Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-09-2010 | Original Paper

Scalar Inferences in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Auteurs: Coralie Chevallier, Deirdre Wilson, Francesca Happé, Ira Noveck

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 9/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

On being told “John or Mary will come”, one might infer that not both of them will come. Yet the semantics of “or” is compatible with a situation where both John and Mary come. Inferences of this type, which enrich the semantics of “or” from an ‘inclusive’ to an ‘exclusive’ interpretation, have been extensively studied in linguistic pragmatics. However, the phenomenon has not been much explored in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs), where pragmatic deficits are commonly reported. Here, we present an experiment investigating these inferences. We predicted that, as a result of the reported pragmatic deficits, participants with ASD would produce fewer inferential enrichments of “or” than matched controls. However, contrary to expectations, but in line with recent findings by Pijnacker et al. (Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618, 2009), performances did not differ across groups. This unexpected finding is discussed in light of the literature on pragmatic abilities in autism.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Alternative, more semantic, treatments are suggested in recent work by Chiercha and his colleagues (Chierchia, 2004, to appear).
 
2
These studies have also shown that children are more likely to draw implicatures when task demands are reduced, which suggests that limitations in cognitive resources—rather than diminished pragmatic competence—may play an important role in children’s pragmatic difficulties (Siegal and Surian 2004).
 
3
This rate of pragmatic enrichment (leading to an exclusive interpretation) is among the highest reported in laboratory tasks dealing with scalar terms.
 
4
The correlation between VIQ and scalar inference rate is not reported for the ASD group as a whole. This correlation is not found in the AS group or in the TD group, both ps > .1.
 
