Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2020

02-04-2019 | Original Article

Shared mechanisms underlying the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects

Auteurs: Chunming Luo, Robert W. Proctor

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A left or right keypress response to a relevant stimulus attribute (e.g., color) is faster when irrelevant left or right stimulus-location information corresponds with the correct response than when it does not. This phenomenon, known as the Simon effect, is obtained not only for physical locations, but also location words “left” and “right” and left- or right-pointing arrows. However, these location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects show different patterns in the reaction-time (RT) distributions, as evident in delta plots. In the present study, we employed procedures, analysis of survival curves and divergence point analysis, which have not previously been applied to the Simon effect, to investigate differences in time course of these various Simon effects in more detail. Also, we examined whether the diffusion model for conflict tasks (DMC), which assumes that automatic activation of task-irrelevant information occurs in a pulse-like function, can capture not only features of the RT distributions for the location-based Simon effect, to which it has been fit previously, but also features of the word- and arrow-based Simon effects, to which it has not. Results showed different survival curves and earliest, maximum, and latest divergence points for the three Simon effects, but DMC was able to capture the basic features of the RT distributions reflected by delta plot and survival curves for all effects. The results imply that the location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects have shared mechanisms, although they have different RT distributions.
Literatuur
go back to reference Ando, E., Matsuki, K., Sheridan, H., & Jared, D. (2015). The locus of Katakana-English masked phonological priming effects. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,18, 101–117. Ando, E., Matsuki, K., Sheridan, H., & Jared, D. (2015). The locus of Katakana-English masked phonological priming effects. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,18, 101–117.
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,30, 365–377.PubMed Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,30, 365–377.PubMed
go back to reference Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Influence of temporal overlap on time course of the Simon effect. Experimental Psychology,59, 88–98. Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Influence of temporal overlap on time course of the Simon effect. Experimental Psychology,59, 88–98.
go back to reference Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 619–641. Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 619–641.
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,20, 731–750.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,20, 731–750.PubMed
go back to reference Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1994). An introduction to the bootstrap. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall. Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1994). An introduction to the bootstrap. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall.
go back to reference Ellinghaus, R., Karlbauer, M., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2018). On the time-course of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Psychological Research,82, 734–743.PubMed Ellinghaus, R., Karlbauer, M., Bausenhart, K. M., & Ulrich, R. (2018). On the time-course of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Psychological Research,82, 734–743.PubMed
go back to reference Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics,29, 885–891. Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics,29, 885–891.
go back to reference Hoedemaker, R. S., & Gordon, P. C. (2017). The onset and time course of semantic priming during rapid recognition of visual words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,43, 881–902.PubMed Hoedemaker, R. S., & Gordon, P. C. (2017). The onset and time course of semantic priming during rapid recognition of visual words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,43, 881–902.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research,55, 280–290. Hommel, B. (1993). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research,55, 280–290.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research,56, 261–268.PubMed Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research,56, 261–268.PubMed
go back to reference Khalid, S., & Ansorge, U. (2013). The simon effect of spatial words in eye movements: Comparison of vertical and horizontal effects and of eye and finger responses. Vision Research, 86, 6–14.PubMed Khalid, S., & Ansorge, U. (2013). The simon effect of spatial words in eye movements: Comparison of vertical and horizontal effects and of eye and finger responses. Vision Research, 86, 6–14.PubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility–a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility–a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology,12, 523–553.PubMed Logan, G. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology,12, 523–553.PubMed
go back to reference Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,2, 174–207.PubMed Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,2, 174–207.PubMed
go back to reference Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Influence of irrelevant information on human performance: Effects of S–R association strength and relative timing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,54, 95–136.PubMed Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Influence of irrelevant information on human performance: Effects of S–R association strength and relative timing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,54, 95–136.PubMed
go back to reference Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2017). How different location modes influence responses in a Simon-like task. Psychological Research,81, 1125–1134.PubMed Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2017). How different location modes influence responses in a Simon-like task. Psychological Research,81, 1125–1134.PubMed
go back to reference Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2018b). The location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects: An ex-Gaussian analysis. Memory and Cognition,46, 497–506.PubMed Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2018b). The location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects: An ex-Gaussian analysis. Memory and Cognition,46, 497–506.PubMed
go back to reference Miles, J. D., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Correlations between spatial compatibility effects: Are arrows more like locations or words? Psychological Research,76, 777–791.PubMed Miles, J. D., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Correlations between spatial compatibility effects: Are arrows more like locations or words? Psychological Research,76, 777–791.PubMed
go back to reference Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary. (1985). Beijing, People’s Republic of China: Beijing Language Institute Press. Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary. (1985). Beijing, People’s Republic of China: Beijing Language Institute Press.
go back to reference Nelder, B. J. A., & Mead, R. (1964). A simplex method for function minimization. The Computer Journal, 7, 308–313. Nelder, B. J. A., & Mead, R. (1964). A simplex method for function minimization. The Computer Journal, 7, 308–313.
