Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 3/2011

01-06-2011 | Original Article

Selective Attention and Health Anxiety: Ill-Health Stimuli are Distracting for Everyone

Auteurs: Cassandra Shields, Karen Murphy

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Uitgave 3/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Psychological theories of anxiety are increasingly referring to information processing paradigms in order to understand the cognitive processes which underlie these disorders. Numerous studies of anxiety have demonstrated an attentional bias towards anxiety relevant stimuli. In addition, there is consistent evidence that it is more difficult to process absent than present information. Prior research has suggested that both these processing biases contribute to the maintenance of health anxiety. The present study investigated differences in attentional processing between participants high and low in health anxiety, using two visual search tasks. The visual search tasks used either letters (domain free stimuli), or words (anxiety-related stimuli). Both low and high health anxious participants demonstrated poorer performance for target absent than target present trials. In addition, all participants showed an attentional bias towards ill-health words over good-health, negative, positive, or neutral words. These results suggest that concern about health is relevant to all people regardless of level of health anxiety.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
A Repeated Measures ANOVA revealed the same pattern of results for both the letter and the word visual search tasks, when the WI or the IAS were used individually, and when the measures were combined to categorize individuals into high and low health anxious groups.
 
2
Power calculations were conducted using the Effect Size Generator element of ClinTools Version 4.1 (Devilly 2007). As the methodology for assessing attentional biases in health anxiety within this study is relatively unique, details entered into ClinTools were based upon three research articles investigating information processing and anxiety. Specifically, Rassin et al. (2008) who reported a medium effect (r = .29) of health anxiety during a visual search task with non-health specific stimuli. When converted to Cohen’s d using ClinTools (Devilly, 2007) r = .29 is equivalent to d = .61. Owens et al. (2004) Stroop paradigm utilising health specific words among high and low health anxious individuals, revealing a large effect (d = .97); and Rinck et al. (2003) who observed a medium to large effect (d = .73) between generalised anxiety patients and controls during a visual search task involving anxiety-related words. Results suggested an adequate sample size of approximately 28 participants per group to achieve power of .8 in a two-tailed analysis with an expected medium to large effect size. Therefore, the current group sample sizes were deemed sufficient to produce suitable power for the analyses.
 
3
In total 6, 12, or 18 items were presented in the word and letter task, for target present and absent trials. This allowed the same number of items to be displayed within the present and absent trials, preventing participants from using counting to respond to target, present versus absent (e.g., 6 items = absent, 7 items = present).
 
4
Regression coefficients were not calculated for the error scores (ms) due to insufficient data (i.e., few errors within some conditions).
 
5
The authors would like to acknowledge the input from one of the reviewers suggesting this further analysis.
 
6
Originally, ANCOVAs were applied to the data sets, with scores from the STAI-S, STAI-T, and BDI as covariates. However, the assumption of linear relationships between the covariates at each level of the independent variables were violated for error data, in the absent condition, for both letter and word tasks. In order to run the same analyses for both the RT and error data to check for speed-accuracy trade-offs, the data were investigated using mixed factorial ANOVA. Initial analyses revealed, results were not influenced by speed-accuracy trade-offs and the pattern of results produced using both errors and mean RT per condition including display size, was similar to that reported using the regression based approach.
 
