Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2015

01-09-2015 | Original Article

Simple arithmetic: evidence of an inhibitory mechanism to select arithmetic facts

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In two experiments we evaluated the coactivation of arithmetic facts and the possible inhibitory mechanism used to select the correct one. To this end, we introduced an adapted version of the negative priming paradigm in which participants received additions and they decided whether they were correct or not. When the addition was incorrect but the result was that of multiplying the operands (e.g., 2 + 4 = 8), participants took more time to respond relative to control additions with unrelated results. This finding corroborated that participants coactivated arithmetic facts of multiplications even when they were irrelevant to perform the task. Moreover, the participants were slower to respond to an addition whose result was that of multiplying the operands of the previous trial (e.g., 2 + 6 = 8). These results support the existence of an inhibitory mechanism involved in the selection of arithmetic facts.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The analysis of second trial depending on the type of first trial. It is important to note that the type of second trial (multiplication 2 vs. unrelated 2) could not be analyzed depending on the type of first trial (multiplication 1 vs. unrelated 1) due to a repetition effect that might have a different impact on the two conditions of trial 2. For example, while the solution 8 is repeated in the multiplication 2 condition: 2 + 6 = 8, after the multiplication 1 condition 2 + 4 = 8; the solution 10 is repeated in the unrelated 2 condition 4 + 6 = 10 after the unrelated 1 condition 2 + 4 = 10. Note, however, that this unbalanced repetition effect is avoided when multiplication 2 and unrelated 2 conditions are directly compared since in both conditions, half of the solutions were explicitly presented in the previous trial. Nevertheless, we performed additional analyses to evaluate the influence of Trial 1 on Trial 2 by avoiding the unbalanced repetition effect. Firstly, the Trial 1 (multiplication 1, unrelated 1 condition) x Trial 2 (multiplication 2, unrelated 2) interaction was significant, F(1,47) = 17.55, p < 0.05, η 2 = 0.27. Afterward, we compared the two conditions that involved repetition: (a) multiplication 1–multiplication 2 condition vs. (b) unrelated 1–unrelated 2 condition. The RTs in Trial 2 were 100 ms slower in (a) vs. (b), F(1,47) = 6.18, p < 0.05, η 2 = 0.12, suggesting that, in spite of the repetition effect, Trial 2 was difficult to perform when it included the result of multiplying the operands of Trial 1. However, the comparison between (c) multiplication 1–unrelated 2 vs. (d) unrelated 1 vs. related 2 was not significant, F(1,47) = 2.71, p > 0.05, η 2 = 0.05 (57 ms difference). These two conditions did not involve repeating the result. Hence, the consequences of inhibiting the result of multiplying the operands of Trial 1 were only evident in Trial 2 when the result was visually present in both trials, the (a) condition.
 