go back to reference Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: A time-course analysis. Experimental Psychology,56, 219–227.PubMed Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: A time-course analysis. Experimental Psychology,56, 219–227.PubMed
go back to reference Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics,72, 2013–2025. Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics,72, 2013–2025.
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Marble, J. G., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2000). Mixing incompatibly mapped location-relevant trials with location-irrelevant trials: Effects of stimulus mode on the reverse Simon effect. Psychological Research,64, 11–24.PubMed Proctor, R. W., Marble, J. G., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2000). Mixing incompatibly mapped location-relevant trials with location-irrelevant trials: Effects of stimulus mode on the reverse Simon effect. Psychological Research,64, 11–24.PubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,18, 242–266.PubMed Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,18, 242–266.PubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Yamaguchi, M., Zhang, Y., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2009). Influence of visual stimulus mode on transfer of acquired spatial associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,35, 434–445.PubMed Proctor, R. W., Yamaguchi, M., Zhang, Y., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2009). Influence of visual stimulus mode on transfer of acquired spatial associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,35, 434–445.PubMed
go back to reference Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review,85, 59–108. Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review,85, 59–108.
go back to reference Ratcliff, R., & Smith, P. L. (2004). A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. Psychological Review,111, 333–367.PubMedPubMedCentral Ratcliff, R., & Smith, P. L. (2004). A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. Psychological Review,111, 333–367.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model: Current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,20, 260–281.PubMedPubMedCentral Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model: Current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,20, 260–281.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Reingold, E. M., Reichle, E. D., Glaholt, M. G., & Sheridan, H. (2012). Direct lexical control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Cognitive Psychology,65, 177–206.PubMedPubMedCentral Reingold, E. M., Reichle, E. D., Glaholt, M. G., & Sheridan, H. (2012). Direct lexical control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of fixation durations. Cognitive Psychology,65, 177–206.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2018). On using distributional analysis techniques for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 260–271. Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2018). On using distributional analysis techniques for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 260–271.
go back to reference Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2014). Estimating the divergence point: A novel distributional analysis procedure for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 1–10. Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2014). Estimating the divergence point: A novel distributional analysis procedure for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 1–10.
go back to reference Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2018). On using distributional analysis techniques for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,71, 260–271. Reingold, E. M., & Sheridan, H. (2018). On using distributional analysis techniques for determining the onset of the influence of experimental variables. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,71, 260–271.
go back to reference Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports,7, 17815.PubMedPubMedCentral Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports,7, 17815.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Schwarz, W., & Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,19, 555–574.PubMed Schwarz, W., & Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,19, 555–574.PubMed
go back to reference Servant, M., White, C., Montagnini, A., & Burle, B. (2016). Linking theoretical decision-making mechanisms in the Simon task with electrophysiological data: A model-based neuroscience study in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,28, 1501–1521.PubMed Servant, M., White, C., Montagnini, A., & Burle, B. (2016). Linking theoretical decision-making mechanisms in the Simon task with electrophysiological data: A model-based neuroscience study in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,28, 1501–1521.PubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology,78, 148–174.PubMed Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology,78, 148–174.PubMed
go back to reference Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2016). Corrigendum to automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions [Cogn. Psychol. 78 (2015) 148–174]. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 150.PubMed Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2016). Corrigendum to automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions [Cogn. Psychol. 78 (2015) 148–174]. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 150.PubMed
go back to reference Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus–response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–143). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus–response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–143). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,27, 731–751.PubMed Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,27, 731–751.PubMed
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: Evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,31, 453–464.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: Evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,31, 453–464.PubMed
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). The Simon effect for vertical S–R relations: Changing the mechanism by randomly varying the S–R mapping rule? Psychological Research,71, 219–233.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). The Simon effect for vertical S–R relations: Changing the mechanism by randomly varying the S–R mapping rule? Psychological Research,71, 219–233.PubMed
go back to reference Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Multidimensional vector model of stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Review,119, 272–303.PubMed Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Multidimensional vector model of stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Review,119, 272–303.PubMed
go back to reference Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (1995). A computational model of the Simon effect. Psychological Research,58, 193–205.PubMed Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (1995). A computational model of the Simon effect. Psychological Research,58, 193–205.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Shared mechanisms underlying the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects
Auteurs
Chunming Luo
Robert W. Proctor
Publicatiedatum
02-04-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01175-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2020

Psychological Research 6/2020 Naar de uitgave