Migration Background
The operationalization of migration background can be done using different approaches and also different “degrees” of migration background (e.g., first generation, second generation, etc.) (see e.g., Frank et al.,
2018). For example, Kemper (
2017) has shown, for example, that different operationalization methods are used for different school statistics in Germany. These different operationalizations also depend on the federal states within Germany and can differ between them. In order to be able to classify study results, it is therefore important to clearly explain which operationalization method a paper is based on and why.
According to the German Federal Statistical Office, a person is deemed to have a migration background if he or she or at least one parent was not born with a German citizenship (Destatis – Statistisches Bundesamt,
2021a). However, an additional distinction is made here between migration background in the narrower sense (information from parents living in the same household is used) and in the broader sense (all information about parents is used) (Destatis – Statistisches Bundesamt,
2021b). According to these definitions, around 21.2 million people with a migration background in the broader sense lived in Germany in 2019 (Destatis – Statistisches Bundesamt,
2020a). However, this operationalization method raises the problem that the treatment of a child with one native and one foreign-born parent is reduced to the characteristics of the foreign-born parent, and the child then also acquires the status of a migration background. The literature criticizes the existing official definition which focuses on “inherited citizenship” and questions why the status of the native-born parent is not given greater priority (Will,
2019). Moreover, and next to the problems raised in the context of research (limited comparability of study results due to different operationalizations of a migration background), the literature also reports that the number of people with a migration background can be underestimated if operationalization is based on nationality (see Sarcletti,
2015).
As mentioned above, different approaches to defining a migration background are proposed in the literature (see also e.g., Kemper,
2010). Ultimately, however, there is no uniform definition of a migration background in the literature and research. One of the approaches suggested takes linguistic aspects, such as the first language learned, as a possible means for characterization (see e.g., Kemper,
2010).
In this study, the characterization as a student with a migration background was based on linguistic aspects (the first learned language). This was done for two main reasons. In addition to avoiding the stigmatization of a child with a migration background due to only one parent having been born abroad (Will,
2019), the construct of social participation is primarily based on interaction and thus strongly reliant on communication. Furthermore, it should be taken into consideration that the first (learned) language (of a child) is probably that of the parent who is/was the primary caregiver – and therefore also plays a major role in terms of socialization. Considering a linguistic aspect (in the sense of communication) to determine the migration background in the context of social participation as a whole therefore appeared to be particularly expedient.
Social Participation
The literature does not offer a precise definition of social participation. However, Koster et al. (
2009) examined the concepts of social participation, social integration, and social inclusion more closely in a literature review (62 articles). In their theoretical investigation, they came to the conclusion that these concepts were largely described inaccurately in the literature, hardly any clear definitions could be found, and, in many articles, only implicit descriptions (e.g., through the measuring instruments used) were given. They found that the constructs are rather used synonymously. Koster et al. (
2009) argue that the concept of social participation is the most suitable one (see also Bossaert et al.,
2013) and Schwab (
2016) or Schwalb and Theunissen (
2018) point(s) out that social participation goes hand in hand with active involvement as well as a concrete right of co-determination. In this context, Koster et al. (
2009) report four key dimensions in their heuristic deduction from the considered papers on social participation (based on the systematic review): “friendships and relationships,” “interactions and contacts,” “peer acceptance,” and “self-perception of social inclusion” (see also Bossaert et al.,
2013).
Many quantitative studies operationalize friendships and relationships using sociometric nominations, which means that friendships are usually determined by nominating three (Mamas,
2012) to five best friends (Avramidis et al.,
2017; Pijl et al.,
2011). Here, positive mutual nominations by two students (reciprocal friends) are seen as particularly emotionally supportive and as a greater resource compared to non-reciprocal friendships (Vaquera & Kao,
2008). However, it must be taken into account here that studies mostly examine mutual friendship (nomination) within a specific group (e.g., within one classroom). Mutual friendships outside this specific group (e.g., friendships with students of another class) can only be identified to a limited extent. Therefore, this must always be considered in this context.
Interaction and contacts refer to verbal and non-verbal communication (Carter et al.,
2007). They are determined by means of (working) time spent together or, for example, social isolation (see Bossaert et al.,
2013).
Peer acceptance, on the other hand, is commonly defined as social rejection, social preference, and social support, but also bullying (Bossaert et al.
2013) and is recorded via sociometric nomination/ratings (Avramidis et al.,
2017). For example, each student in a class could be given a class list on which he or she is asked to indicate how much he or she would prefer (social preference) or reject (social rejection) each classmate sitting next to him or her (see e.g., Huber,
2011; Krawinkel et al.,
2018).
Self-perception of social inclusion, however, is not only concerned with the self-concept of the student but also, for example, with emotions like loneliness (Bossaert et al.,
2013). This last dimension of social participation is therefore mainly characterized by the self-perception of a student, whereas the first three dimensions are based on the peers’ view of a specific student.
Social Participation of Students with a Migration Background
In order to provide an overview of the current literature, the four different dimensions of social participation addressed below, with special consideration given to students with a migration background:
Graham et al. (
2009) report that students prefer peers of the same ethnic origin rather than students of other ethnic origins as friends. In this context, an explanation can be seen through the homophily effect (preference for individuals with similar or comparable characteristics; see e.g., Graham et al.,
2009; Leszczensky & Pink,
2015; Hoffmann et al.,
2021). However, the current literature also shows that preferences in the context of the homophily effect clearly go beyond ethnicity, as Campigotto et al. (
2021) describe in their study. According to the authors’ research, gender, among other things, but also cultural characteristics seem to be important here. For students born abroad, country of birth, generational status and religion seem to be important.