2
Possible differences due to the gender of participants were examined. In Experiment 1, the results obtained in the first trial did not show differences between females and males, F(1,46) = 1.61, p > 0.05, η 2 = 0.03, and the Gender × Type of addition result was not significant F < 1. In the second trial, Gender was not a significant variable, F(1,46) = 2.35, p > 0.05, η 2 = 0.05, nor this variable interacted with type of results of previous trial, F < 1. In Experiment 2, the results of trial 1 did not show a significant effect of Gender, F < 1, and this variable did not interact with type of addition results, F < 1. In the second trial, Gender was not significant, F(1,31) = 1.12, p > 0.05, η 2 = 0.03, and this variable did not interact with type of result of previous trial, F(1,31) = 1.20, p > 0.05, η 2 = 0.04. Hence, there were no differences due to the gender of participants in this study.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ashcraft, M. H. (1987). Children’s knowledge of simple arithmetic: A developmental model and simulation. In J. Bisanz, C.J. Brainerd, & R. Kail (Eds.), Formal methods in developmental psychology: Progress in cognitive development research (pp. 302–338). New York: Springer. Ashcraft, M. H. (1987). Children’s knowledge of simple arithmetic: A developmental model and simulation. In J. Bisanz, C.J. Brainerd, & R. Kail (Eds.), Formal methods in developmental psychology: Progress in cognitive development research (pp. 302–338). New York: Springer.
go back to reference Campbell, J., & Graham, D. J. (1985). Mental multiplication skill: structure, process and acquisition. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 338–366. doi:10.1037/h0080065.CrossRef Campbell, J., & Graham, D. J. (1985). Mental multiplication skill: structure, process and acquisition. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 338–366. doi:10.​1037/​h0080065.CrossRef
go back to reference Campbell, J., & Thompson, V. (2012). Retrieval–induced forgetting of arithmetic facts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 118–129. doi:10.1037/a0025056.CrossRefPubMed Campbell, J., & Thompson, V. (2012). Retrieval–induced forgetting of arithmetic facts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 118–129. doi:10.​1037/​a0025056.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Censabella, S., & Noël, M. P. (2004). Interference in arithmetic facts: are active suppression processes involved when performing simple mental arithmetic? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 635–671. Censabella, S., & Noël, M. P. (2004). Interference in arithmetic facts: are active suppression processes involved when performing simple mental arithmetic? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 635–671.
go back to reference Colomé, A., Bafalluy, M. G., & Noël, M. P. (2011). Getting to the source: a questionnaire on the learning and use of arithmetical operations. Psicológica, 32, 223–253. Colomé, A., Bafalluy, M. G., & Noël, M. P. (2011). Getting to the source: a questionnaire on the learning and use of arithmetical operations. Psicológica, 32, 223–253.
go back to reference Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., & Ebner, F. (2013). The function of the left angular gyrus in mental arithmetic: evidence from the associative confusion effect. Human Brain Mapping, 24, 1013–1024. doi:10.1002/hbm.21489.CrossRef Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., & Ebner, F. (2013). The function of the left angular gyrus in mental arithmetic: evidence from the associative confusion effect. Human Brain Mapping, 24, 1013–1024. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​21489.CrossRef
go back to reference Groen, G. J., & Parkman, J. M. (1972). A chronometric analysis of simple addition. Psychological Review, 79, 329–343.CrossRef Groen, G. J., & Parkman, J. M. (1972). A chronometric analysis of simple addition. Psychological Review, 79, 329–343.CrossRef
go back to reference Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z. (2006). Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 119–135. doi:10.1017/S1366728906002483.CrossRef Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z. (2006). Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 119–135. doi:10.​1017/​S136672890600248​3.CrossRef
go back to reference LeFreve, J.-A., Sadesky, G. S., & Bisanz, J. (1996). Selection of procedures in mental addition: reassessing the problem size effect in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 216–230. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.216.CrossRef LeFreve, J.-A., Sadesky, G. S., & Bisanz, J. (1996). Selection of procedures in mental addition: reassessing the problem size effect in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 216–230. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​22.​1.​216.CrossRef
go back to reference Lemaire, P., Fayol, M., & Abdi, H. (1991). Associative confusion effect in cognitive arithmetic: evidence for partially autonomous processes. Current Psychology of Cognition, 11, 587–604. Lemaire, P., Fayol, M., & Abdi, H. (1991). Associative confusion effect in cognitive arithmetic: evidence for partially autonomous processes. Current Psychology of Cognition, 11, 587–604.
go back to reference Macizo, P., Bajo, T., & Martín, M. (2010). Inhibitory processes in bilingual language comprehension: evidence from Spanish–English interlexical homographs. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 232–244. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.002.CrossRef Macizo, P., Bajo, T., & Martín, M. (2010). Inhibitory processes in bilingual language comprehension: evidence from Spanish–English interlexical homographs. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 232–244. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jml.​2010.​04.​002.CrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E–Prime user´s guide (Version 1.1). Pittsburg: Psychology Software Tools. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E–Prime user´s guide (Version 1.1). Pittsburg: Psychology Software Tools.
go back to reference Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: how do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229–293). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: how do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229–293). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., & Driver, J. (1988). Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli. Memory and Cognition, 16, 64–70.CrossRefPubMed Tipper, S. P., & Driver, J. (1988). Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli. Memory and Cognition, 16, 64–70.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Whalen, J. (2000). The influence of semantic number representations on arithmetic fact retrieval. PhD thesis, Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore. Whalen, J. (2000). The influence of semantic number representations on arithmetic fact retrieval. PhD thesis, Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore.
go back to reference Winkelman, J. H., & Schmidt, J. (1974). Associative confusions in mental arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 734–736.CrossRef Winkelman, J. H., & Schmidt, J. (1974). Associative confusions in mental arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 734–736.CrossRef
go back to reference Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (1986). On the autonomy of mental processes: a case study of arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 115, 118–131.CrossRefPubMed Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (1986). On the autonomy of mental processes: a case study of arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 115, 118–131.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (2005). What everyone finds: the problem size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 331–346). New York: Psychology Press. Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (2005). What everyone finds: the problem size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 331–346). New York: Psychology Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Simple arithmetic: evidence of an inhibitory mechanism to select arithmetic facts
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2015
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0603-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2015

Psychological Research 5/2015 Naar de uitgave