Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

09-04-2018 | Original Paper

Selective Pragmatic Impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Indirect Requests Versus Irony

Auteurs: Gaétane Deliens, Fanny Papastamou, Nicolas Ruytenbeek, Philippine Geelhand, Mikhail Kissine

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 9/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is often described as being characterised by a uniform pragmatic impairment. However, recent evidence suggests that some areas of pragmatic functioning are preserved. This study seeks to determine to which extent context-based derivation of non-linguistically encoded meaning is functional in ASD. We compare the performance of 24 adults with ASD, and matched neuro-typical adults in two act-out pragmatic tasks. The first task examines generation of indirect request interpretations, and the second the comprehension of irony. Intact contextual comprehension of indirect requests contrasts with marked difficulties in understanding irony. These results suggest that preserved pragmatics in ASD is limited to egocentric processing of context, which does not rely on assumptions about the speaker’s mental states.
Voetnoten
1
In order to make the task as non-challenging as possible for ASD participants, our design did not include negative ironic sentences, which would correspond to ‘ironic compliments’. Pexman et al. (2011) found a floor performance on ironic compliments for children with autism (as well as for age- and verbal-matched typically developing children). Such forms of irony are much less canonical (Kreuz 2000), and are very difficult to grasp, be it by adults (Climie and Pexman 2008) or children (Filippova and Astington 2008). This methodological choice should have no impact on potential group differences on ironic items. This is especially so because, using exactly the same design, Deliens et al. (2018) found no evidence that NT participants would rely on the fact that negative statements are always sincere.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2009). Inhibition and the validity of the Stroop task for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), 1112–1121.PubMedCrossRef Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2009). Inhibition and the validity of the Stroop task for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), 1112–1121.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Wasington: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Wasington: American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Katsos, N. (2017). The contribution of grammar, vocabulary and theory of mind in pragmatic language competence in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 996.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Katsos, N. (2017). The contribution of grammar, vocabulary and theory of mind in pragmatic language competence in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 996.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Attwood, A. (2015). The complete guide to asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley. Attwood, A. (2015). The complete guide to asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Out of sight or out of mind? Another look at deception in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(7), 1141–1155.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Out of sight or out of mind? Another look at deception in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(7), 1141–1155.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. Understanding other minds. Perspectives from developmental cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. Understanding other minds. Perspectives from developmental cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(21), 63–75. Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(21), 63–75.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 15–17.CrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 15–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.CrossRef Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.CrossRef
go back to reference Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 11–48.CrossRef Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 11–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39(1), 15–22.PubMedCrossRef Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39(1), 15–22.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bramham, J., Ambery, F., Young, S., Morris, R., Russell, A., Xenitidis, K., et al. (2009). Executive functioning differences between adutls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autistic spectrum disorder in initiation, planning and strategy formation. Autism, 13, 245–264.PubMedCrossRef Bramham, J., Ambery, F., Young, S., Morris, R., Russell, A., Xenitidis, K., et al. (2009). Executive functioning differences between adutls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autistic spectrum disorder in initiation, planning and strategy formation. Autism, 13, 245–264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Breheny, R. (2006). Communication and folk psychology. Mind and Language, 21(1), 74–107.CrossRef Breheny, R. (2006). Communication and folk psychology. Mind and Language, 21(1), 74–107.CrossRef
go back to reference Brock, J., Norbury, C. F., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-mouvements. Cognition, 108, 896–904.PubMedCrossRef Brock, J., Norbury, C. F., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-mouvements. Cognition, 108, 896–904.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bryant, G. A. (2012). Is verbal irony special? Language and Linguistics Compass, 6, 673–685.CrossRef Bryant, G. A. (2012). Is verbal irony special? Language and Linguistics Compass, 6, 673–685.CrossRef
go back to reference Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 164, 231–239.CrossRef Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 164, 231–239.CrossRef
go back to reference Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F. G. E., & Wilson, D. (2011). What’s in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the theory of mind account of autism. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 507–517.PubMedCrossRef Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F. G. E., & Wilson, D. (2011). What’s in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the theory of mind account of autism. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 507–517.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chevallier, C., Wilson, D., Happé, F. G. E., & Noveck, I. (2010). Scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1104–1017.PubMedCrossRef Chevallier, C., Wilson, D., Happé, F. G. E., & Noveck, I. (2010). Scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1104–1017.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clark, H. H., & Lucy, P. (1975). Understanding what is meant from what is said: A study in conversationally conveyed requests. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 56–72.CrossRef Clark, H. H., & Lucy, P. (1975). Understanding what is meant from what is said: A study in conversationally conveyed requests. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 56–72.CrossRef