7
One methodological suggestion which may have been considered to ensure priming effects in the visual search task was to block trials by distractor type (we thank a reviewer for this insightful suggestion). However if this procedural adjustment had been made it is likely that the distractor words may have lost their valence and meaning value, in turn minimising any priming effects. Researchers have previously demonstrated that repeated presentation of a word type results in habituation or a loss of valence to that word type (e.g., McNally et al. 1990).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Text Rev., 4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Text Rev., 4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Anderson, J. R., & Schunn, C. D. (2000). Implications of the ACT-R learning theory: No magic bullets. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 1–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Anderson, J. R., & Schunn, C. D. (2000). Implications of the ACT-R learning theory: No magic bullets. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 1–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Baños, R. M., Quero, S., & Botella, C. (2008). Detection and distraction effects for threatening information in social phobia and change after treatment. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 55–63. doi:10.1002/da.20269.PubMedCrossRef Baños, R. M., Quero, S., & Botella, C. (2008). Detection and distraction effects for threatening information in social phobia and change after treatment. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 55–63. doi:10.​1002/​da.​20269.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1.PubMedCrossRef Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. doi:10.​1037/​0033-2909.​133.​1.​1.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 737–753. doi:10.1080/026999398379411.CrossRef Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 737–753. doi:10.​1080/​026999398379411.CrossRef
go back to reference Calvo, M. G., Ramos, P., & Estevez, A. (1992). Test anxiety and comprehension efficiency: The role of prior knowledge and working memory deficits. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 5, 125–138. doi:10.1080/10615809208250492.CrossRef Calvo, M. G., Ramos, P., & Estevez, A. (1992). Test anxiety and comprehension efficiency: The role of prior knowledge and working memory deficits. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 5, 125–138. doi:10.​1080/​1061580920825049​2.CrossRef
go back to reference Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef
go back to reference Fazio, R. H., Sherman, S. J., & Herr, P. M. (1982). The feature-positive effect in the self-perception process: Does not doing matter as much as doing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 404–411. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.3.404.CrossRef Fazio, R. H., Sherman, S. J., & Herr, P. M. (1982). The feature-positive effect in the self-perception process: Does not doing matter as much as doing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 404–411. doi:10.​1037/​0022-3514.​42.​3.​404.CrossRef
go back to reference Hair, J. F., Jr, Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall. Hair, J. F., Jr, Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
go back to reference Jellis, S., et al. (2001). Encarta thesaurus. Sydney: Pan Macmillan. Jellis, S., et al. (2001). Encarta thesaurus. Sydney: Pan Macmillan.
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Triesman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. A. Davis (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 28–61). San Diego: Academic Press. Kahneman, D., & Triesman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. A. Davis (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 28–61). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Karademas, E. C., Christopoulou, S., Dimostheni, A., & Pavlu, F. (2008). Health anxiety and cognitive interference: Evidence from the application of a modified stroop task in two studies. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1138–1150. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.007.CrossRef Karademas, E. C., Christopoulou, S., Dimostheni, A., & Pavlu, F. (2008). Health anxiety and cognitive interference: Evidence from the application of a modified stroop task in two studies. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1138–1150. doi:10.​1016/​j.​paid.​2007.​11.​007.CrossRef
go back to reference Kellner, R., Abbott, P., Winslow, W. W., & Pathak, D. (1987). Fears, beliefs, and attitudes in DSM-III hypochondriasis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 20–25.PubMedCrossRef Kellner, R., Abbott, P., Winslow, W. W., & Pathak, D. (1987). Fears, beliefs, and attitudes in DSM-III hypochondriasis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 20–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lees, A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentional biases for pictorial and linguistic health-threat cues. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 453–462. doi:10.1080/02699930441000184.CrossRef Lees, A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentional biases for pictorial and linguistic health-threat cues. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 453–462. doi:10.​1080/​0269993044100018​4.CrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Lundqvist, D. (1999). Unconscious emotion: Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysical data, and neuropsychological mechanisms. In R. L. A. L. Nadel (Ed.), The cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 296–327). New York: Oxford University Press. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Lundqvist, D. (1999). Unconscious emotion: Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysical data, and neuropsychological mechanisms. In R. L. A. L. Nadel (Ed.), The cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 296–327). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., Kellerman, J., & Roth, W. T. (2003). Selective attention in anxiety: Distraction and enhancement in visual search. Depression and Anxiety, 18, 18–28. doi:10.1002/da.10105.PubMedCrossRef Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., Kellerman, J., & Roth, W. T. (2003). Selective attention in anxiety: Distraction and enhancement in visual search. Depression and Anxiety, 18, 18–28. doi:10.​1002/​da.​10105.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Criteria for scale selection and evaluation. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Criteria for scale selection and evaluation. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Rooney, K., et al. (2001). Encarta concise English dictionary. Sydney: Pan Macmillan. Rooney, K., et al. (2001). Encarta concise English dictionary. Sydney: Pan Macmillan.
go back to reference Speckens, A. E. M., Spinhoven, P., Sloekers, P. P. A., Bolk, J. H., & van Hemert, A. M. (1996a). A validation study of the Whitely Index, the Illness Attitude Scales, and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale in general medical and general practice patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40, 95–104. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(95)00561-7.PubMedCrossRef Speckens, A. E. M., Spinhoven, P., Sloekers, P. P. A., Bolk, J. H., & van Hemert, A. M. (1996a). A validation study of the Whitely Index, the Illness Attitude Scales, and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale in general medical and general practice patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40, 95–104. doi:10.​1016/​0022-3999(95)00561-7.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Speckens, A. E. M., Van Hemert, A. M., Spinhoven, P., & Bolk, J. H. (1996b). The diagnostic and prognostic significance of the Whiteley Index, the Illness Attitudes Scales and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale. Psychological Medicine, 26, 1085–1090. doi:10.1017/S0033291700035418.PubMedCrossRef Speckens, A. E. M., Van Hemert, A. M., Spinhoven, P., & Bolk, J. H. (1996b). The diagnostic and prognostic significance of the Whiteley Index, the Illness Attitudes Scales and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale. Psychological Medicine, 26, 1085–1090. doi:10.​1017/​S003329170003541​8.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spielberger, C. D., Gorsch, R. L., & Lushene, R. (1970). State-trait anxiety inventory. Palto Alto: Consulting Psychology Press. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsch, R. L., & Lushene, R. (1970). State-trait anxiety inventory. Palto Alto: Consulting Psychology Press.
go back to reference Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 55, 1007–1026. doi:10.1080/02724980143000659.CrossRef Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 55, 1007–1026. doi:10.​1080/​0272498014300065​9.CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Metagegevens
Titel
Selective Attention and Health Anxiety: Ill-Health Stimuli are Distracting for Everyone
Auteurs
Cassandra Shields
Karen Murphy
Publicatiedatum
01-06-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Uitgave 3/2011
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9351-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2011

Cognitive Therapy and Research 3/2011 Naar de uitgave