5
“A and B” is only true if both A and B are true, whereas “A or B” is true if A alone is true, or if B alone is true, or if both A and B are true when an inclusive reading of the disjunction is chosen.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Asperger, H. (1944). Die „Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 117(1), 76–136. Asperger, H. (1944). Die „Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 117(1), 76–136.
go back to reference Baltaxe, C. A. M., & Guthrie, D. (1987). The use of primary sentence stress by normal, aphasic, and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(2), 255–271.CrossRefPubMed Baltaxe, C. A. M., & Guthrie, D. (1987). The use of primary sentence stress by normal, aphasic, and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(2), 255–271.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A 15-year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3–21). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A 15-year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3–21). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge: The MIT Press. Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Bott, L., & Noveck, I. (2004). Some utterances are underinformative: The onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(3), 437–457.CrossRef Bott, L., & Noveck, I. (2004). Some utterances are underinformative: The onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(3), 437–457.CrossRef
go back to reference Braine, M., & Rumain, B. (1981). Development of comprehension of `Or’: Evidence for a sequence of competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31(1), 46–70.CrossRef Braine, M., & Rumain, B. (1981). Development of comprehension of `Or’: Evidence for a sequence of competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31(1), 46–70.CrossRef
go back to reference Braun, B., & Tagliapietra, L. (in press). The role of contrastive intonation contours in the retrieval of contextual alternatives. Language and Cognitive Processes. Braun, B., & Tagliapietra, L. (in press). The role of contrastive intonation contours in the retrieval of contextual alternatives. Language and Cognitive Processes.
go back to reference Breheny, R., Katsos, N., & Williams, J. (2006). Are generalised scalar implicatures generated by default? An on-line investigation into the role of context in generating pragmatic inferences. Cognition, 100(3), 434–463.CrossRefPubMed Breheny, R., Katsos, N., & Williams, J. (2006). Are generalised scalar implicatures generated by default? An on-line investigation into the role of context in generating pragmatic inferences. Cognition, 100(3), 434–463.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Burton-Roberts, N. (2007). Pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave-Macmillan. Burton-Roberts, N. (2007). Pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave-Macmillan.
go back to reference Carston, R. (1998). Informativeness, relevance and scalar implicature. Relevance Theory: Applications and Implications, 1, 79–236. Carston, R. (1998). Informativeness, relevance and scalar implicature. Relevance Theory: Applications and Implications, 1, 79–236.
go back to reference Carston, R. (2002). Thoughts and utterances: The pragmatics of explicit communication. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRef Carston, R. (2002). Thoughts and utterances: The pragmatics of explicit communication. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRef
go back to reference Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F., & Wilson, D. (2009). From acoustics to grammar: Perceiving and interpreting grammatical prosody in adolescents with Asperger Syndrome. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 502–516.CrossRef Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F., & Wilson, D. (2009). From acoustics to grammar: Perceiving and interpreting grammatical prosody in adolescents with Asperger Syndrome. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 502–516.CrossRef
go back to reference Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Nazir, T., Bott, L., Lanzetti, V., & Sperber, D. (2008). Making disjunctions exclusive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(11), 1741–1760.CrossRefPubMed Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Nazir, T., Bott, L., Lanzetti, V., & Sperber, D. (2008). Making disjunctions exclusive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(11), 1741–1760.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chierchia, G. (2004). Scalar implicatures, polarity phenomena and the syntax/pragmatics interface. In A. Belletti (Ed.), Structures and beyond (pp. 39–103). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Chierchia, G. (2004). Scalar implicatures, polarity phenomena and the syntax/pragmatics interface. In A. Belletti (Ed.), Structures and beyond (pp. 39–103). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Chierchia, G., Fox, D., & Spector, B. (to appear). The grammatical view of scalar implicatures and the relationship between semantics and pragmatics The handbook of semantics. Chierchia, G., Fox, D., & Spector, B. (to appear). The grammatical view of scalar implicatures and the relationship between semantics and pragmatics The handbook of semantics.
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference De Neys, W., & Schaeken, W. (2007). When people are more logical under cognitive load: Dual task impact on scalar implicature. Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 128–133.PubMed De Neys, W., & Schaeken, W. (2007). When people are more logical under cognitive load: Dual task impact on scalar implicature. Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 128–133.PubMed
go back to reference Dennis, M., Lazenby, A. L., & Lockyer, L. (2001). inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 47–54.CrossRefPubMed Dennis, M., Lazenby, A. L., & Lockyer, L. (2001). inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 47–54.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Diehl, J., Watson, D., Bennetto, L., McDonough, J., & Gunlogson, C. (2009). An acoustic analysis of prosody in high-functioning autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(03), 385–404.CrossRef Diehl, J., Watson, D., Bennetto, L., McDonough, J., & Gunlogson, C. (2009). An acoustic analysis of prosody in high-functioning autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(03), 385–404.CrossRef
go back to reference Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Whetton, K. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: National Foundation for Education Research—Nelson. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Whetton, K. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: National Foundation for Education Research—Nelson.
go back to reference Findlay, J. M. (1978). Estimates on probability functions: A more virulent PEST. Perception and Psychophysics, 23(2), 181–185. Findlay, J. M. (1978). Estimates on probability functions: A more virulent PEST. Perception and Psychophysics, 23(2), 181–185.
go back to reference Fisher, N., Happé, F., & Dunn, J. (2005). The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(4), 409–419.CrossRefPubMed Fisher, N., Happé, F., & Dunn, J. (2005). The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(4), 409–419.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frith, U. (1998). What autism teaches us about communication. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 23(2), 51–58.CrossRef Frith, U. (1998). What autism teaches us about communication. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 23(2), 51–58.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 346(1315), 97–104.CrossRef Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 346(1315), 97–104.CrossRef