go back to reference Climie, E. A., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Eye gaze provides a window on children’s understanding of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9(3), 257–285.CrossRef Climie, E. A., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Eye gaze provides a window on children’s understanding of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9(3), 257–285.CrossRef
go back to reference Colich, N. L., Wang, A.-T., Rudie, J. D., Hernandez, L. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2012). Atypical neural processing of ironic and sincere remarks in children and adolescent with autism spectrum disorders. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 70–92.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Colich, N. L., Wang, A.-T., Rudie, J. D., Hernandez, L. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2012). Atypical neural processing of ironic and sincere remarks in children and adolescent with autism spectrum disorders. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 70–92.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Currie, C. E., Elton, R. A., Todd, J., & Platt, S. (1997). Indicators of socioeconomic status for adolescents: The WHO Health Behaviour in School-aged Children Survey. Health Education Research, 12(3), 385–397.PubMedCrossRef Currie, C. E., Elton, R. A., Todd, J., & Platt, S. (1997). Indicators of socioeconomic status for adolescents: The WHO Health Behaviour in School-aged Children Survey. Health Education Research, 12(3), 385–397.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deliens, G., Antoniou, K., Clin, E., Ostashchenko, E., & Kissine, M. (2018). Context, facial expression and prosody in irony processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 99, 35–48.CrossRef Deliens, G., Antoniou, K., Clin, E., Ostashchenko, E., & Kissine, M. (2018). Context, facial expression and prosody in irony processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 99, 35–48.CrossRef
go back to reference de Villiers, J., Fine, J., Ginsberg, G., Vaccarella, L., & Szatmari, P. (2006). Brief report: A scale for rating conversational impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1375–1380.CrossRef de Villiers, J., Fine, J., Ginsberg, G., Vaccarella, L., & Szatmari, P. (2006). Brief report: A scale for rating conversational impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1375–1380.CrossRef
go back to reference Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 26–138.CrossRef Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 26–138.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerrig, R. J., & Goldvarg, Y. (2000). Additive effects in the perception of sarcasm: Situational disparity and echoic mention. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(4), 197–208.CrossRef Gerrig, R. J., & Goldvarg, Y. (2000). Additive effects in the perception of sarcasm: Situational disparity and echoic mention. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(4), 197–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibbs, R. W. (2002). A new look at literal meaning in understanding what is said and implicated. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(4), 457–486.CrossRef Gibbs, R. W. (2002). A new look at literal meaning in understanding what is said and implicated. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(4), 457–486.CrossRef
go back to reference Glucksberg, S., Gildea, P., & Bookin, H. B. (1982). On understanding nonliteral speech: Can people ignore metaphors? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(1), 85–98.CrossRef Glucksberg, S., Gildea, P., & Bookin, H. B. (1982). On understanding nonliteral speech: Can people ignore metaphors? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(1), 85–98.CrossRef
go back to reference Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press. Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism. A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.PubMedCrossRef Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism. A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.PubMedCrossRef Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The awkward moments test: A naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 395–406.CrossRef Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The awkward moments test: A naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 395–406.CrossRef
go back to reference Hermann, I., Haser, V., Van Elst, L., Ebert, D., Müller-Feldmeth, D., Riedel, A., et al. (2013). Automatic metaphor processing in adults with Asperger syndrome: A metaphor interference effect task. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, S177–S187.PubMedCrossRef Hermann, I., Haser, V., Van Elst, L., Ebert, D., Müller-Feldmeth, D., Riedel, A., et al. (2013). Automatic metaphor processing in adults with Asperger syndrome: A metaphor interference effect task. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, S177–S187.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jary, M., & Kissine, M. (2014). Imperatives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Jary, M., & Kissine, M. (2014). Imperatives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Joliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The strange story test: A replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 395–406.CrossRef Joliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The strange story test: A replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 395–406.CrossRef
go back to reference Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new ’advanced’ test of Theory of Mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(4), 517–528.CrossRef Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new ’advanced’ test of Theory of Mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(4), 517–528.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, S. H., Paul, R., Tager-Flusberg, H., Lord, C., Volkmar, F. R., Paul, R., et al. (2014). Language and communication in autism language and communication in autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, S. J. Rogers, & K. A. Pelphrey (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (4th ed., pp. 230–262). Hoboken: Wiley. Kim, S. H., Paul, R., Tager-Flusberg, H., Lord, C., Volkmar, F. R., Paul, R., et al. (2014). Language and communication in autism language and communication in autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, S. J. Rogers, & K. A. Pelphrey (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (4th ed., pp. 230–262). Hoboken: Wiley.
