention and Time, 2010, 371–392.CrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Ogden, W. C. (1978). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. Modes of Perceiving and Processing Information, 137(158), 2. Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Ogden, W. C. (1978). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. Modes of Perceiving and Processing Information, 137(158), 2.
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(2), 160.CrossRef Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(2), 160.CrossRef
go back to reference Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 446–461.PubMedCrossRef Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 446–461.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Faulks, M., Maxwell, J. W., & Gaspelin, N. (2020). Attentional dwelling and capture by color singletons. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 3048–3064.CrossRef Ruthruff, E., Faulks, M., Maxwell, J. W., & Gaspelin, N. (2020). Attentional dwelling and capture by color singletons. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 3048–3064.CrossRef
go back to reference Tsal, Y. (1983). Movement of attention across the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(4), 523.PubMed Tsal, Y. (1983). Movement of attention across the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(4), 523.PubMed
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99.PubMedCrossRef Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (2018). Visual selection: Usually fast and automatic; seldom slow and volitional. Journal of Cognition, 2018, 14. Theeuwes, J. (2018). Visual selection: Usually fast and automatic; seldom slow and volitional. Journal of Cognition, 2018, 14.
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance, 18, 105–124. Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance, 18, 105–124.
go back to reference Toledano, D., Sasi, M., Yuval-Greenberg, S. & Lamy, D. (2021). On the timing of attentional deployment: Eye-movement evidence for a priority accumulation framework. In Poster Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Israel Conference of Cognitive Psychology (ISCOP). Toledano, D., Sasi, M., Yuval-Greenberg, S. & Lamy, D. (2021). On the timing of attentional deployment: Eye-movement evidence for a priority accumulation framework. In Poster Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Israel Conference of Cognitive Psychology (ISCOP).
go back to reference Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems. Cognitive Theory, 3, 199–139. Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems. Cognitive Theory, 3, 199–139.
go back to reference Vandierendonck, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the binning procedure. Behavior Research Methods, 49(2), 653–673.PubMedCrossRef Vandierendonck, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the binning procedure. Behavior Research Methods, 49(2), 653–673.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watson, D. G., & Humphreys, G. W. (2000). Visual marking: Evidence for inhibition using a probe-dot detection paradigm. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(3), 471–481.CrossRef Watson, D. G., & Humphreys, G. W. (2000). Visual marking: Evidence for inhibition using a probe-dot detection paradigm. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(3), 471–481.CrossRef
go back to reference Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature, 400(6747), 867.PubMedCrossRef Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature, 400(6747), 867.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601.PubMed Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601.PubMed
go back to reference Yaron, I., & Lamy, D. (2021). Spatial cueing effects are not what we thought: On the timing of attentional deployment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47(7), 946.PubMed Yaron, I., & Lamy, D. (2021). Spatial cueing effects are not what we thought: On the timing of attentional deployment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47(7), 946.PubMed
go back to reference Zelinsky, G. J., & Bisley, J. W. (2015). The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1339(1), 154.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Zelinsky, G. J., & Bisley, J. W. (2015). The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1339(1), 154.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Spatial cueing effects do not always index attentional capture: evidence for a priority accumulation framework
Auteurs
Maya Darnell
Dominique Lamy
Publicatiedatum
06-10-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2022
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01597-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2022

Psychological Research 5/2022 Naar de uitgave