go back to reference Geurts, B. (2009). Scalar implicature and local pragmatics. Mind & Language, 24, 51–79.CrossRef Geurts, B. (2009). Scalar implicature and local pragmatics. Mind & Language, 24, 51–79.CrossRef
go back to reference Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J., & Rutherford, M. (2007). The ‘reading the mind in the voice’ test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1096–1106.CrossRefPubMed Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J., & Rutherford, M. (2007). The ‘reading the mind in the voice’ test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1096–1106.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Green, S. B., & Salkind, N. J. (2008). Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh: Analysing and understanding data (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall. Green, S. B., & Salkind, N. J. (2008). Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh: Analysing and understanding data (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
go back to reference Happé, F. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.CrossRefPubMed Happé, F. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.CrossRefPubMed Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.CrossRefPubMed Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horn, L. (1972). On the semantic properties of logical operators in English. Los Angeles: University of California Los Angeles. Horn, L. (1972). On the semantic properties of logical operators in English. Los Angeles: University of California Los Angeles.
go back to reference Horn, L. (2004). Implicature. In L. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp. 4–28). Oxford: Blackwell. Horn, L. (2004). Implicature. In L. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp. 4–28). Oxford: Blackwell.
go back to reference Horn, L., & Ward, G. (2004). The handbook of pragmatics. Oxford: Blackwell. Horn, L., & Ward, G. (2004). The handbook of pragmatics. Oxford: Blackwell.
go back to reference House, J. (2006). Constructing a context with intonation. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1542–1558.CrossRef House, J. (2006). Constructing a context with intonation. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1542–1558.CrossRef
go back to reference Howell, D. C. (1997). Statistical methods for psychology (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Howell, D. C. (1997). Statistical methods for psychology (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
go back to reference Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome: Is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2), 149–185.CrossRefPubMed Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome: Is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2), 149–185.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine, 30(5), 1169.CrossRefPubMed Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine, 30(5), 1169.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
go back to reference Kelley, E., Paul, J. J., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(6), 807–828.CrossRefPubMed Kelley, E., Paul, J. J., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(6), 807–828.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klin, A., Schultz, R., & Cohen, D. (2000). Theory of mind in action: Developmental perspectives on social neuroscience. Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 357–388). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Klin, A., Schultz, R., & Cohen, D. (2000). Theory of mind in action: Developmental perspectives on social neuroscience. Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 357–388). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature: Mit Pr. Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature: Mit Pr.
go back to reference Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. Journal of Child Langauge, 34(1), 1–20.CrossRef Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. Journal of Child Langauge, 34(1), 1–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.CrossRefPubMed Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 311–328.CrossRefPubMed Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 311–328.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McCann, J., & Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(4), 325–350.CrossRef McCann, J., & Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(4), 325–350.CrossRef
go back to reference Mitchell, S., Jessica Brian, P., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Isabel Smith, P., et al. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S69.CrossRefPubMed Mitchell, S., Jessica Brian, P., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Isabel Smith, P., et al. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S69.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Noveck, I. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition, 78(2), 165–188.CrossRefPubMed Noveck, I. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition, 78(2), 165–188.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Noveck, I., & Posada, A. (2003). Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study. Brain and Language, 85(2), 203–210.CrossRefPubMed Noveck, I., & Posada, A. (2003). Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study. Brain and Language, 85(2), 203–210.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Noveck, I., & Reboul, A. (2008). Experimental pragmatics: A Gricean turn in the study of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 425–431.CrossRefPubMed Noveck, I., & Reboul, A. (2008). Experimental pragmatics: A Gricean turn in the study of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 425–431.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Noveck, I., & Sperber, D. (2004). Experimental pragmatics. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRef Noveck, I., & Sperber, D. (2004). Experimental pragmatics. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRef
go back to reference Noveck, I., & Sperber, D. (2007). The why and how of experimental pragmatics: The case of ‘scalar inferences’. In N. Burton-Roberts (Ed.), Pragmatics (pp. 184–212). Basingstoke: Palgrave. Noveck, I., & Sperber, D. (2007). The why and how of experimental pragmatics: The case of ‘scalar inferences’. In N. Burton-Roberts (Ed.), Pragmatics (pp. 184–212). Basingstoke: Palgrave.
go back to reference Papafragou, A., & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognition, 86(3), 253–282.CrossRefPubMed Papafragou, A., & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognition, 86(3), 253–282.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Paris, S. G. (1973). Comprehension of language connectives and propositional logical relationships. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16(2), 278–291.CrossRef Paris, S. G. (1973). Comprehension of language connectives and propositional logical relationships. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16(2), 278–291.CrossRef
go back to reference Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 205–220.CrossRefPubMed Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 205–220.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1015.CrossRef Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1015.CrossRef
go back to reference Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 271–311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 271–311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J.-P., & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618.CrossRefPubMed Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J.-P., & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pouscoulous, N., Noveck, I., Politzer, G., & Bastide, A. (2007). Processing costs and their impact on the development of scalar implicature. Language Acquisition, 14(4), 347–375.CrossRef Pouscoulous, N., Noveck, I., Politzer, G., & Bastide, A. (2007). Processing costs and their impact on the development of scalar implicature. Language Acquisition, 14(4), 347–375.CrossRef