go back to reference Kissine, M. (2012). Pragmatics, cognitive flexibility and autism spectrum disorders. Mind and Language, 27, 1–28.CrossRef Kissine, M. (2012). Pragmatics, cognitive flexibility and autism spectrum disorders. Mind and Language, 27, 1–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Kissine, M. (2013). From utterances to speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Kissine, M. (2013). From utterances to speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Kissine, M., Cano-Chervel, J., Carlier, S., De Brabanter, P., Ducenne, L., Pairon, M.-C., et al. (2015). Children with autism understand indirect speech acts: Evidence from a semi-structured act-out task. PLoS ONE, 10, e0142191.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kissine, M., Cano-Chervel, J., Carlier, S., De Brabanter, P., Ducenne, L., Pairon, M.-C., et al. (2015). Children with autism understand indirect speech acts: Evidence from a semi-structured act-out task. PLoS ONE, 10, e0142191.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kissine, M., De Brabanter, P., & Leybaert, J. (2012). The interpretation of requests in children with autism: The effect of the sentence-type. Autism, 16, 523–532.PubMedCrossRef Kissine, M., De Brabanter, P., & Leybaert, J. (2012). The interpretation of requests in children with autism: The effect of the sentence-type. Autism, 16, 523–532.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kowatch, K., Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2013). Irony comprehension in action: A new test of processing verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 50, 301–315.CrossRef Kowatch, K., Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2013). Irony comprehension in action: A new test of processing verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 50, 301–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Kreuz, R. J. (2000). The production and processing of verbal irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 99–107.CrossRef Kreuz, R. J. (2000). The production and processing of verbal irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 99–107.CrossRef
go back to reference Kreuz, R. J., & Link, K. E. (2002). Asymmetries in the use of verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(2), 127–143.CrossRef Kreuz, R. J., & Link, K. E. (2002). Asymmetries in the use of verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(2), 127–143.CrossRef
go back to reference Lai, C. L. E., Lau, Z., Lui, S. S. Y., Lok, E., Tam, V., Chan, Q., et al. (2017). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(5), 911–939.PubMedCrossRef Lai, C. L. E., Lau, Z., Lui, S. S. Y., Lok, E., Tam, V., Chan, Q., et al. (2017). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(5), 911–939.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Landry, O., & Al-Taie, S. (2016). A meta-analysis of the wisconsin card sort task in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 1220–1235.PubMedCrossRef Landry, O., & Al-Taie, S. (2016). A meta-analysis of the wisconsin card sort task in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 1220–1235.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Lenth, R. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software, 69(1), 1–33.CrossRef Lenth, R. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software, 69(1), 1–33.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS) (2nd ed.). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS) (2nd ed.). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Loukusa, S., Leinonen, E., Kuusikko, S., Jussila, K., Mattila, M.-L., Ryder, N., et al. (2006). Use of context in pragmatic language comprehension by children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1049–1059.CrossRef Loukusa, S., Leinonen, E., Kuusikko, S., Jussila, K., Mattila, M.-L., Ryder, N., et al. (2006). Use of context in pragmatic language comprehension by children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1049–1059.CrossRef
go back to reference MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2005). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20(1), 13–32.CrossRef MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2005). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20(1), 13–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language, 85, 451–466.PubMedCrossRef Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language, 85, 451–466.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 311–328.PubMedCrossRef Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 311–328.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mary, A., Slama, H., Mousty, P., Massat, I., Capiau, T., Drabs, V., et al. (2016). Executive and attentional contributions to theory of mind deficit in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 22(3), 345–365.PubMedCrossRef Mary, A., Slama, H., Mousty, P., Massat, I., Capiau, T., Drabs, V., et al. (2016). Executive and attentional contributions to theory of mind deficit in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 22(3), 345–365.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Monetta, L., & Champagne, M. (2004). Processuscognitifs sous jacents déterminant les troubles de la communication verbale chez les cérébrolésés droits. Réeducation orthophonique, 42(219), 27–41. Monetta, L., & Champagne, M. (2004). Processuscognitifs sous jacents déterminant les troubles de la communication verbale chez les cérébrolésés droits. Réeducation orthophonique, 42(219), 27–41.