In addition, Darmody et al. (
2016) show that the number of friendships varies among different ethnicities. They found that individuals of Eastern European descent or Asian children are more likely to have fewer friends compared to other nationalities. However, in this context, the authors also report on different participation among children with different migration backgrounds, taking into account different contexts (e.g., sport/fitness club, cultural activities, etc.). In their considerations, they discuss, for example, that sports participation could be due to different sports traditions in the individual countries or a lack of knowledge about local facilities. Thus, the authors emphasize the importance of the specific context here.
Beyond that, Kokkonen et al. (
2015) show that individuals are more likely to be friends with immigrants if they work at diverse workplaces (compared to homogenous workplaces). Similar results may be expected for students in the classroom context. Also, the time spent in an immigrant country seems to play a crucial role for inclusion. Gabrielli et al. (
2013) showed that students with a migration background gained more and more native friends over time.
Furthermore, recent studies, such as those by Wahl et al. (
accepted), report that students with a migration background receive slightly fewer positive sociometric nominations, whereas the negative nominations are significantly higher compared to their peers without a migration background. Comparable results can also be found in a study by Krawinkel et al. (
2018). In this study, migrant children had a lower electoral status (social preference) and a higher rejection status than non-immigrant children. In addition, previous studies have shown that children with a migration background are more often affected by negative nominations than by positive ones (Kronig et al.,
2000). Plenty and Jonsson (
2017) investigated the isolation of students more precisely and show that especially students of the first migration generation are affected by isolation, compared to students of the second one (the educational level of the parents did not have any effect). Furthermore, in contrast to the number of students with a migration background, the general class size does have an influence on rejection; the probability of rejection is reduced with increasing class size.
With regard to social acceptance, elementary school students with a migration background have a lower social acceptance and a higher social rejection (Krull et al.,
2018). These findings are also consistent with the study by Wahl et al. (
accepted). However, the acceptance of students also seems to depend on class composition. For instance, Asendorpf and Motti-Stefanidi (
2017) pointed out in a longitudinal study that students with a migration background receive fewer positive and more negative nominations than their classmates without a migration background. They also observed that this was not the case in classes with a proportion of pupils with a migration background of more than two-thirds. In those classes, students with a migration background found more acceptance among their peers at the beginning of the study. Over the three-years course of their study, the initially stronger rejection directed at students with a migrant background lessened – regardless of the proportion of migration-background students within the class. Even in classes with a lower proportion, students with a migration background received increasingly positive nominations (Asendorpf & Motti-Stefanidi,
2017). So, a high number of children with a migration background in class, but also being female, having higher cognitive abilities, coming from a household with a higher income and having parents with a higher educational status than their classmates seems to reduce rejection (Plenty & Jonsson,
2017). Furthermore, it shows that the risk for students with a migration background of the first generation is higher than for students of the second generation – regardless of their origin (Plenty & Jonsson,
2017). More precisely, Plenty and Jonsson (
2017) reported that second-generation students are less affected by victimization. The research basis for victimization of students with a migration background is, however, mixed: some studies report that children with a migration background are bullied more often than their peers (Hjern et al.,
2013; Sulkowski et al.,
2014). Whereas, for example, Fandrem, et al. (
2009) or Hamel et al. (
2021) did not find any differences in the level of victimization. However, the exact migration background also seems to be important in this context. Stefanek et al. (
2012) report that students of a Turkish origin are less affected by victimization compared to (Austrian) native students, but also compared to students of other origins (e.g., the former Yugoslavia). Likewise, Strohmeier et al. (
2008) report that minorities are less often identified as victims than majority students.
To the best of the authors’ knowledge, there is currently little research on the self-perceived social participation rate, especially of students with a migration background. Rich Madsen et al. (
2016), however, outline that immigrant adolescents are more likely to feel negative emotions like loneliness than adolescents without such a background; the same could not be shown for the descendants of immigrants.
Finally, and in addition to the findings already described, there are also indications in the literature of gender-related differences in the areas of social participation. For example, Wittek et al. (
2020) report that, in addition to ethnic origin, gender also seems to have an influence on friendships. This is corroborated by the study by Huber et al. (
2018) which showed that boys rated children without a migration background more positively in their comparison of social acceptance of children with and without a migration background. In turn, girls rated both groups similarly. Similar results can also be found in longitudinal studies (e.g., Von Marées & Petermann,
2010).
In summary, the presented findings show that students with a migration background seem to be at risk of being disadvantaged in their social participation. Also taking into account later effects, this seems to be a cause for concern. Resch et al. (
2021), for example, examine social inclusion in the context of academic stays abroad during higher education studies. On the one hand, they describe that students with a migration background report a lower level of social inclusion, but on the other hand also that students (also with a migration background) who report a high level of social inclusion (among others with their peers) at the beginning of their studies are more likely to study abroad later. This also shows the possibility of impairments in later (working) life that are related to a lower level of social inclusion.
However, the perspectives of the available literature focus primarily on selected sub-dimensions of social participation. The simultaneous consideration of all four target dimensions of social participation of students with a migration background (based on one sample) is not yet available at the present time to the best of the authors’ knowledge. It is also unclear whether and to what extent the available results of the literature can be transferred to Germany.