go back to reference Recanati, F. (2003). Embedded implicatures. Philosophical Perspectives, 17, 299–332.CrossRef Recanati, F. (2003). Embedded implicatures. Philosophical Perspectives, 17, 299–332.CrossRef
go back to reference Rutherford, M., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger Syndrome and High Functioning Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 189–194.CrossRefPubMed Rutherford, M., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger Syndrome and High Functioning Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 189–194.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with High-Functioning Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 44(5), 1097–1115.CrossRef Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with High-Functioning Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 44(5), 1097–1115.CrossRef
go back to reference Siegal, M., & Surian, L. (2004). Conceptual development and conversational understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 534–538.CrossRefPubMed Siegal, M., & Surian, L. (2004). Conceptual development and conversational understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 534–538.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smith, N., Hermelin, B., & Tsimpli, I. (2003). Dissociation of social affect and theory of mind in a case of Asperger Syndrome. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 15, 303–322. Smith, N., Hermelin, B., & Tsimpli, I. (2003). Dissociation of social affect and theory of mind in a case of Asperger Syndrome. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 15, 303–322.
go back to reference Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.CrossRef Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.CrossRef
go back to reference Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G. (accepted). 17-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ communication. Developmental Science. Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G. (accepted). 17-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ communication. Developmental Science.
go back to reference Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986/1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986/1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.
go back to reference Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language, 17(1), 3–23.CrossRef Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language, 17(1), 3–23.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, R. (1979). Developmental patterns in the encoding and combination of logical connectives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28(3), 469–498.CrossRefPubMed Sternberg, R. (1979). Developmental patterns in the encoding and combination of logical connectives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28(3), 469–498.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean Maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(1), 55–71.CrossRefPubMed Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean Maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(1), 55–71.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2008). Language and communication in autism and Asperger Syndrome. In B. Stemmer & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience of language. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2008). Language and communication in autism and Asperger Syndrome. In B. Stemmer & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience of language. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Sutcliffe, P., & Bishop, D. (2005). Psychophysical design influences frequency discrimination performance in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91(3), 249–270.CrossRefPubMed Sutcliffe, P., & Bishop, D. (2005). Psychophysical design influences frequency discrimination performance in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91(3), 249–270.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 1, 335–364. Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 1, 335–364.
go back to reference Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.CrossRefPubMed Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wang, A., Lee, S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129(4), 932.CrossRefPubMed Wang, A., Lee, S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129(4), 932.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wang, A., Lee, S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2007). Reading affect in the face and voice: Neural correlates of interpreting communicative intent in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(6), 698.CrossRefPubMed Wang, A., Lee, S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2007). Reading affect in the face and voice: Neural correlates of interpreting communicative intent in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(6), 698.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49(3), 367.CrossRefPubMed Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49(3), 367.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference WHO. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organisation. WHO. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organisation.
go back to reference Williams, D. (2004a). Not just anything: A collection of thoughts on paper. London: Jessica Kingsley. Williams, D. (2004a). Not just anything: A collection of thoughts on paper. London: Jessica Kingsley.
go back to reference Williams, K., Tuck, M., Helmer, M., Bartak, L., Mellis, C., & Peat, J. (2008). Diagnostic labelling of Autism Spectrum Disorders in NSW. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 44(3), 108–113.CrossRefPubMed Williams, K., Tuck, M., Helmer, M., Bartak, L., Mellis, C., & Peat, J. (2008). Diagnostic labelling of Autism Spectrum Disorders in NSW. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 44(3), 108–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wilson, D. (in press). Pragmatic processes and metarepresentational abilities: The case of verbal irony. In T. Matsui (Ed.), Pragmatics and theory of mind. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Wilson, D. (in press). Pragmatic processes and metarepresentational abilities: The case of verbal irony. In T. Matsui (Ed.), Pragmatics and theory of mind. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
go back to reference Wilson, D., & Wharton, T. (2006). Relevance and prosody. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1559–1579.CrossRef Wilson, D., & Wharton, T. (2006). Relevance and prosody. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(10), 1559–1579.CrossRef
go back to reference Witwer, A., & Lecavalier, L. (2008). Examining the validity of autism spectrum disorder subtypes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(9), 1611–1624.CrossRefPubMed Witwer, A., & Lecavalier, L. (2008). Examining the validity of autism spectrum disorder subtypes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(9), 1611–1624.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Scalar Inferences in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Auteurs
Coralie Chevallier
Deirdre Wilson
Francesca Happé
Ira Noveck
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 9/2010
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0960-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 9/2010

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 9/2010 Naar de uitgave