go back to reference Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 141–171.CrossRef Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 141–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 383–399.CrossRef Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 383–399.CrossRef
go back to reference Ozonoff, S. (1997). Components of executive function in autism and other disorders. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 179–211). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Ozonoff, S. (1997). Components of executive function in autism and other disorders. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 179–211). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1996). An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain and Language, 52, 411–434.PubMedCrossRef Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1996). An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain and Language, 52, 411–434.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1081–1105.CrossRef Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1081–1105.CrossRef
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., South, M., & Provencal, S. (2005). Executive functions. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed., pp. 606–627). Hoboken: Wiley. Ozonoff, S., South, M., & Provencal, S. (2005). Executive functions. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed., pp. 606–627). Hoboken: Wiley.
go back to reference Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). Comprehension of indirect requests in adults with autistic disorders and mental retardation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28(4), 475–479.PubMedCrossRef Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). Comprehension of indirect requests in adults with autistic disorders and mental retardation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28(4), 475–479.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2008). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 115–125.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2008). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 115–125.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Perkins, M. R. (2007). Pragmatic impairment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Perkins, M. R. (2007). Pragmatic impairment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Perner, J., & Winner, E. (1985). “John thinks, that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order belief by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Child Experimental Psychology, 39, 437–471.CrossRef Perner, J., & Winner, E. (1985). “John thinks, that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order belief by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Child Experimental Psychology, 39, 437–471.CrossRef
go back to reference Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., Ranick, J., & St. Clair, M. (2013). Teaching children with autism to detect and respond to sarcasm. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 71, 193–198.CrossRef Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., Ranick, J., & St. Clair, M. (2013). Teaching children with autism to detect and respond to sarcasm. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 71, 193–198.CrossRef
go back to reference Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1097–1112.PubMedCrossRef Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1097–1112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J. P, & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618.PubMedCrossRef Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J. P, & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
go back to reference Recanati, F. (2004). Literal meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Recanati, F. (2004). Literal meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Russell, J., & Hughes, C. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477–492.PubMedCrossRef Russell, J., & Hughes, C. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477–492.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ruytenbeek, N., Ostashchenko, E., & Kissine, M. (2017). Indirect request processing, sentence-types and illocutionary forces. Journal of Pragmatics, 119, 46–62.CrossRef Ruytenbeek, N., Ostashchenko, E., & Kissine, M. (2017). Indirect request processing, sentence-types and illocutionary forces. Journal of Pragmatics, 119, 46–62.CrossRef
go back to reference Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (Vol. 3, pp. 59–82). New York: Academic Press. Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (Vol. 3, pp. 59–82). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Senju, A., Southgate, V., Miura, Y., Matsui, T., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., et al. (2010). Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 353–360.PubMedCrossRef Senju, A., Southgate, V., Miura, Y., Matsui, T., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., et al. (2010). Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 353–360.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209.PubMedCrossRef Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shintel, H., & Keysar, B. (2009). Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 260–273.PubMedCrossRef Shintel, H., & Keysar, B. (2009). Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 260–273.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language, 17, 3–23.CrossRef Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language, 17, 3–23.CrossRef
go back to reference Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(1), 55–71.PubMedCrossRef Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(1), 55–71.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129(4), 932–943.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129(4), 932–943.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Wearing, C. (2010). Autism, metaphor and Revelance theory. Mind and Language, 25(2), 196–216.CrossRef Wearing, C. (2010). Autism, metaphor and Revelance theory. Mind and Language, 25(2), 196–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV) (4th ed., Vol. 22). San Antonio: NCS Pearson. Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV) (4th ed., Vol. 22). San Antonio: NCS Pearson.
go back to reference Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283–307.PubMedCrossRef Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283–307.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zelazo, P. D., Jacques, S., Burack, J. A., & Frye, D. (2002). The relation between theory of mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development, 11, 171–195.CrossRef Zelazo, P. D., Jacques, S., Burack, J. A., & Frye, D. (2002). The relation between theory of mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development, 11, 171–195.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Selective Pragmatic Impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Indirect Requests Versus Irony
Auteurs
Gaétane Deliens
Fanny Papastamou
Nicolas Ruytenbeek
Philippine Geelhand
Mikhail Kissine
Publicatiedatum
09-04-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 9/2018
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3561-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 9/2018

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 9/2018 Naar de uitgave